SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING (HAPPINESS) - Continuing ...

Page created by Joe May
 
CONTINUE READING
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING (HAPPINESS)

                                                    Presented by

                              CONTINUING PSYCHOLOGY EDUCATION INC.

                              6 CONTINUING EDUCATION CONTACT HOURS

“Happiness and misery depend as much on temperament as on fortune.”
                            Francois de la Rochefoucauld, Maximes

Course Objective                                              Learning Objectives
The purpose of this course is to provide an                     Upon completion, the participant will be able to:
understanding of the concept of happiness,                      1. Describe major components and specific
entitled “subjective well-being” within                             elements of happiness.
psychological literature. Major topics include                   2. Discuss demographic effects, such as sex,
the demographic correlates of happiness,                            age, income, education, etc., upon
associated traits, the effect of personality and                    subjective well-being.
temperament, and theories explaining personality’s               3. Acknowledge environmental effects
influence on happiness.                                              including marriage, job satisfaction, health,
                                                                     leisure, competencies, etc., upon happiness.
Accreditation                                                    4. Understand causes, predictors, and
Continuing Psychology Education Inc. is recognized                    correlates of happiness, and accompanying
by the New York State Education Department’s                        theoretical justification.
State Board for: Social Work as an approved provider             5. Explain key personality traits which
of continuing education for licensed social workers                 promote greater well-being, such as
#SW-0387; Mental Health Practitioners as an approved                extroversion, optimism, self-esteem, etc.
provider of continuing education for licensed mental             6. Determine personality and temperament
health counselors #MHC-0080, and licensed marriage                  effects upon happiness.
and family therapists MFT-0043; Psychology as an                 7. Discuss the relationship between
approved provider of continuing education for licensed              personality and happiness.
psychologists #PSY-0006.

Mission Statement
Continuing Psychology Education provides the                  Faculty
highest quality continuing education designed to              Neil Eddington, Ph.D.
fulfill the professional needs and interests of mental        Richard Shuman, LMFT
health professionals. Resources are offered to
improve professional competency, maintain
knowledge of the latest advancements, and meet
continuing education requirements mandated by
the profession.                                                Copyright © 2021 Continuing Psychology Education Inc.

1 Continuing Psychology Education Inc. P.O. Box 12202 Albany, NY 12212 FAX: (858) 272-5809 phone: 1 800 281-5068
                                    Email: contpsyched@netzero.com www.texcpe.com
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

INTRODUCTION                                                      with specific life domains, the presence of frequent positive
                                                                  affect (pleasant moods and emotions), and a relative absence
   Since the dawn of civilization great thinkers have discussed   of negative affect (unpleasant moods and emotions).
the quality of human existence and “the good life.” To some         The major components are reduced into more specific
individuals the ideal state is one of wealth, to others, having   elements. Positive affect is commonly divided into joy,
significant relationships, while some report helping those in     elation, contentment, pride, affection, happiness and ecstasy.
need is central. These individuals vary in external               Negative affect is separated into guilt and shame, sadness,
circumstance, yet they may share a subjective feeling of          anxiety and worry, anger, stress, depression, and envy. Life
wellbeing.                                                        satisfaction is categorized by satisfaction with current life,
   The term “subjective well-being” (SWB) refers to people’s      satisfaction with past, satisfaction with future, significant
evaluations of their lives - including cognitive judgments,       others’ views of one’s life, and desire to change life. Domain
such as life satisfaction; and affective evaluations (moods       satisfaction is composed of work, family, leisure, health,
and emotions), such as positive and negative emotional            finances, self, and one’s group.
feelings. People are said to have high SWB if they are              The field of SWB has several cardinal characteristics
satisfied with their life-conditions, and experience frequent     (Diener, 1984). First, it is concerned with well-being from
positive emotions and infrequent negative emotions. SWB is        the perspective of the respondent, hence, importance is
the psychological term for “happiness” and is preferred due       granted to the respondent’s own views of his or her life.
to the many connotations of the latter term. Within the           Second, the researcher is mainly interested in long-term
literature, the terms are used interchangeably.                   levels of satisfaction and affect, though, short-term moods
   The scientific study of subjective well-being has shown        and emotions are studied as well. For example, a state lottery
increasing interest the past two decades as psychology            winner will be examined longitudinally for stable and
progressed from radical behaviorism and emphasis on               permanent changes in mood and life satisfaction. Third,
negative states. Psychological articles researching negative      healthy personality variables are researched, not only
states outnumber those examining positive states by a ratio of    negative states such as depression and anxiety. Attainment
17 to 1 (Myers & Diener, 1995). SWB researchers explore           of SWB not only involves avoiding sadness, but also
the full range of psychological well-being such that focus is     experiencing life satisfaction and pleasant emotions.
upon factors that keep one from being depressed and factors         Transient factors such as current mood and even current
that lead one to becoming elated. This trend is not surprising    weather conditions affect judgment of life satisfaction
because happiness and life satisfaction are major goals for       (Schwarz and Strack, 1991), however, despite these
most people. Emphasis is placed on understanding the              temporary perceptions, SWB is moderately stable across
processes which underlie happiness, in turn, people’s goals,      situations (Diener and Larsen, 1984) and across the life span
coping efforts, and dispositions are studied.                     (Costa and McCrae, 1988; Magnus and Diener, 1991).
   The interaction of one’s personality, personal goals, and      Hence, as we move through life our goals and needs change
available resources affects SWB and highlights the relative       but SWB remains somewhat stable.
importance of these variables across the life span. Our wants       A significant proportion of stable SWB is due to
and resources to accomplish goals change over time and offer      personality. Francois La Rochefoucauld stated that
insight into the role these domains play in subjective            “happiness and misery depend as much on temperament as
wellbeing.                                                        on fortune.” Research supports this notion that pleasant or
   External demographic factors which change across the life      unpleasant emotion, and life satisfaction vary more in
span such as income, health, and social contacts have a           accordance with temperament than life circumstances or
surprisingly small effect on SWB. Diener and Suh (1998)           momentary factors. Campbell, Converse, and Rodgers
revealed that from ages 40 to 90, average income and percent      (1976) found that the demographic factors of age, sex,
of people who are married drop steadily, yet mean levels of       income, race, education, and marital status accounted for less
life satisfaction remain stable across age groups.                than 20% of the variance in SWB. Argyle (1999) determined
   As many nations of the world enter an era of                   that external circumstances account for approximately 15%
postmaterialism in which basic survival needs are met,            of the variance in SWB. It is concluded that personal
interest in SWB is likely to grow affecting ways                  reactions to life’s circumstances are more important than the
policymakers govern. Continued research into SWB may              events themselves and that personality affects our reactions.
ultimately answer the question of what composes the “good         In fact, personality is one of the strongest and most consistent
life.”                                                            predictors of subjective well-being.

COMPONENTS OF SUBJECTIVE                                          CAUSES AND CORRELATES OF
WELL-BEING                                                        HAPPINESS

  Subjective well-being is composed of several major                Extensive research exists on the correlations of
components, including global life satisfaction, contentment       demographic and other environmental factors with happiness.

2   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

These findings started with Cantril’s (1965) study of 23,875       opportunity for intense levels of happiness upon encountering
people in 11 countries, the research of Bradburn (1969) and        good events.
Campbell, Converse, and Rodgers (1976) in the United
States, and Inglehart’s (1990) analysis of Eurobarometer           AGE
studies of 16 countries with over 163,000 respondents.
Veenhoven and colleagues (1994) later reviewed 603 such              Campbell (1981) stated that “the literary image of the
studies from 69 countries. It is concluded that demographic        crotchety old person, dissatisfied with everything, is not a
and environmental factors affect happiness at varying levels,      very realistic picture of older people” (p. 203). This pleasant
but to a lesser degree than personality.                           finding may be due to older people being healthier and
                                                                   staying involved in more life domains compared to past
SEX DIFFERENCES                                                    generations (Bass, 1995). Current studies agree that life
                                                                   satisfaction often increases, or at least does not drop with age
   Sex differences in SWB are small or nonexistent in              (Herzog & Rodgers, 1981; Horley & Lavery, 1995; Larson,
Western nations. The greatest data comes from the World            1978; Stock, Okun, Haring, & Witter, 1983).
Value Survey (Inglehart, 1990) in which approximately                International studies of representative samples from
170,000 representatively sampled respondents from 16               multiple countries also indicate that life satisfaction does not
nations were surveyed; the differences in SWB between men          decline with age (Butt & Beiser, 1987; Inglehart, 1990,
and women were very small. Michalos (1991) studied                 Veenhoven, 1984). Diener and Suh (1998) surveyed 60,000
18,000 college students in over 30 nations and found very          adults from 40 nations and discovered a slight upward trend
small sex differences in life satisfaction and happiness.          in life satisfaction from the 20s to the 80s in age, and little
   Due to the breaking down of traditional sex roles for men       change across age cohorts with respect to the experience of
and women, the trait which was formerly called                     negative affect. In contrast, pleasant affect declined with
“masculinity” is now called “agency,” and the trait formerly       age. Likewise, Okma and Veenhoven (1996) found no
called “femininity” is now called “communion.”                     evidence of a decrease in life satisfaction across the life span
Communion includes characteristics as warmth, concern for          but did observe a small decline in mood. The reported age
others, and understanding. Agency includes independence,           declines in positive affect may disappear, however, if lower
selfconfidence, and decisiveness. These new terms are              arousal states such as contentment and affection were studied
preferred to the old terms because they recognize that men or      rather than the standard higher arousal states of “energetic”
women can possess either set of characteristics. Although          and “feeling on top of the world” which are used. Supporting
men do not report higher SWB than women, men and women             this belief is the finding that emotional intensity declines with
who are agentic have higher SWB, and men and women who             age (Diener, Sandvik, & Larsen, 1985).
are low in agency have lower SWB.                                    The lack of significant decrease in life satisfaction across
   Another interesting finding within the sex and SWB              the life span suggests people’s ability to adapt to their
literature is that women report more negative affect and           conditions. Declines in income and marriage occur across
depression than men and are more likely to seek therapy for        age cohorts in later adulthood, yet, life satisfaction is stable.
this disorder; yet, men and women report approximately the         Some researchers have suggested that these findings serve as
same levels of global happiness. One explanation is that           evidence that people readjust their goals as they age
women more readily admit to negative feelings whereas men          (Campbell et al., 1976; Rapkin & Fischer, 1992). Continuing
deny such feelings. Thus, it is possible that both sexes           with this line of thinking, Ryff (1991) found that older adults,
experience similar levels of negative affect and depression,       compared with younger people, demonstrate a closer fit
but women report these feelings and seek professional help         between ideal and actual self-perceptions. Brandtstadter and
more often. Another explanation for the paradoxical sex            Renner (1990) believe that overcoming adversities is
differences in SWB is offered by Fujita, Diener, and Sandvik       performed either by changing life circumstances to personal
(1991). They suggest that in the social role of nurturer,          preferences (assimilative coping) or by adjusting personal
women are socialized to be more open to emotional                  preferences and goals to given situational constraints
experiences, including both positive and negative emotions,        (accommodative coping). Both types of coping strategies
in turn, they may experience more positive and more                were related to life satisfaction, but they found a gradual shift
negative affect. Their research did reveal that women report       from the assimilative to the accommodative style with
greater amounts of positive affect as well, thus, it may be that   increasing age. This supports Campbell et al.’s conclusion
women experience, on average, both positive and negative           that the gap between one’s goals and circumstances decreases
emotions more strongly and frequently than men. These              with age. Further, the lack of life satisfaction decline with
researchers also discovered that gender was responsible for        age suggests that the connection between life circumstances
less than 1% of the variance in happiness but over 13% of the      and SWB is mediated by expectations.
variance in the intensity of emotional experiences. Fujita et
al. hypothesize that women’s, on average, openness to
intense emotional experiences, creates a vulnerability to
depression given many bad events, but also creates
3   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

EDUCATION                                                         declining at least one-half a standard deviation in income
                                                                  over a 10-year period.
  Small but significant correlations between education and          Income change at the national level, as with the personal
SWB are indicated (Campbell et al., 1976; Cantril, 1965;          level, shows little effect upon SWB. Despite strong
Diener et al., 1993). In a meta-analysis of the literature,       economic growth in France, Japan, and the United States
Witter, Okun, Stock, and Haring (1984) observed a median          from 1946 to 1990, there was no increase in mean reports of
effect size of .13. This effect size was similar to education’s   SWB (Diener & Suh, 1997). Disposable income rose
influence upon life satisfaction (.15), morale (.15), and         dramatically (even controlling for inflation and taxes), but
quality of life (.12). Education correlates with well-being       well-being levels remained stable. Oswald (1997) studied
more for individuals with lower incomes (Campbell, 1981;          nine European nations during a time of rapid economic
Diener et al., 1993), and in poor countries (Veenhoven,           growth and also found virtually no increase in SWB.
1994a); possibly in the former case due to education’s              Contrarily, Gross National Product (GNP) per capita
creating wider leisure interests resulting in other sources of    correlated approximately .50 with life satisfaction across 39
happiness, and in the latter case due to the social status        nations (Diener et al., 1993). These results were supported
conveyed by education. The effect of education on SWB has         by a sample of 55 nations using both GNP and purchasing
become weaker over time in the United States. Campbell            power as indicators of national financial status (Diener,
(1981) notes that in 1957, 44% of college graduates reported      Diener, and Diener, 1995). The relation between national
being very happy compared to 23% of those with no high            wealth and SWB may be due, in part, to other benefits
school, while in 1978, the corresponding percentages were 33      received by people of wealthier countries such as more
and 28 percent. Oishi, Diener, and Lucas (2007) found that        democratic and egalitarian treatment, hence, the direct effect
moderately happy people but not the happiest people attained      of wealth itself may not be the only determining factor.
the highest levels of educational achievement and income.           When examining the effect of materialistic goals
  Much of the relation between education and SWB results          themselves upon SWB, Richins & Dawson (1992) found that
from the correlation of education with occupational status        people who value money more highly than other goals are
and income (Campbell, 1981; Witter et al., 1984). Hence, in       less satisfied with their standard of living and with their lives,
America, education is only indirectly related to well-being.      and this association persists even when income is controlled
Surprisingly, education may even potentially interfere with       (Crawford, 1998). Several theories attempt to explain why
SWB if it leads to expectations which cannot be met, thus         materialism is a negative predictor of SWB. Materialistic
increasing the goal-achievement gap.                              pursuits may be counterproductive because they interfere
                                                                  with other prosocial and self-actualization goals (Scitovsky,
INCOME                                                            1976), and the extrinsic goal of money is not conducive to
                                                                  meeting inherent needs in economically advanced societies
  Overall, a small but significant correlation of .12 between     (Kasser, 1997).
income and SWB was found in a nationally representative             Thus, findings do not support a strong causal direction from
sample in the United States (Diener et al., 1993).                income to SWB. Wealthy people are only somewhat happier
  The effects of income on happiness are small even when          than poor people in rich nations, though, wealthy nations
examining extremely wealthy individuals. Individuals with a       appear much happier than poor nations, and changes in
net worth over $125 million were compared to randomly             income may sometimes have negative effect. Studying
selected controls from the same geographical areas. Results       expectations and material desires would be beneficial in
showed the very rich to be, on average, somewhat happier          explaining these patterns. For example, wealth affects SWB
than the mean of national samples, but there was                  more in countries where basic needs of food, shelter, and
considerable overlap in the distributions of the wealthy and      health care are of concern, but has a much lesser effect within
not wealthy groups (Diener, Horwitz, and Emmons, 1985).           countries where basic needs are met.
Generally, wealthier people are happier than poorer people,         Interpreting these results may lead public policy-makers as
however, the effects are small. Brickman et al. (1978)            well as individuals to question the worth of pursuing ever
determined that lottery winners were happier than controls        greater amounts of money. Many experience increasing time
but not significantly so. Smith and Razzell (1975) studied        shortage yet choose to work even longer hours. The sacrifice
individuals who won large amounts of money in the British         of important values such as self-growth, leisure time, and
football pools and found 39% described themselves to be           significant relationships to attain more wealth may interfere
very happy compared to 19% of the controls. Unexpectedly,         with happiness rather than enhance it. In question is not the
some negative affect also surfaced as the winners quit their      pursuit of wealth and economic growth by nations and
jobs and then experienced lost relationships and decreased        individuals, rather, how increases in wealth may be used and
feelings of accomplishment, as well as some tension from          at what costs.
family and friends who expected financial assistance. Diener
et al. (1993) found no differences in hedonic level (pleasant
versus unpleasant affect) between groups advancing or

4   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

MARRIAGE                                                           level of stimulation that people find pleasurable
                                                                   (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Scitovsky, 1976), positive social
  The positive relation between marriage and SWB has been          relationships, and a sense of identity and meaning.
consistently reported in the United States (Glenn, 1975; Gove        Characteristics of satisfying jobs have been extensively
& Shin, 1989), Canada (White, 1992), Norway (Mastekaasa,           researched; for example, person-organization fit generally
1995) and in international studies (Diener, Gohm, Suh, &           correlates with job satisfaction (Bretz & Judge, 1994), and
Oishi, 1998). These large-scale surveys indicate that married      intrinsic rewards and social benefits are significant predictors
people report greater happiness compared to those who were         of satisfaction (Mottaz, 1985).
never married or are divorced, separated, or widowed. Those          People satisfied with their lives generally find more
who cohabit with a partner and are not married are                 satisfaction in their work (Sones & Kozma, 1986) revealing
significantly happier in some cultures than those living alone     the relation between job and life satisfaction may reflect
(Kurdek, 1991; Mastekaasa, 1995). Marriage and well-being          internal personality factors.
correlate significantly even when age and income are                 Unemployed people have higher distress, lower life
controlled (Glenn & Weaver, 1979; Gove, Hughes, & Style,           satisfaction, and higher rates of suicide than employed
1983). A meta-analysis by Haring-Hidore, Stock, Okun, and          individuals (Oswald, 1997; Platt & Kreitman, 1985).
Witter (1985) determined an average correlation of .14             Unemployment typically causes lower SWB (Clark, 1998).
between marital status and SWB.
  Lee, Seccombe, & Shehan (l991) studied the differences           HEALTH
between married and nonmarried men and women from 1972
to 1989 and found that married women were consistently               Campbell et al. (1976) found respondents to judge “good
happier than unmarried women, and married men were                 health” as the most important of various life domains.
consistently happier than unmarried men. Diener et al.             Pressman and Cohen (2005) illustrated many benefits of
(1998) discovered that marriage offers greater benefits for        positive attitude upon health, such as fewer symptoms, less
men than for women in terms of positive emotions, but              pain, and better pulmonary function. Strong correlations
married men and women do not differ in life satisfaction.          between health and SWB exist for self-reported health
  Many researchers believe that marriage serves as a buffer        measures (George & Landerman, 1984), but not for objective
against the hardships of life and it provides emotional and        health ratings by physicians (Watten, Vassend, Myhrer, &
economic support which produces positive states of                 Syversen, 1997). Therefore, perceptions of health appear to
wellbeing (Coombs, 1991; Gove, Style, & Hughes, 1990;              be more important than objective health in their effects on
Kessler & Essex, 1982). Marital benefits may be limited,           SWB. Explanation for this unexpected result is that people in
however, given the finding by Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, and        poor health downplay the importance of their health when
Diener (2003) that marriage did not lead to lasting changes in     evaluating their global life satisfaction, and secondly, people
happiness.                                                         use cognitive coping strategies that promote a positive image
  In a longitudinal study, Headey, Veenhoven, and Wearing          of their health condition.
(1991) found that among the six life domains they studied            Individuals with a severe disabling condition or multiple or
(e.g., job, health), only marital satisfaction had a significant   chronic problems, however, may report low SWB, whereas if
causal influence on global life satisfaction.                      the condition is less severe, adaptation is possible (Mehnert
  Happy people may have a better chance of getting married,        et al., 1990). Large-scale longitudinal studies show that
in turn, upon committing to the marriage, the benefits of          many people do not adapt to severe life-circumstance
companionship can further raise SWB. Thus, both selection          changes such as becoming disabled (Lucas, 2007), and they
effects and the benefits of marriage may underlie the relation     may not regain their pre-incidence happiness level, which
between marriage and SWB (Mastekaasa, 1995). Marital               suggests that the concept of a “set-point” of happiness is not
quality is obviously also a predictor of life satisfaction.        fixed and static (Diener, Lucas, & Scollon, 2006).
Variables including the structure of interactions (Gottman &         Poor health is thought to negatively influence SWB
Levenson, 1986), emotional expressiveness (King, 1993),            because it interferes with goal-attainment.
and role sharing (Hendrix, 1997) are related to more
satisfying marriages.                                              RELIGION

JOB SATISFACTION                                                     Marx described religion at the “opiate of the masses,”
                                                                   proposing that it contributes to well-being. Many large
  Tait, Padgett, and Baldwin (1989) performed a meta-              studies, often based on national samples, do show that SWB
analysis of 34 studies and determined an average correlation       correlates significantly (although the effect sizes are not
of .44 between job satisfaction and life satisfaction. They        large) with religious certainty (Ellison, 1991), strength of
also discovered a stronger relation between job and life           one’s relationship with the Divine (Pollner, 1989), prayer
satisfaction for women in recent decades as their societal         experiences (Poloma & Pendleton, 1991), and devotional and
roles have changed and career-choice has expanded. Work is         participatory aspects of religiosity (Ellison, Gay, & glass,
thought to be related to SWB because it offers an optimal
5   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

1989), even after controlling for age, income and marital          through such activities, including intimacy, cooperation and
status.                                                            so forth (Argyle, 1996). Csikszentmihalyi and
  Religious experiences may offer a sense of meaning in            Csikszentmihalyi (1988) suggest that the greatest satisfaction
daily life (Pollner, 1989) and during major life crises            is derived from activities in which challenges are met by
(McIntosh, Silver, & Wortman, 1993). Additionally, religion        skills, producing a state of “flow.” Hence, leisure may be
offers social fulfillment through exposure to social networks      more satisfying when there is some commitment, some use of
composed of people who share similar attitudes and values          skill, and some kind of achievement.
(Taylor & Chatters, 1988). The benefits of church                    The most popular form of leisure in the modern world,
membership are greater for those who have lost other forms         surprisingly, is watching television. Heavy television
of social support, such as retirees and widows, suggesting         watchers are less happy than others. This activity, however,
increased social support may underlie a portion of the             does provide positive and relaxing pleasure at a very low
advantages of religion.                                            level of arousal - at a point between being awake and asleep
  Ellison (1991) concluded that religious variables account        (Kubey and Csikszentmihalyi, 1990).
for approximately 5%-7% of life satisfaction variance, but           Holidays are a source of happiness and relaxation.
only 2%-3% of the variance in affective well-being. Ellison        Rubenstein (1980) surveyed a large number of “Psychology
suggests that religion’s benefits are mainly cognitive,            Today” readers and found that 21% had headaches when not
offering an interpretive framework to make sense of life           on holiday compared to only 3% while on vacation; similar
experience, rather than eliminating negative events or             differences were found for being tired, irritable, or
increasing positive events and therefore may not affect            constipated.
emotional well-being.
  Strawbridge, Shema, Cohen, Roberts, and Kaplan (1998)            LIFE EVENTS
reported evidence that religiosity buffers the effects of some
stressors on depression, but exacerbates the effects of other        The frequency of daily positive events correlates with
stressors, for example, marital problems and abuse. This           positive affect at .33 (Kanner et al., 1981). The percentage of
implies that the effects of religiosity are not inevitably         time when subjects were experiencing positive affect
positive, suggesting a need for more research in this area.        correlated with happiness at approximately .50, and average
                                                                   intensity of positive affect when subjects were feeling happy
LEISURE                                                            correlated with happiness at about .25 (Diener, Sandvik, and
                                                                   Pavot, 1991). These researchers suggest that intense positive
  In surveys reviewed by Veenhoven and colleagues (1994),          experiences do not have much effect on happiness, partly due
happiness was correlated with leisure satisfaction and level of    to their rare occurrence - they occurred in only 2.6 percent of
leisure activities at .40, but after controlling for various       days in one study.
demographic variables including employment and social                The main causes of joy in a study of five European
class, the correlation fell to .20. The correlation was greatest   countries were: relationships with friends; the basic pleasures
for those not working such as the unemployed, retired, or old,     of food, drink, and sex; and success experiences (Scherer et
those of greater wealth or social class, and the married           al., 1986).
without children at home (Zuma, 1989).                               People experience a more positive mood when with friends
  Causal effects of leisure on happiness are offered by            compared to being with family or alone (Larson, 1990), and
longitudinal studies. Glancy, Willits, and Farrell (1986)          happiness correlates with number of friends, frequency of
studied 1521 high school students for 24 years and                 seeing them, going to parties and dances, and belonging to
determined that adolescent leisure predicted adult life            teams and clubs (Argyle and Lu, 1990). Headey, Holmstrom,
satisfaction, with a number of controls applied. Heady,            and Wearing (1985) discovered that favorable events within
Holmstrom, and Wearing (1985) followed up 600 Australians          the realms of friendship and work predicted increases in
at two-year intervals and found that leisure satisfaction          SWB over two-year intervals.
increased subjective well-being.                                     Frequency of sexual intercourse correlates with happiness,
  Stronger causal evidence is provided by the effects of sport     as does satisfaction with sex life, being in love, and
and other forms of leisure. Exercise, such as aerobics for 8 to    frequency of interaction with spouse, however, having liberal
10 weeks, two to four times a week, has increased happiness        sexual attitudes has a negative relationship.
and reduced clinical depression and anxiety (Biddle and              The great outdoors, including the sea, sun, mountains, and
Mutrie, 1991). Additionally, periods of exercise reduce the        wild, natural settings also produces positive effects.
negative effects on heart rate and blood pressure resulting          Rated highest is falling in love and the effects are positive
from performing stressful tasks (Kimbell and Basford, 1996).       as long as the love lasts. Oishi, Diener, and Lucas (2007)
Sport and exercise are effective partly due to the release of      discovered that the happiest people experienced the greatest
endorphins, the social interaction with others, and the            success within the domain of romantic relationships.
experience of success or self-efficacy. Dance and music are        Parenthetically, Lyubomirsky, King, and Diener (2005)
effective given their mood-inducing nature and social              found a correlation between happiness assessed at one point
benefits. It is possible to fulfill a number of social needs       in life with positive outcomes at a later time; specifically,
6   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

happy people were more likely to be experiencing a stable         TRAITS ASSOCIATED WITH SWB
romantic relationship than less happy people ten years later.
  Having a baby is rated highly but also judged to be stressful      Traits are behavioral response tendencies which show a
for some time. As noted earlier, winning a lottery offers a       degree of consistency across situations and stability over
temporary increase in happiness but level of happiness            time. DeNeve and Cooper (1998) identified 137 personality
returns to normal after a period of time. These events have       traits correlated with SWB constructs, hence, recognizing the
complex effects upon SWB because they promote major               most important traits needed for subjective well-being is
change in one’s life which may be positive yet disrupting at      difficult. In recent years, the Five-Factor Model or “Big
the same time. Religious experiences, as noted earlier, can       Five” has received the most theoretical attention, research,
have longlasting positive effects. Peak experiences have          and popularity. Five cardinal traits compose the Five-Factor
been known to occur through religion, music, or other intense     Model, including extroversion, neuroticism,
activities.                                                       conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness to
                                                                  experience. It is believed that other personality
COMPETENCIES                                                      characteristics (included in this section) are derived from
                                                                  these and that many other traits may be mapped onto the Big
  Studies have found very small, but positive correlations        Five. The low pole of each BigFive trait is represented by its
between the competency of intelligence and happiness.             corresponding antonym: introversion, emotional stability,
Similar to education, intelligence may raise aspirations which    spontaneity, assertiveness, and low openness to experience.
might not be met, thereby, increasing the goal-achievement
gap.                                                              EXTROVERSION AND NEUROTICISM
  Physical attractiveness has a stronger effect, especially for
young women. It generates greater happiness (Agnew, 1984)           The traits having received the most empirical attention in
possibly because it produces popularity with the opposite sex,    relation to SWB are extroversion and neuroticism.
teachers and employers, and it is a source of upward mobility     Extroversion includes characteristics such as sociability,
(Argyle, 1994). Height in men has similar effects and has a       stimulus seeking, dominance, high activity, warmth,
small correlation with happiness.                                 dominance, and correlates significantly with pleasant
  Social-skills are much more important than the preceding        emotions, for example, joy and affection. Neuroticism
competencies. Argyle and Lu (1990) found that extroverts          includes characteristics such as anxiety, pessimism,
have more social-skills and this is one reason for their          irritability, bodily complaints and interpersonal sensitivity; in
happiness; also, they are happy due to their greater              other words, it encompasses a wide range of unpleasant
assertiveness skills, which mediated the extroversion-            thoughts and emotions.
happiness relation. Social-skills lead to happiness because         Fujita (1991) determined the correlation between
they improve the probability of attaining desired relationships   extroversion and pleasant affect to be .80 and that
with others. Contrarily, those who are socially incompetent       neuroticism and the experience of negative affect were
are likely to be socially rejected, and become isolated and       virtually indistinguishable. Many studies have replicated the
unable to find social support or companionship (Sarason and       relationship between these constructs promoting Watson and
Sarason, 1985).                                                   Clark (1984) to relabel the trait of neuroticism as negative
  Studies have also found that happiness is related to            affectivity and they suggested that positive affectivity forms
cooperativeness, leadership, and heterosexual skills (Argyle,     the core of the broad trait of extroversion (Watson & Clark,
Martin and Lu, 1995).                                             1997).
  Fordyce (1977, 1983) formulated an intervention program           Costa and McCrae (1980) discovered that extroversion
designed to assist individuals in acquiring the competencies      predicted pleasant affect and neuroticism predicted
of happy people, such as being organized, staying busy,           unpleasant affect over a ten-year period. Magnus and Diener
increasing socialization, creating and maintaining a positive     (1991) showed that extroversion and neuroticism scores
outlook, and working on developing a healthy personality.         predicted life satisfaction over four years; personality was a
The program increased levels of happiness as compared to a        stronger predictor of life satisfaction than life events.
placebo control and to groups only receiving partial                Diener, Sandvik, Pavot, and Fujita (1992) determined that
information, and the positive intervention effects were extant    extroversion predicted SWB in a representative sample of
in follow-up evaluations 9-28 months after the study.             Americans. Extroverts were on average happier despite
Similarly, kindness interventions (Otake, Shimai,                 living alone or with others, working in solitary or social
TanakaMarsumi, Otsui, & Fredrickson, 2006), and gratitude         occupations, or living in small or large cities. They were also
interventions (Emmons & McCullough, 2003) have shown              happier across diverse ethnic, gender, and age groups. The
promising results as a means of increasing SWB.                   close relation between extroversion and SWB across different
                                                                  environments and samples suggested the bond may be due to
                                                                  biological temperament rather than situational differences
                                                                  between extroverts and introverts. A temperament factor was
                                                                  suggested again when Pavot, Diener, and Fujita (1990) found
7   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

that extroverts reported more positive affect at random            on neural approach-and-avoidance systems and are
moments in an experience sampling study regardless of being        automatically associated with more positive or negative
alone or in a social setting.                                      affect, respectively, the relation of conscientiousness and
  Watson and Clark (1997) suggest extroverts and neurotics         agreeableness to subjective well-being is thought to depend
have a temperamental susceptibility to experience positive         on whether individuals with these traits are rewarded within
and negative affect, respectively. They base their belief on       particular environments. These variables have not been
Gray’s (1991) theory of personality. Gray stated that two          extensively studied and warrant additional analysis.
brain systems are responsible for much of the individual
differences in personality. The behavioral activation system       OPENNESS TO EXPERIENCE
(BAS) is sensitive to cues of reward and nonpunishment and
controls approach behavior. The behavioral inhibition                The fifth trait of the Big Five, openness to experience, is
system (BIS) is sensitive to cues of punishment and                generally not related to an individual’s experience of positive
nonreward, corresponds to the dimension of anxiety and is          or negative affect or life satisfaction. For example,
responsible for inhibiting behavior during the threat of           Gottfredson (1994) determined that openness to experience
punishment. Individuals sensitive to signals of reward (high       did not correlate with job satisfaction. This variable is
BAS) are extroverted and those sensitive to signals of             thought to relate to emotional intensity by influencing how
punishment (high BIS) are introverted. Gray believes that          happy and unhappy moments are experienced rather than
the BAS, a system reactive to reward stimuli, is responsible       influencing how happy or unhappy a person is.
for the production of pleasant affect, while the BIS, a system
reactive to punishment stimuli, is responsible for the             SELF-ESTEEM
production of unpleasant affect.
  Based on Gray’s (1991) theories, Lucas et al. (1998)               Many clinicians believe that self-acceptance is mandatory
suggested that extroverts are more sensitive to rewards and        for psychological well-being. Supportively, Lucas et al.
this sensitivity results in greater pleasant affect when exposed   (1996) found self-esteem to strongly predict SWB in Western
to rewarding stimuli. Greater positive affect then leads           cultures. Contrarily, self-esteem is not a universal
individuals to approach rewarding stimuli such as social           determinant of SWB. Diener and Diener (1995) determined
situations in which extroverts’ elevated positive affect and       that self-esteem is a weak correlate (0.8) for women in
sensitivity to rewards leads to increased social behavior.         collectivist societies where family is valued over the
Hence, these researchers believe that extroversion may result      individual. Likewise, Kwan, Bond, and Singelis (1997)
from individual differences in pleasant affect.                    revealed a strong relation between self-esteem and life
  The relationship between extroversion and positive affect is     satisfaction in America but also discovered that relationship
strongly supported, but the reasons are not clear. One             harmony was also a strong predictor of life satisfaction in
hypothesis is based on temperament in which extroverts are         Hong Kong. Hence, self-esteem may not be a strong
biologically predisposed to react more strongly to rewards.        predictor of SWB in cultures which value the group above
A second view states that extroverts spend more time in            the individual but is significant in Western societies.
social situations which tend to produce positive affect. A
third view is that extroverts may experience more positive         OPTIMISM
events due to an ability to elicit rewarding outcomes for
themselves; in fact, Headey and Wearing (1992) discovered            Scheier and Carver (1985) developed a theory of
that extroverts experienced more positive events over a            dispositional optimism in which optimism is a generalized
period of years.                                                   tendency to expect favorable outcomes in one’s life.
  Neurotics generally experience more unpleasant life events.      Specifically, those who believe that positive outcomes will
Headey and Wearing (1992) found that highly neurotic               follow working toward a goal are more likely to achieve their
individuals recurrently experience more negative life events       goal, whereas, those who expect failure are more likely to
than less neurotic people. One explanation is that negative        disengage from their goal. This pattern leads optimists to
thoughts and emotions yield self-defeating behaviors.              achieve their goals more often than pessimists. Lucas,
  Schimmack, Oishi, Furr, & Funder (2004) concluded that           Diener, and Suh (1996) revealed that optimism correlates
cheerfulness, a characteristic of extroversion, and depression,    with measures of SWB such as life satisfaction, pleasant
a facet of neuroticism, explain individual differences in life     affect and unpleasant affect. Scheier and Carver (1993)
satisfaction more than the global traits of extroversion and       reviewed findings showing that optimists maintain higher
neuroticism as a whole.                                            levels of SWB when facing a stressor. Evidence showing the
                                                                   underlying mechanisms of optimism is offered by Scheier,
CONSCIENTIOUSNESS AND AGREEABLENESS                                Weintraub, and Carver (1986) who found that optimists tend
                                                                   to use problem-focused coping, seek social support, and
  Both these variables correlate moderately with SWB but at        emphasize positive aspects of the situation upon encountering
lower levels than extroversion and neuroticism. Whereas            difficulties. Pessimists tend to use denial, focus on stressful
research suggests that extroversion and neuroticism are based      feelings, and disengage from relevant goals. It appears that
8   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

those who think positively use more effective forms of            .50 range between the spouse’s ratings of the target person’s
coping.                                                           emotions at time 1 and the target person’s self-rating six
                                                                  years later.
SELF-EFFICACY                                                        An alternative explanation that SWB stability is due to
                                                                  constancy in people’s external conditions is not supported by
   The feeling that an individual possesses what is needed to     research of changing conditions across the life span. Costa,
accomplish important goals, termed self-efficacy, predicts        McCrae, and Zonderman (1987) studied people living under
life satisfaction and positive emotions as shown by Feasel        relatively stable circumstances versus those living in
(1995). Additionally, this researcher observed that feeling       changing conditions such as divorce or becoming widowed.
high selfefficacy toward more important goals was a stronger      The highchange group demonstrated only slightly lower
predictor of well-being than such a feeling toward less           stability estimates than the low-change group. Diener et al.
important goals. This demonstrates that feeling efficacious is    (1993) discovered that people revealed approximately the
important to well-being but also highlights the need to feel      same level of SWB over a 10 year period regardless of their
competent in areas of life that one feels are significant.        income increasing, decreasing or staying the same.
                                                                     Personality has shown a more significant role than specific
POSITIVE ILLUSIONS                                                life events in determining SWB. Magnus and Diener (1991)
                                                                  found that over a four-year period, time 1 personality
   Taylor and Brown (1988) noted that many people possess         predicted time 2 life satisfaction and other measures of
positive illusions which include unrealistically positive         subjective well-being beyond the effect of intervening life
selfperceptions, overly optimistic views of the future, and       events. Further, highly valued resources such as wealth
overestimates of environmental control. Surprisingly, these       (Diener et al., 1993), physical attractiveness (Diener, Wolsic,
illusions foster SWB as well as caring for others and the         and Fujita, 1995), and objective health (Okun and George,
ability to engage in productive work. Taylor and Armor            1984) barely correlated with SWB.
(1996) observed that positive illusions correlate with               These studies reveal that SWB, like personality,
successful adjustment to stressful circumstances, including       demonstrates some stability over time. Hence, the amount of
extreme adversity. Erez, Johnson, and Judge (1995) found          pleasant affect, unpleasant affect and life satisfaction one
that individuals with a positive disposition tended to use        experiences during college years is likely to remain
more self-deception, which in turn increased their SWB.           moderately stable unrelated to future events such as marriage,
Similarly, Lightsey (1994) indicated that negative events         divorce, finding a job or becoming unemployed. Diener and
have a weaker relation to depression among people with            Larsen (1984) report that average levels of pleasant mood in
automatic positive cognitions. Finally, Seidlitz and Diener       work situations correlated .70 with average levels of pleasant
(1993) discovered that happy people can remember more             mood in recreation situations. Average levels of unpleasant
good events; this results from their manner of initially          affect in work situations correlated .74 with mean levels of
encoding more of their life-events in a positive way.             unpleasant affect in recreation situations. Similar
                                                                  consistency levels for pleasant and unpleasant affect were
PERSONALITY AND SWB                                               noted across social versus alone situations, and novel versus
                                                                  typical situations. Mean levels of life satisfaction were even
   Personality is defined as characteristic response tendencies   more consistent with stability coefficients in the .95 range.
in adults composed of biological and learned components;          Thus, people display consistent characteristic emotional
extensive research supports its significant influence on SWB.     responses to their environments even when the environment
   Stability across time in SWB should predictably exist if       changes.
personality is a major influence upon this concept. Costa            Happiness remains moderately stable despite changing life
(1994) observed that over a period of up to 30 years, adults      conditions. Moreover, evaluations of specific events and life
are stable in their personalities. Such support is noted by       domains in one’s life are affected by one’s overall level of
Magnus and Diener (1991) who determined that life                 happiness. Kozma (1996) noted that life domain (work,
satisfaction correlated .58 with the same measure                 home, relationships) satisfaction judgments were correlated
administered four years later. The correlation was .52 when       but upon overall happiness being controlled, correlations
life satisfaction was self-reported at time 1 and four years      between the different domains were no longer significant.
later reported by family and friends of the respondent.           Concluded is that overall happiness influences feelings about
Utilizing two measurement sources - self versus informant         specific domains and contentment with specific domains is a
report - demonstrates the stability of SWB is not due to social   function of specific factors within the domain and one’s
desirability or acquiescence.                                     general level of SWB. The extent to which one’s needs are
   The affective components of SWB (pleasant and unpleasant       met is positively correlated with the level of life satisfaction
affect) also yield stability over time. Watson and Walker         (Sheldon, Elliot, Kim, & Kasser, 2001).
(1996) determined that trait affect scales showed a moderate         The stable individual difference of how people think about
level of time stability over a six-year period. Costa and         and perceive the world affects SWB. Different levels of
McCrae (1988) found significant stability coefficients in the     accessing pleasant versus unpleasant information, and how
9   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

accurate and efficient one can process pleasant versus               Studies of temperament in early life indicate the influence
unpleasant information influences SWB (Robinson &                  of inherited biology upon SWB. Goldsmith and Campos
Kirkeby, 2005; Robinson, Vargas, Tamir, & Solberg, 2004).          (1986) report that biologically based emotional reactions
Happy individuals attend to and remember the pleasant              develop early in life, are somewhat stable over time, and
events and nuances of life to a greater degree than others         offer the building blocks for adult personality dimensions.
(Tamir & Robinson, 2007). Furthermore, happy people often            Kagan (1994) described two groups of infants that had
utilize broad, abstract criteria to judge their own lives while    either an avoidant style to unfamiliar events (inhibited
unhappy people frequently use concrete criteria (Updegraff &       children) or an approach-oriented style (uninhibited
Suh, 2007). These findings infer that happiness is not simply      children). These different emotional styles are thought to be
a product of who we are, but of how we think about and             biologically based resulting from differential excitability of
perceive our lives as well.                                        the amygdala and its projections to the motor system, the
                                                                   cingulate and frontal cortex, the hypothalamus, and the
TEMPERAMENT EFFECTS ON SWB                                         sympathetic nervous system (Kagan, Snidman, & Arcus,
                                                                   1992). LaGasse, Gruber, and Lipsitt (1989) were able to
   Temperament is defined as a biological predisposition to        predict inhibited behavior by observing sucking in two-day
certain types of responding that appears early in life and has a   old newborns; these results suggest biologically rather than
large genetic component. Temperament is believed to exert a        environmentallybased differences. The effects of this
causal role in predisposing some to experience high SWB            inhibited style lasted at least eight years into childhood.
and others low SWB. Supporting evidence for this                   Kagan and Moss (1962) found that shyness (a trait of
temperamental predisposition to experience various levels of       inhibited children) shown in the first three years of life
SWB is offered by behavior-genetic studies of heritability.        persisted into adulthood. The researchers observed,
Heritability research estimates the effect of genetic makeup       contrarily, that not all inhibited children remained as such
upon SWB.                                                          through childhood, suggesting environment may influence
   Tellegen et al. (1988) examined monozygotic (identical)         emotional development beyond early temperament.
and dizygotic (fraternal) twins who were reared together and         The biological basis of SWB is further illustrated by
others who were reared apart. Comparing monozygotic twins          Davidson and Fox (1982) who demonstrated that fearful
who share all of their genes with dizygotic twins who share        behavior shown by infants in a laboratory situation correlates
on average half of their genes illustrates the effect of genes     with the relative amounts of left frontal versus right frontal
on personality. Comparing such sets of twins reared apart          cerebral activity displayed before testing began. Frontal
versus those reared together shows the effect of                   cerebral asymmetry appears to indicate current emotional
environmental differences upon personality. Tellegen               states and a predisposition to experience those states.
discovered that even when monozygotic twins grew up in             Implied is that genetic factors may lead to differences in the
different homes they were extremely similar in SWB and             reactivity of the emotional centers of the brain; these
dizygotic twins raised in the same home were on average far        differences predispose people to experience greater or lesser
less similar. Twins raised in the same family were not much        amounts of pleasant and unpleasant mood and emotion. In
more similar compared to twins reared apart. The study             fact, these differences may represent basic physiological
estimated that genetics account for about 40% of the variance      predispositions to experience high or low levels of SWB.
in positive emotionality, 55% of the variance in negative            Related to this research, Heady and Wearing (1989)
emotionality, and 48% of the variance in well-being. Shared        discovered that people ultimately return to a baseline of
family environment accounts for 22% of the variance in             positive or negative affect after the occurrence of good or bad
positive emotionality and 2% in negative emotionality.             events. They offered a “dynamic equilibrium” theory which
Surprising is the finding that shared family environment has       states that personality determines baseline levels of emotional
such a small influence.                                            response; specific life experiences will move people above or
   Lykken and Tellegen (1996) calculated that 80% of               below this baseline, but, in time, they will return to their
longterm SWB is heritable. Hence, heritability has a               stable set point.
moderate influence on happiness during a specific period in          Valliant and Valliant (1990) showed the importance of
life and a substantial effect over the long term (this study       temperament versus environment in a longitudinal study of
looked at the portion of affect that was stable over a 10 year     Harvard graduates over a period of 45 years. Over time,
period).                                                           early childhood traumas such as a parent’s death and the
   Caution is advised, however, in identifying a single, precise   stability of the parental marriage had less effect on the well-
heritability coefficient for personality. The coefficient is       being of respondents. By late midlife, effects of these early
influenced by a number of factors, including the sample and        traumas were gone. Combined with heritability studies
measures used and the effects of environment. Still, several       which reveal a small influence of early shared home
accepted studies examining the effects of genes on                 environment on personality, these results show that
experiencing positive and negative affect have consistently        childhood events may not significantly affect later SWB.
found significant genetic effects between .30 and .60                Research has revealed the influence of temperament on
(Tellegen et al., 1988; Baker, Cesa, Gatz, & Mellins, 1992).       SWB. Infants demonstrate typical emotional reactions that
10   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING

may persist over time. Twin studies indicate that subjective      found that quadriplegics and paraplegics adapted to their
well-being is in part inherited and fairly stable over time.      spinal cord injury in short time frames.
The strong stability of adult personality and the relative          The concept of habituation underlies the idea of one’s
stability of SWB along with their inter-relatedness, suggests     affect returning to baseline, however, research shows that
that personality may continually influence feelings of well-      people do not habituate to all events. For example, Mehnert,
being. This notion has support from longitudinal studies          Krauss, Nadler, and Boyd (1990) found that a representative
showing personality can predict SWB over a period of years.       sample of people with disabilities was less satisfied with life
Biological indicators, such as cerebral asymmetry, can            than a representative sample of the nondisabled. Further,
predict people’s typical emotional reactions. Environmental       respondents in very poor countries, such as India and Nigeria,
factors often only exhibit minor influence upon long-term         reported much lower SWB than people in highly
SWB. All told, people’s biologically based temperament is         industrialized nations, such as northern Europe (Diener,
significant in determining their level of SWB and                 Diener, and Diener, 1995). Therefore, personality provides a
environment may be of less significance than previously           long-term baseline effect on SWB, but, some life conditions
thought.                                                          also influence the baseline.

THEORIES EXPLAINING PERSONALITY’S                                 EMOTION SOCIALIZATION MODELS
INFLUENCE ON SWB
                                                                    This model attempts to explain the relation between
  Identifying those personality traits which are related to       personality and SWB by emphasizing that classical
SWB, their direction of causality, and the processes that         conditioning, instrumental learning, and imitation, condition
underlie these relations is considered important in attaining a   people from birth as to which emotions are acceptable to feel
complete scientific understanding of the concept of               and express. These socialization processes lead to
happiness. This section explores established theoretical          differences in affect among individuals.
models which explain the relationship between personality           Mothers teach infants, through socialization, “how to
and SWB.                                                          modulate their feeling states and expressive behavior so as to
                                                                  conform to cultural norms” (Malatesta et al., 1986). These
RETURN TO BASELINE MODEL                                          researchers found that from age two and a half months to age
                                                                  seven and a half months, infants revealed a linear increase in
  The dynamic equilibrium model of Heady and Wearing              pleasant affect and a decrease in unpleasant affect. There
(briefly discussed earlier) offers explanation for the            were corresponding increases in maternal contingent
connection between personality and SWB. They believe that         responding to infant pleasant affect expressions and
a person’s baseline level of happiness is determined by           decreases in maternal contingent responding to infant
temperament; specifically, extroversion and neuroticism           unpleasant affect expressions. Evidence for infant learning
determine one’s baseline level of happiness through the           effects were found when the data was examined to establish
strength of his or her reward and punishment systems.             direction of causality. Thus, long-term differences in the
Though life circumstances temporarily move people away            amount of affect people express may be due to emotion
from their baseline, their reward and punishment systems will     socialization processes.
ultimately return them to their baseline level. For example,
winning the lottery will move a person above his or her           PERSON-ENVIRONMENT FIT
normal state for a period of time, but slowly the person will
adapt and move back to baseline as determined by                    The central idea behind this model’s attempt to explain the
temperament and the lottery money will not affect the             connection between personality and SWB is that a person
person’s happiness. Likewise, a bad event such as the loss of     will experience subjective well-being to the degree that he or
a loved one will move the person below baseline, but after        she finds the environment rewarding and that environments
time needed to adapt and not suffer from the event, return to     are most rewarding when the individual’s personality “fits”
prior baseline will occur. Due to biologically determined         the situation. Thus, individuals in life circumstances that fit
“set-points” of reactivity to stimuli, one’s reward and           their personalities should experience higher SWB than those
punishment system adapts to positive or negative stimuli,         in life situations not fitting their personality.
causing a return to one’s baseline.                                 Supportive to this model, Brandstatter (1994) found that
  Heady and Waring (1989) determined that baseline levels         extroverts are happier in high-stimulation situations whereas
of well-being were predicted by extroversion and                  this was not true for introverts. Diener, Sandvik, Pavot, and
neuroticism, even across a number of years, and that people       Fujita (1992) determined that extroverts less often live alone
did return to previous levels of positive and negative affect     and more frequently work in social occupations.
after good and bad events. Suh, Diener, and Fujita (1996)           Nonetheless, the person-environment model has received
replicated this finding showing that people adapt to most life    only mixed empirical support (Diener, Larsen, & Emmons,
events in a surprisingly short time period. Silver (1980)         1984; Furnham, Toop, Lewis, & Fisher, 1995). Although
                                                                  people sometimes respond positively to an environment
11   Continuing Psychology Education Inc.
You can also read