Taking a Bite Out of Fiction-Media Effects and Social Fears. A Case Study on 'Jaws' - Sharklab-Malta

Page created by Maurice Christensen
 
CONTINUE READING
University of Malta
              Faculty of Education, Psychology Department

                             Alexia Curmi

 Taking a Bite Out of Fiction-Media Effects and

                           Social Fears.

                        A Case Study on

                                ‘Jaws’

Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of Education in Part Fulfilment of the
Requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Psychology (Honours) at the
                            University of Malta

                                April 2005
                                  Malta

                                     1
“If a White Shark were music, this would be a solo cello; molto piano.”

             RICHARD ELLIS, Great White Shark (1991).

                                    2
ABSTRACT

       “Sublime terror”- says W.H. Rockett -“rests in the unseen”.

                                                          W.H. Rockett, 1982

       Say the word “shark" and the first image most people conjure up is a Jaws-inspired

white shark devouring unsuspecting bathers while well-meaning authorities and scientists

helplessly stand by.

       It seems to the author that fear of the unknown is a large part of the human life and

thus, when it came to asking about sharks, most people were scared of what they did not know

probably because they did not have enough information to judge the animal. The study was

created by the author and comprised of a three page questionnaire with nineteen questions.

There were 225 participants in this study. Findings show that participants were fearful of

swimming out to sea because of the fact that they did not know what lay beneath the surface.

Sharks being the most common fear as stated in most of the questionnaires.

                                              3
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

        I would really like to thank Alex Buttigieg for his constant support and determination

to help and get me through this piece of research. He has been a true inspiration and force of

energy as well as a very good advisor to me all along. I would also like the thank Dr.

Maryanne Lauri for pushing me on to constantly improve my work and Dr. Josef Lauri for his

assistance and guidance when it came to the mathematics, research and data analysis area of

this thesis.

        André Portelli has also been there through this piece of research and was always

calming me down and driving me forward. He helped me to feel prouder and want to work

harder at something that I absolutely love. I would also like to thank those who proof-read my

thesis and helped me perfect my style and my delivery. Last but not least I would also like to

thank my parents for their extreme patience with my stressed moods.

                                              4
Declaration

The undersigned, Alexia Curmi, hereby declares that the research
presented in this dissertation is her own and has never been submitted
for any degree in any other institution.

                                             _____________________
                                                   Alexia Curmi

                                      5
CONTENTS

Abstract ……………………………………………………………………                                                                      iii
Acknowledgments ......................................................................................   iv
Declaration ………………………………………………………………...                                                                        v

1. Introduction ……………………………………………………..……...                                                              1

2. Literature Review ………………………………………………………                                                               4

          2.1 A BRIEF HISTORY ……………………………………………                                                          4

          2.2 THE BURGEONING AGE OF FEAR …………………………                                                      7

          2.3 ATTITUDES AND FEAR ……………………………………..                                                        8

          2.4 CULTURE OF FEAR ………………………………………….                                                          12

          2.5 LEARNING FROM THE MEDIA …………………………….                                                       15

          2.6 SUMMER OF THE SHARK …………………………………..                                                        18

          2.7 THE OMNIPRESENT MEDIA ………………………………..                                                       20

          2.8 MEDIA, FEAR AND ANXIETY ……………………………...                                                     24

          2.9 SHARKS - THE FACTS ………………………………………                                                         31

3. Design and Methodology ………………………………………………                                                             35

4. Results …………………………………………………………………..                                                                   39

5. Discussion ………………………………………………………………                                                                   48

6. Conclusion and Implications ………………………………………….                                                         55

Bibliography ……………………………………………………………...                                                                  57

Appendices ………………………………………………………………..                                                                    60

                                                                6
CHAPTER 1.

                                    INTRODUCTION

                “This elusive quality it is, which causes the thought of whiteness, when
                divorced from more kindly associations, and coupled with an object terrible in
                itself, to heighten the terror to the furthest bounds. Witness…… the white shark
                of the tropics; what but their smooth, flaky whiteness makes them the
                transcendent horrors they are? The ghastly whiteness it is which imparts such
                an abhorrent mildness, even more loathsome than terrific, to the dumb gloating
                of their aspect. So that not the fierce-fanged tiger in his heraldic coat can so
                stagger courage as the … shark.”

                                                HERMAN MELVILLE, Moby Dick (1851).

        A black triangular fin appears in a tropical bay. The pulsating beat of the soundtrack

accelerates as the callous killer locks on to its prey. The fish strikes: cut to a close-up of jaws

and hacking teeth. A cloud of blood and a mutilated bather complete the image. This is the

shark as a film star; the dominant fish that terrorizes humans.

        The jaws of the white shark are the things of legends. A best selling book and a major

motion picture made the mouth of this beast the most terrifying image to ever feature in the

media. The list of the characteristics of the white shark, existent and professed is endless;

savage, fearsome, loathsome, strong, regal, sinister, terrifying, hateful, menacing, malevolent,

pitiless, formidable, and insatiable. But this list does not add up to the white shark; it is both a

further and lesser amount of the sum of its adjectives, more and less than its reputation and a

vast deal more than the miniscule amount that is known about it. It may be the one creature

that the human race will never fully understand. The white shark appears with its reputation

fully developed, yet the truth remains obscure. In this thesis I will attempt to see if the film

                                                  7
‘Jaws’ has created a mass phobia of sharks. I would like to know which are the stories that are

true and which are those led by phobic fear?

       By definition, a phobia is:

               “A persistent, abnormal, and irrational fear of a specific thing or situation that

               compels one to avoid it, despite the awareness and reassurance that it is not

               dangerous.”

                       The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (2004)

       If a fear is not irrational then it is simply a fear and not a phobia. For example, if a fear

of high places prevents a person from crossing necessary bridges to get to work, that fear is

irrational. If certain fears keep person from enjoying life or even preoccupy their thinking so

that they are unable to work, or sleep, or do the things they wish to do, then it becomes

irrational. One key to diagnosing a phobic disorder is that the fear must be excessive and

disproportionate to the situation. Most people who fear heights (but are not phobic of them)

would not avoid visiting a friend who lived on the top floor of a tall building; a person with a

phobia of heights would keep away. Fear alone does not distinguish a phobia; both fear and

avoidance must be evident.

       Selachophobia is described as being a phobia of sharks. Like all fears and phobias, fear

of sharks is created by the unconscious mind as a protective mechanism. At some point in

one’s past, there was an event that likely linked sharks to emotional trauma. Whilst the

original catalyst may have been a real-life scare of some kind, the condition can also be

triggered by myriad, benign events like movies, TV, or perhaps seeing someone else

experience trauma. So long as the negative association is powerful enough, the unconscious
                                                 8
mind thinks the whole situation is clearly very dangerous. It proceeds to attach terrible

feelings to the animal in order to avoid it so that the person will be safe in the future.

Attaching emotions to situations is one of the primary ways that humans learn. The actual

phobia manifests itself in different ways. Some sufferers experience it almost all the time;

others develop it as a response to direct stimuli such as a shark bite or maybe even a horror

film.

        For a long time the author could not determine why ‘Jaws’ had such a profound effect

on popular culture. Why was there such a fixation with sharks, such an eagerness to be

frightened? Journalists, sociologists and psychologists have failed to come up with an

explanation. At last an answer was found. A socio-biologist E.O Wilson writes in Richard

Ellis’ book: ‘Monsters of the Sea’:

               “We are not just afraid of predators, we are transfixed by them, prone to weave

        stories and fables and chatter endlessly about them, because fascination creates

        preparedness, and preparedness- survival. In a deeply tribal sense- we love our

        monsters.”

                                                                       E.O Wilson 2001

                                                 9
CHAPTER 2.

                             LITERATURE REVIEW

2.1 A BRIEF HISTORY

       The trend for reporting gory and often sensational shark attacks could probably date as

far back as 2500 years ago when the Greek historian Herodotus (485-425 BC) described a

dreadful sea battle off Athos, in north-eastern Greece, during which many boats sunk and lots

of sailors were mauled by sharks (Cawardine, 2004, p.92). Several centuries later, in 77 AD,

the Roman naturalist Pliny the Elder reported attacks on sponge fishermen. The sharks’ bad

reputation continued to gather momentum and by the end of the sixteenth century, the French

naturalist Guillaume Rondelet was describing complete human bodies being removed from the

stomachs of large sharks, including one (perhaps apocryphal) occasion, a headless knight in

full suit of armour. Then, in the summer of 1916, a rogue shark struck the New Jersey

shoreline in the Eastern USA, killing several swimmers (Fernicola, 2002, p.24). Popular

opinion at the time suggested that it was a white, but the shark was never properly identified,

and today experts believe it was more likely to have been a bull shark. Whatever it may have

been, it caused sensation. President Woodrow Wilson even called a cabinet meeting to discuss

ways of dealing with the ‘crises’.

       One of the first recorded shark attacks took place in 1580, when a sailor fell overboard

during a voyage from Portugal to India. He was thrown a rope, but as members of the crew

pulled him up, a large shark appeared and tore the poor man apart before he could be hauled to

safety. Another famed attack happened nearly two centuries later, in 1749, when Brooks

Watson, who later became Lord Mayor of London, had one of his legs bitten off by a shark.

                                               10
Notwithstanding the risk of shark attack did not really become a significant fear in the minds

of the public until the 20th century. The Second World War was a major turning point, when

servicemen from shipwrecks and downed planes were suddenly thrown into ‘shark-infested

waters’. The USS Indianapolis is a classic example of the havoc wreaked by sharks during

some of these wartime disasters (Rotman, 1999).

       The warship had been to Guam, delivering components for the Philippines. There were

1199 men on board, when, at midnight on July 30th 1945, the ship was torpedoed by a

Japanese submarine. There was no time to put life-boats into the water. The stricken vessel

sank in only 12 minutes- and the sailors had to leap over-board for their lives. More than 900

managed to get off the ship but only 316 survivors were found. Almost immediately after the

ship sank, sharks came to investigate. Eyewitness accounts suggest that they may have been

oceanic white-tip sharks, but that has never been verified and indeed there could have been

many more possible species to suspect. The men could actually see the sharks circling them in

the clear water below for several terrifying days. Then one night there were anguished screams

as the sharks began to move in for the kill and slowly but surely, most of the sailors were

killed before their shipmates’ eyes.

       In recent years, a more cosmopolitan news-gathering system and a greater demand for

shocking and titillating stories are partly to blame for our obsession with sharks: a headline

reading “Shark Attack” sells newspapers and does little to curtail our fear of them (Rotman,

1999). Another contributing factor is the growing amount of time that people spend by, on or

in the sea. In the USA, for instance, the past 25 years or so has witnessed a mass rush for the

seashore; more than half of the population of 280 million or so people now lives within 80km

of the sea. Many others visit for short periods. Perhaps, inevitably, when so many people are

                                               11
spending so much time in the water, they take a personal interest in what might be lurking in

the hidden depths.

       At least part of the blame must be put on the book ‘Jaws’ published in 1974, and with

the high-profile movies that followed. They literally scared audiences out of the water; and

many shark conservationists believe they were largely responsible for the anti-shark hysteria

that ensued and has gripped the Western world ever since. Even the book’s author, Peter

Benchley, publicly laments the impact of his book on our attitude towards sharks, and is now

actively involved in shark conservation. In the days when ‘Jaws’ was published, many experts

were just as unenlightened (Rotman, 1999). Surprisingly until recently, diving magazines and

books used to suggest that the only sensible thing to do if a shark appeared was to leave the

water. The mere hint of sharks at a diving resort was enough to drive tourists away and

threaten the livelihood of the local operators.

       Statistically, even if you spend a great deal of time in the water, the likelihood of being

attacked by a shark is miniscule. Here are a few revealing comparisons to put the risk into

perspective: there is a far greater chance of winning the lottery than of being attacked by a

shark; more than six times as many people are struck by lightning in America; approximately

300,000 people drown for every person who is bitten by a shark; many times more people

have been known to be killed by coconuts falling on their heads rather than are attacked by a

shark; and, according to figures published by the New York City Health Department; for every

person who is bitten by a shark, 25 people are actually bitten by New Yorkers (Matthews,

1996). Perhaps the most shocking statistic (and this really puts things into perspective) is that

in an average year, for every person killed by a shark the human race kills as many as 25

million sharks (Matthews, 1996). There are many more weird and wonderful facts and figures,

                                                  12
but they all add up to the same thing; sharks are not intent on hurting people at all. If they

were, there would be many, many more fatalities each year due to shark attacks.

       Many people have an immense fear of sharks, especially the infamous white. A lot of

this stems from movies like ‘Jaws’ (which is based on a number of attacks that really

happened in New Jersey in 1916). Some people think that there are masses of sharks waiting

to eat them if they venture out into the ocean, which is simply not true. White sharks rarely

ever eat humans and are solitary animals. They travel alone or in groups of two. Shark attacks

are quite atypical; in the US there are two to three fatal attacks on swimmers, surfers, and

divers per year. By the number of reported incidents, dogs kill more people each year than

whites have killed in the last 100 years.

2.2 THE BURGEONING AGE OF FEAR

       There is no doubt that signs and symbols with the potential to provoke feelings of fear

and anxiety are everywhere. Widespread fear and anxiety is not a new phenomenon, nor is it

limited to our little island of Malta. In fact, documented studies of paranoia date back to

Freud, and instances of extreme anxiety in society are held accountable for such devastating

historical atrocities as the Salem Witch Trials and the Holocaust.

       Is fear of the shark something that goes deeper into our collective psyche? A glimpse

at some dark and summoning evil, heedless of restraint, emerging unpredictably from the

blackness and returning just as silently from whence it came? Is it our atavistic fear of being

eaten that lends detachment and disquieting proximity to the white shark? It is the only beast

                                                13
in our reckoning that we fear can and worse will eat us, and that is the substance of legends. At

any rate, there is something about the shark that has caused communal tremors ever since we

have had the impudence to occupy its domain (Benchley, 1973). Shrewd of Peter Benchley to

name his novel ‘Jaws’ and consequently establish the business end of the white shark as an

enduring fixture in the language of fear.

       Since 1971, with the release of Gimbel’s film ‘Blue Water, White Death’, the seeds

were planted for the growth of the phenomenon that became ‘Jaws’. The novel and the films,

or perhaps just a mass mindset established by the films, sustained in large measure by the

reality of the white shark, have made each of us pause, if only for a moment before entering

the water. And anything that can provoke that same brief, dark thought in all of us is a

powerful force indeed. After 1975, in the wake of ‘Jaws’ the novel and ‘Jaws’, the movie

there was a phenomenal increase in white shark hunting which encouraged deep-sea anglers to

test their mettle and skill against the so-called ‘man-eaters’. The white shark was on its way to

the pantheon of renowned savage beasts, and with the publication of ‘Jaws’, its front rank

place was secured.

2.3 ATTITUDES AND FEAR

       Has a film genre experienced a more miserable devolution than the shark movie? After

beginning so deliciously with ‘Jaws’; Steven Spielberg's high-seas Hitchcock with Roy

Schneider, Richard Dreyfuss, and a mechanical first-time SAG-card holder named Bruce, the

shark movie rapidly spiraled into Jaws 2, 3 (in 3-D), and 4 and pulpy derivations like ‘Deep

Blue Sea’ (with LL Cool J) and ‘Night of the Sharks’, which sounds like a Charles Laughton

film but actually involves drug-smuggling sharks feeling peckish for Treat Williams. Terrible

shark movies have also chomped the credibility of Samuel L. Jackson (devoured in ‘Deep
                                               14
Blue Sea’), Louis Gossett Jr. (nearly digested in ‘Jaws 3’) and Michael Caine, who was spared

in ‘Jaws: The Revenge’ but not before missing his Oscar for ‘Hannah and Her Sisters’ while

marooned on location. The bad-shark-movie genre may have reached its nadir in 2000 with

‘Red Water’, a made-for-TV extravaganza featuring Lou Diamond Phillips, Coolio and the

guy who played the principal on ‘Saved by the Bell’ fumbling along a Louisiana river, hunting

what looks to be a mildly irritable bar of soap. A new trend has also emerged: “Shark Attack

1, 2 and 3”. The third deals with the now more popular image of megalodon, a larger ancestor

of the white. There are already 2 movies dealing with this huge 20 metre plus monster.

       The reason for such an attitude towards sharks may stem from myriads of things- fear

of the unknown, fear of things that are not within our control, or even the very lucid fear of

being eaten alive. Attitudes are evaluative judgements about an object, issue, and person and

so on. According to Allport (as cited in Hogg & Vaughan, 2002), attitudes are:

               “A mental and neutral state of readiness organised through experience, exerting

               a directive or dynamic influence upon the individual’s response to all objects

               and situations with which it is related”

       There has been much debate regarding what constitutes an attitude. One accepted view

is that of McGuire who suggests that an attitude comprises of the affective, behavioural and

cognitive mechanisms (1989) (as cited in Wiggins, Wiggins & Vander Zanden, 1994). This

three-component attitude model accentuates the significance of thought, feeling and action as

fundamental to the human experience. Attitudes have certain definable characteristics for

example; they are comparatively lasting (a person who does not like sharks now will probably

still not like them in ten years time), they are limited to publicly significant events or objects;

and they are generalisable (for a person who has seen ‘Jaws’, the experience may have led
                                                 15
him/her to fear all sharks, not just the one depicted in the film and therefore develop an

attitude towards them). Attitudes influence how we process information, remember events,

and shape our view of the world. There have been various theories put forward on what

attitudes are and how they affect man, some of which I shall now explain.

        Devine (1989) has suggested that attitudes are implicit and automatic judgements of

which the person holding them is unaware (Hogg & Vaughan, 2002). Attitudes may be

affected by classical conditioning-, which is possible in the case of ‘Jaws’. Assuming that a

shark was a neutral stimulus beforehand, through repeated association of the film ‘Jaws’ with

frightening stimuli, blood and gore, the neutral image of a shark would now bring about fear

and anger. Attitudes are also very much affected by the mass media, particularly in cases

where that certain attitude is not as yet strongly held (Goldberg & Gorn, 1974).

        Most psychologists such as Allport and Bandura concur that attitudes are learned

through mere exposure, conditioning, and socialization. Specifically, attitudes can be acquired

from others (i.e. social learning) in the form of classical conditioning, instrumental

conditioning, and modelling; as well as being acquired via direct experience. Daily exposure

to television provides a centralized mass media production of a coherent set of images and

messages produced for total populations, and in its relatively non-selective, almost ritualistic

use by most viewers. This total pattern accounts for the historically new and distinct

consequences of living with television as a cultivation of shared conceptions of reality among

otherwise diverse populations. Compared to other media, television provides a relatively

restricted set of choices for a virtually unrestricted variety of interests and public gratification.

Most of its programs distribute material by commercial necessity designed to be watched by

large and heterogeneous audiences in a relatively non-selective fashion. Media tends to have a

long and lasting effect on the way our attitudes are formed and kept intact.
                                                 16
The Source Credibility theory states that people are more likely to be persuaded when

the source presents itself as credible (Hovland, Janis & Kelley, 1953). Therefore if news

casters present themselves, and the information they are giving as credible, people are more

likely to believe what they are saying. The theory is broken into three models that can be used

to more aptly apply the theory. The names of those models are: the factor model, the

functional model, and the constructivist model. The three models help to narrow the wide

scope of the source credibility theory, while also making it a much more focused strategy to

use when studying communication. The factor model (a covering laws approach) helps

determine to what extent the receiver judges the source as credible (Hovland., Janis & Kelley,

1953). The functional model (also a covering laws approach) views credibility as the degree

to which a source satisfies a receiver's individual needs (Hovland, Janis & Kelley, 1953). The

constructivist model (a human action approach) analyzes what the receiver does with the

source's proposal (Hovland, Janis & Kelley, 1953).

       Priming is a term used to describe incidents when people witness, read or hear of an

event via the mass media, ideas having a similar meaning are activated in them for a short

while afterwards, and these thoughts in turn can activate other semantically related ideas and

action tendencies (Reber & Reber, 1995). This theory derives from a cognitive-neo-associative

perspective that regards memory as a collection of networks, with each network consisting of

units or nodes that represent substantive elements of thoughts, feelings and so forth, linked

through associated pathways. The presentation of a certain stimulus primes other semantically

related concepts, thus heightening the likelihood that thoughts with much the same meaning as

the presented stimulus will come to mind. Priming in the media refers to a process by which

certain portions of media content are brought to the forefront and certain other portions are

relegated to the back ground. This process allows the media to exercise control over public

opinion.
                                               17
2.4 CULTURE OF FEAR

        Fear is a word familiar to everyone. At one time or another, something has caused one

to be “afraid,” claim that one is “scared” or given one the “creeps.” But what, exactly, is fear?

The two-and-a-half pages that it takes the Oxford English Dictionary (1961) to properly define

the word, in all its forms and uses, are a good indication of the complexity of the term. The

word originates from the old English “faer” for sudden calamity or danger, and was later used

to describe the ensuing emotion. Fear can encompass various feelings and emotions; fear may

have an explicit meaning for one person but may signify something entirely different for

another. Some people fear the dark, others are afraid of drowning, and still others simply fear

failing a test.

        Psychological science has identified four influences on our intuitions about risk and

fear. First, we fear what our ancestral history has prepared us to fear. Human emotions were

tried and tested in the Stone Age. Yesterday's risks prepare us to fear snakes, sharks, and

spiders. Secondly, we fear what we cannot control. Skiing, by one estimate, poses 1000 times

the health and injury risk of food preservatives. Yet many people gladly assume the risk of

skiing, which they control, but avoid preservatives. Third, we fear what's immediate. For

example; teens are indifferent to smoking's toxicity because they live more for the present than

the far distant future. Fourth, we fear what's most readily available in memory. This would

explain why thousands of safe car trips have extinguished our initial anxieties about driving.

In less familiar realms, vivid, memorable images dominate our fears. We can know that

unprovoked white shark attacks have claimed merely 67 lives worldwide since 1876. Yet

after watching ‘Jaws’ and reading vivid accounts of the 2001 Atlantic coastal shark attacks,

                                               18
individuals may feel chills on entering the water, for fear of coming across something that may

harm or worse, kill them.

       Fear can be what keeps you up at night, what prevents you from participating in certain

activities or what motivates you to strive harder. Fear is timeless; it is an intrinsic human

emotion that has been used for different means throughout history, for both good and bad. For

example, many societies used the fear of God or some other supernatural entity to keep people

in line. Fear is also a mechanism used to bind cultures together; certain societies can be

defined by what instils fear in them. Fear can be as much of a tradition as an emotion, as much

of a unifier as a divider. However, most generally and for the purposes of this thesis, fear is

meant to describe:

       “The emotion of pain or uneasiness caused by the sense of impending danger or by the

       prospect of some possible evil”.

                                       The Oxford English Dictionary, 196, pp. 114 –116

       Although people generally regard fear with a negative connotation, it is fundamentally

a valuable protective response. In the words of Marks:

               “Fear is a vital evolutionary legacy that leads an organism to avoid threat, and
       has obvious survival value. It is an emotion produced by the perception of present or
       impending danger and is normal in appropriate situations. Without fear few would
       survive long under natural conditions. Fear girds our loins for rapid action in the face
       of danger and alerts us to perform well under stress. It helps us fight the enemy, drive
       carefully, parachute safely, take exams, speak well to a critical audience, keep a
       foothold in climbing a mountain.”
                                                                     Marks, I.M 1987, p.3

                                                19
Both literature and films dealing with the subject of fear offer interesting viewpoints,

and each work brings up different and interesting perspectives on the matter. However, there is

not much argument about whether we are living in a fear-saturated society, or whether our

fears are often disproportionate to the actual risks. All seem to agree on this. Nor is there much

dispute that too much fear over wrong things is bad both for individuals and for society. The

principal disagreement is over what shapes these fears, where they originate from and why

they become so menacing to some people. Many media theorists claim that fears are primarily

a product of society and the current environment of the time. Furedi (2002) contends that

while disasters and catastrophes have happened throughout history it is the mood of society at

that time determines how people react to these events.

       Altheide (2002) believes that fear is a social product and not an individual failing.

Specifically, he believes that “fear is a manufactured response that has been produced by a

mass-mediated symbol machine.” (Altheide, 2002). His main argument is that the mass media

and popular culture are the most important contributors to fear. He explains that fear defines a

certain cultural space that is shaped by experiences, interpretations, and narratives conveyed

through storytellers like parents, journalists, and others who uncannily connect something new

with something old. ‘Jaws’ was an example of just that- a terrifying new blockbuster that

brought the old fear of being eaten alive back to the surface. Altheide (2002) refers to fear as:

               “The pervasive communication, symbolic awareness, and expectation that

               danger and risk are central features of the effective environment or the physical

               and symbolic environment as people define and experience it in everyday life.”

                                                                      Altheide, 2002, p.23

                                                20
2.5 LEARNING FROM THE MEDIA

       Words like fear, terror, evil, anxiety, paranoia, risk, threat, enemy, victim and the like

permeate the media. Before the invention of mass media, the individual’s imagination and

fears were primarily shaped by personal experiences. However with the surge in electronic and

wireless communication, people in the developed world have access to sophisticated media

outlets bringing that world directly to them, forcing them to increasingly rely on the media to

keep them informed about that world. Altheide (2002) holds that the mass media and popular

culture are part of our environment, and believes that the mass media provide for citizens the

bulk of cultural experiences about crime and fear (Altheide, 2002). Although it is difficult to

gauge the actual power the media has in determining people’s perception of the world, studies

suggest that repeated exposure to consistent media portals and themes influences people’s

perception of those items in the direction of media portrayals (Wimmer & Dominick, 2000).

       Altheide (2002) further contends that the fear that has consumed today’s society has

been produced through the interaction of commercial media, entertainment formats and

programming, and the rise of the problem frame. He describes the problem frame as:

               “Promoting a discourse of fear that may be defined as the pervasive

       communication, symbolic awareness and expectation that danger and risk is a central

       feature of the effective environment. The mass media, in general, and especially the

       electronic news media, are part of a ‘problem generating machine’ geared to

       entertainment, voyeurism and the quick fix.”

                                                                      Altheide, 2002, p.41

                                               21
Fear seems to be the great engine of news and the media. It focuses and engages the

mind. It demands a ranking of dangers. It searches close and far paths for safety. This is why

reports of terror trump the fear of growing fat, and as the fear recedes into the blackness of the

mind, so will the thirst for relevant news. Once the fear returns, so does the demand for

terrifying news related to it. This could be an explanation as to why people hardly ever hear of

shark attacks in the winter. There are much less people in the water and the fear has gone into

hibernation; only to return the next summer, as will the sensationalism of the news. “If it

bleeds it leads” (Young, 2003).

       The media like to appeal to emotions because they cause arousal, and we generally like

to be aroused whether it is happiness, sadness or fear. The easiest emotion to arouse is fear, so

it makes sense that a vast amount of what the media produces attempts to strike a cord with

our fears. It is also not the intent of this thesis to debate what the media’s motives are in doing

this, whether for advertising demands, to drive in consumers, save money or meet corporate

demands. The point is to understand how the trend for the media to present increasingly

negative stories may affect viewers, and to calculate what the consequences may be in regard

to instilling fear and anxiety. So, if people often learn about the world from the media, and

which often portrays graphic and fearful images, how might this be a cause for increased,

prolonged fear and anxiety in individuals?

       Despite evidence that people increasingly learn about the world from media sources,

few people seem to think the media influence them. In her book ‘Screened Out’, Brooks

Johnston (2000) warned:

                                                22
“The tragedy is that most of us have no idea how much the media influence,

               often control, our feelings and behavior. True to the definition of propaganda,

               people today do not realize they are being programmed any more than did the

               people of Germany, Italy, Japan and the Soviet Union.”

                                                              Brooks Johnston, 2000, p. 11

       The frequency and recency of construct activation can have significant influence on a

person’s cognitions. Generally, the more frequently and recently a construct is activated, the

easier it is to recall and recognize. The Recency theory of social cognition is intrinsically

linked with frequency. In regards to media effects, particularly considering cultivation theory,

heavy media consumers should more frequently activate constructs portrayed on television

than light consumers, especially if those constructs are portrayed more on television than

occur in the real-world situations. Moreover, given that heavy viewers have a higher

probability of having viewed recently than light viewers; accessibility may be enhanced for

heavy viewers through the recency of viewing as well. Recency theory suggests that images

seen most recently dominate people’s thoughts. In effect, things that are shown frequently are

often also seen recently since the chances of incidence of viewing are heightened (Haberlandt,

1997, pp. 231-236).

       Considering that most of what we know is learned from personal experience or from

information presented by the media, the media have great potential to influence people’s

perception of reality. It has also been demonstrated that if an individual sees something

frequently, he or she tends to assume it has a high rate of occurrence. In effect, if the media

routinely reports regularly a sensational news item, audiences are likely to believe that the

                                                23
problem is prevalent and a serious threat to society. An example of such a thing is the summer

of the shark news bonanza in 2001 which will be discussed shortly.

       Closely related to social cognition theory is cultivation theory, which suggests that

repeated exposure to consistent media outlets and themes influences our perception of these

items in the direction of media portrayals (Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, Signorielli & Shanahan,

(Ed). 2002). As a result, audience perception of the real world is often skewed towards the

media’s representation of the world instead of their actual social environment. Therefore, if

the media present sharks as being instinctively driven toward the pursuit of human flesh- then

that is how people will view them.

2.6 SUMMER OF THE SHARK

       Unfortunately fear, not respect, is what makes news. And the summer of 2001 was no

exception. Fear reached an all time high as a direct result of a media feeding frenzy that had

not been seen before and has not been seen since. It began at dusk on July 6th 2001, when an

eight-year-old boy by the name of Jesse Arborgast lost an arm and a third of one thigh during

an attack by a 2.1m bull shark in shallow water off Pensacola Florida. His quick thinking

uncle somehow managed to wrestle the shark ashore and, with the help of a park ranger,

retrieved Jesse’s severed arm from its mouth. He survived, and doctors even managed to re-

attach his arm. Time magazine proclaimed it the ‘summer of the Shark’ on the cover of its

30th July issue. Then, on Labour Day weekend that September, a ten-year-old boy was killed

by a shark at Virginia Beach in Virginia, and a 28-year-old man was killed in North Carolina.

                                               24
Shark attacks became a major news item, until September 11th, when international news took

the centre stage (Strano, 2004).

       Almost instantly, the world had been led to believe that 2001 was an exceptional year

for the shark attack, with the press keeping count of the supposed carnage rampages,

particularly along the east coast of the USA. Self-proclaimed experts came up with various

theories to explain the non-existent problem, ranging from a lack of fish prey to a population

explosion of bull sharks. There was even a story on CNN 2001 claiming Fidel Castro had

concocted a scheme to breed dangerous sharks and let them loose on an unsuspecting

American public. As Peter Benchley, the author of ‘Jaws’ commented; “Never before has so

much ink and so much airtime been devoted to so few events of little national or international

consequence”. The ‘summer of the Shark’ was complete fiction. By US standards, 2001 was

an average year; by international standards, it was actually below average (Miller, 2003).

       The facts proved that the hysteria was completely and utterly generated by the media.

As a result, there was no summer of the shark. Around the world, the number of unprovoked

shark attacks recorded in 2001 was 68, down from 78 in the previous year; the number of

serious shark attacks was down too: there were 4 fatalities in 2001 and 11 the year before. In

the USA, the number of unprovoked attacks was 53 in 2001, exactly one less than in 2000.

Many more statistics, based on data recorded over the years, support the view that 2001 was a

fairly normal year by any standard. What should have made the headlines, perhaps, was the

fact that 4000 people drowned in 2001 in the USA alone. That should have put things into

perspective. But even if 2000 had been an exceptional year with a record number of shark

attacks, experts do not assign too much significance to year-by-year variability. They view

short term trends- up or down- with a certain amount of caution. There are simply too many

                                              25
variables, from the weather to the economic climate that can influence the local abundance of

sharks and the number of people in the water (and therefore the odds of an attack).

(Cawardine, 2004).

2.7 THE OMNIPRESENT MEDIA

       Considering that today’s society is the most media-saturated in the developed world,

and that media messages are increasingly more realistic, more graphic and more negative, it

certainly seems possible that the media may be at least somewhat responsible for the present

state of heightened fear and anxiety. Media images invade homes through an endless number

of television channels, radio stations, Internet connections, print publications and even

telephone outlets. In the car, people listen to the radio, pass elaborate billboards, see buses

adorned with advertisements, stop by numerous news-stands and encounter the occasional

political bumper sticker.

       In the office, waiting room, café, shopping centre and even classroom, there is usually

a television and sometimes a computer. There is almost always a newspaper or magazine

nearby, and as long as there are other people around, someone is likely to be talking about

something they heard or saw from the media. Clearly, there are increasingly fewer places

where media messages are not in our face, or at least easily accessible. Not only are media

messages increasingly disturbing, but with the rise of non-stop live coverage of events and

breaking news reports, audience exposure to media images is often unplanned and incidental.

Scary stories about ‘new dangers’ do not simply make people more anxious or fearful. Such

stories often reinforce pre-existing apprehensions and help to shape and even alter the way in

which people conduct their lives.

                                                26
Anxiety about the impact of the mass media on children has increased over the last few

years. As more and more evidence comes to light that TV, movies and videos can have a

harmful effect, and as the media are becoming more invasive, more intrusive, and more

unsettling in content, many parents are at a loss as to what to do to protect their children.

Television seems to be an especially intimidating form of media because it brings into the

home that which parents would never actually allow their children to be exposed to. No one

would deliver anything unordered to any given home, but television provides content that is

sometimes disturbing and this content is readily available, day or night at the touch of a

button.

          In an American study conducted by Harrison and Cantor (2002), 90% of the

participants reported an intense fear reaction to something in the media. This number is

especially telling, since the respondents could have received full extra credit for participating

in the study if they simply said “no,” meaning they never had such an experience, and thereby

avoid writing a paper and filling out a three-page questionnaire (Cantor, 2002). Joanne Cantor

researched the prevalence of enduring fear responses to television or film. They asked 103

first year college students to fill out a questionnaire dealing with lasting impressions that a

fearful film or television programme had left on them.

          Ninety six people completed the questionnaire and reported a lingering fright reaction.

Here are some statistics Cantor compiled: students recounted many enduring effects: 22%

stated mental preoccupation with what they had seen. In their words they "couldn't get the

movie off their mind" or they "couldn't get those disturbing images out of their head." Forty

                                                 27
percent evaded or dreaded the situation depicted in the movie or program; effects like

declining to swim in the ocean after seeing ‘Jaws’, being hesitant about taking a shower after

watching ‘Psycho’, or spiders after any number of arachnid-infested horror films (Cantor,

2002: 56 ).

       Eleven percent generalized these aversions to associated circumstances -- for example,

it was surprising how many people recall giving up swimming in lakes or even pools after

seeing ‘Jaws’. The most regular outstanding effects involve 45 % more trouble with eating or

sleeping. More common effects were nightmares, insomnia and the refusal to sleep alone. In

fact, the phrase "I slept with my parents for two whole weeks" was so common in such

retrospective reports that Cantor called the first chapter of her book ‘The Suddenly Crowded

Queen-Size Bed.’

       The most notable data to emerge from this study is related to the extent of these

residual effects. Figure 1 overleaf shows this data. Only one-fifth of these students said the

effects lasted less than a day, and only a third said the effects lasted less than a week. An

astonishing 33% said the effects lasted more than a year. Finally, one fourth of these students

said that the effects of what they had seen (an average of six years earlier) were still ongoing.

                                                28
Figure 1- Lasting affects of frightening media messages

                                                                              Cantor 2002

    It is no wonder now, three decades after its release, with ‘Jaws’ still being shown as a

television movie, that children continue to be afraid of sharks and believe that what they see is

actually real. Whilst playing a game called ‘what sound does that animal make?’ with my

three nephews and nieces I proceeded to ask what sound sharks make- knowing full well that

they don’t make any noise whatsoever (except for the slight “whoosh” sound when they swim

past). They immediately began to recite the score from ‘Jaws’; the pounding “Da Dum Da

Dum”, even though they have never seen any of the films. Thanks to the film ‘Jaws’, whites

are considered to be the serial killers of the sea (not to mention the only predators that attack

to their own universally recognized theme music).

                                                      29
One of the most salient negative cognitions in fear is the sense of uncontrollability. This

feeling is typified by a state of helplessness due to a perceived inability to predict, control, or

obtain desired results. Does it ever seem that the victims in ‘Jaws’ appear ridiculously

helpless, succumbing to the sheer power and might of the shark? In fact, most anxiety

disorders seem to be rooted in these thoughts of helplessness, unpredictability and

uncontrollability. Overwhelming fears about flying, cancer, being eaten alive or natural

disasters, to name but a few, usually revolve around a person’s sense of inevitable victim

hood. The way that the media present these things as not only unpredictable, but also highly

probable only serves to accentuate these anxieties. People may attempt to engage in activities

that will decrease their vulnerability, such as the bubble curtains used to ward off sharks from

Australian beaches. However, when these people realize that such actions do not protect them

they feel even more helpless and their fear may become more acute.

2.8 MEDIA, FEAR AND ANXIETY

               “I am seriously considering eliminating television watching and newspaper
       reading from my daily experience – at least for a while. Every time I turn on the
       television to watch the news or open the Washington Post to catch a glimpse of current
       events while drinking my morning coffee, I end up with a nauseating pain in the pit of
       my stomach. It is not the coffee; my reaction is caused by the news. It has been this
       way for several months now, and the stories are getting increasingly worse – more
       gruesome, more unbelievable, more devastating, more hopeless, and just sad.”

       The preceding quote was obtained from an article in the winter 2002 issue of the

American Psychological Association of Graduate Student’s (APAGS) newsletter. The author,

Carol Williams-Nickelson, is the associate executive director of the APAGS. The media in

general seem to portray such an array of dismal and unpleasant images that increased exposure

                                                 30
may negatively affect some people. One media scholar George Gerbner, defined a specific

syndrome that results from excessive exposure to harmful media images: Gerbner’s ‘mean

world syndrome’. From his studies, George Gerbner concluded that growing up with this

“unprecedented diet of violence” has three consequences, which he calls the “mean world

syndrome.” (Gerbner, 1994). The basic assumption is that if you grow up in a home where you

watch more than three hours of television per day, for all practical purposes, you live in a

meaner world and act accordingly, rather than your next-door neighbour who lives in the same

world but watches less television. In other words, watch enough brutality on TV, and one

comes to believe one is living in a cruel and gloomy world in which one feels vulnerable and

insecure. As a result, television programming reinforces the worst fears and apprehensions and

paranoia of people. Gerbner maintains that television is more than just programs. Rather, he

believes, “television is a mythology – highly organically connected, repeated every day so that

the themes run through all programming and news have the effect of cultivating conceptions

of reality.” (Gerbner, 1994).

       The most debilitating consequence that Gerbner observes is the pervasive sense of

insecurity and vulnerability that television watching instills. It should be reiterated here that

increased vulnerability, or helplessness, are good indicators that a person is at risk of

developing an anxiety disorder. The aim of this study is to define what role the media in

particular may play in intensifying, sometimes even instigating these emotions. From media

content to media effects, several ideas and theories about fear and anxiety in relation to the

media will be explored.

       Generally, the term ‘media’ is meant to include all means of mass communication that

facilitate the dissemination of information. The most popular media are film, television, print,

                                                31
radio and the internet. Whether they offer news items regarding health, politics, and current

affairs, or entertainment pieces about recreational activities, celebrities and fiction, millions of

people each day are exposed to their messages on a daily basis. Media content dealing with

news probably has the most potential to invoke fear and anxiety because it purportedly offers

fact-based information about the world around us, and more often than not paints quite a bleak

picture of that world. Although all media forms essentially relay information to the public,

they all have a unique quality and distinct characteristics that separate them from one another.

While newspapers can offer daily accounts of news, and sometimes even a late second edition,

they cannot provide the most up-to-the minute reporting that radio, television and the Internet

can produce. Print media also requires a literate audience, and is limited to publishing still

images and photographs. Radio can report events as they are happening and have a basically

unlimited group of potential listeners.

       It is also probably the cheapest and easiest medium to access. People can listen to the

radio in the car, while in the shower, while running, at work or even at sea. People can listen

to the radio virtually anywhere so long as there is a receiver that can pick up a signal. Radio

can also utilise sound and music to enhance its messages, and the tone of the announcer’s

voice can influence how the message is received. Despite all its advantages, radio lacks any

visual capabilities. As a result, it cannot impose any graphic images on its listener; only the

imagination of each listener can create visuals, which are the key to a lasting impression. The

Internet, the latest medium of mass communication has basically all the capabilities of any

other media portal. It can serve as a newspaper, radio, telephone and even television. It

enables users to conduct their own research, access multiple sources of information, and

communicate with both other users and media organizations.

                                                 32
While the internet is constantly growing in user activity and in capabilities, it has yet to

replace television as the most customary medium for obtaining information. Studies have

shown that most people will get their information from more traditional media first and use the

Internet to gain background or more specific in-depth coverage of a certain issue. Also, access

is still somewhat limited, especially in times of crisis, (for example: following September 11

attacks, servers became overloaded and not all users may have been able to obtain access).

Hence film and television are the remaining modes of popular mass communication. The

nature of visual media often makes its messages more salient than those received via audio or

print formats. Technological advances have given images unprecedented power to depict

events as if they were happening in front of the viewer. Film in particular can include high-

tech visual graphics and sound rivalled by no other medium.

       While some argue that it is inappropriate to apply technically medical terms like

anxiety or paranoia to people’s reactions to the media, the author disagrees and would argue

that the media has the potential to cause real psychological responses in individuals. All

people are unique, and the risk of being affected by media images and messages differs

according to a variety of factors. It is also true that although the media may exaggerate or

overplay certain problems, their reports are usually grounded in legitimate concerns. However,

my previous discussion holds that the media plays a significant role in creating a heightened

sense of fear and anxiety in today’s society. I also contend that, in some instances, media

images have the potential to cause extreme psychological reactions in certain individuals like

those in Cantor’s study. Finally, it seems that media-induced fear and anxiety could be

harmful, and that while there are some reasonable things to be worried about, our biggest

dilemma may, in fact, be fear itself.

                                               33
Simply stated, fear is the usually unpleasant feeling that arises as a normal response to

realistic danger. Anxiety is an emotion similar to fear, but it arises without a clear, objective

source of danger. Compared with anxiety, fear has more physiological associations and its

cause is more obvious. Unlike fear, anxiety is not aroused by predictable threats. Fear is

usually short-term and involves specific phobias, while anxiety is more long-term and

typically generalized. It could be said that if fear refers to something definite, then anxiety has

a quality of indefiniteness and lack of object. Anxiety feeds upon our unknown elements of

our fears.

       Sharks get all the publicity; even kids' movies love them for example ‘Bruce’ in

‘Finding Nemo’. There are plenty of other things that can eat you too. ‘Alligator’ (1980): A

cute baby alligator is flushed down the toilet that grows up to be 36-foot mutant monster,

thanks to a steady diet of research lab animals. Problems arise when he develops a taste for

sewer workers. Other films that include monstrous animals are: ‘The Birds’ (1963). ‘Cujo’

(1983) where rabies makes all the difference between man's best friend and worst nightmare.

‘The Ghost and the Darkness’ (1996) where in Africa, 1898, two lions actually killed 130

people in less than a year with demon-like intelligence. The truth is honestly much scarier than

fiction. ‘Night of the Lepus’ (1972), Dr. McCoy (DeForest Kelley) battles mutant bunny

rabbits. ‘Orca: Killer Whale’ (1977)-the movie that dares to reverse ‘Moby Dick’. The late,

great Richard Harris is relentlessly pursued across time and space by a killer whale bent on

revenge. ‘Piranha’ (1978) includes flesh-eating fish are released into the river and spawn many

happy beginnings and many happy meals out of men.

       A lot of what people know or think they know comes from stories they hear or

programs they see rather than from personal experience. This phenomenon not only refers to

                                                34
You can also read