Florida State University - A Classroom Demonstration of Self-Monitoring

Page created by Eva Campbell
 
CONTINUE READING
The Behavior Analyst                   1995, 18, 141-146                          No. 1 (Spring)

        A Classroom Demonstration of Self-Monitoring,
           Reactivity, and Interobserver Agreement
             James E. Carr, Steven L. Taylor, and John Austin
                         Florida State University
A brief classroom demonstration designed to introduce students to the concepts of self-monitoring,
reactivity, and interobserver agreement is presented. The demonstration provides students with op-
portunities to monitor their own behavior, record the behavior of others, and calculate interobserver
agreement percentages. Results of using the demonstration with students from two classes are pre-
sented.
   Key words: self-monitoring, reactivity, interobserver agreement, teaching behavior analysis

   It has been our experience that                intervention. As a method of assess-
hands-on techniques for teaching psy-             ment, self-monitoring provides the be-
chological concepts improve students'             havior analyst with a dependent mea-
ability to learn the material and also            sure on which to base treatment deci-
provide concrete examples of their ap-            sions or evaluate treatment effective-
plication. The purpose of this demon-             ness (Thoresen & Mahoney, 1974).
stration is to provide students with an           Because self-monitoring often results
opportunity to learn about self-moni-             in desired behavior change, it can also
toring, reactivity, and interobserver             serve as a form of treatment (e.g., to
agreement in a hands-on manner. A                 help students pay attention in class,
brief review of these concepts is fol-            Kneedler & Hallahan, 1981; to de-
lowed by a detailed description of the            crease aggression, Lovitt, 1973; to stop
classroom demonstration.                          nail biting, Maletzky, 1974). It is im-
   Self-monitoring (sometimes called              portant for the behavior analyst to un-
self-recording or self-observation) re-           derstand the complexities and benefits
fers "to an individual's systematically           of self-monitoring in order to ensure
observing his own behavior and re-                maximum behavior change.
sponding to the occurrence or nonoc-                 Reactivity is the "process by which
currence of a specified target re-                clients' behaviors change because the
sponse" (Cooper, Heron, & Heward,                 clients know they are being observed"
1987, p. 524). Self-monitoring has                (Spiegler & Guevremont, 1993, p.
been called "the lifeblood of effective           520). For example, a classroom ob-
self-control methods" (Thoresen &                 server might influence student behav-
Mahoney, 1974, p. 41) and has been                ior simply by being in the room with a
widely researched and utilized in the             recording apparatus. Reactivity is typ-
applied behavioral literature (e.g., Bro-         ically a negative side effect that may
den, Hall, & Mitts, 1971; Lipinski,               occur during behavioral observations.
Black, Nelson, & Ciminero, 1975;                  It is imperative that behavioral observ-
Maletzky, 1974; McFall, 1977). An in-             ers understand reactivity and take mea-
dividual can monitor his or her own
behavior as a means of assessment or              sures to counter, assess, or take advan-
                                                  tage of it. In general, the more obtru-
                                                  sive the observation procedure and
   We thank Jon Bailey, Crystal Carr, William     measurement    system, the more reactiv-
Heward, and Richard Malott for their helpful ity will occur (Kazdin, 1974). Observ-
comments on an earlier version of this manu- er obtrusiveness has been demonstrated
script.
   Reprints may be obtained from the first author to create reactivity such that data col-
at Department of Psychology, Florida State Uni- lected obtrusively often bear little or no
versity, Tallahassee, Florida 32306-1051.         relation to those collected unobtrusive-
                                                141
142                           JAMES E. CARR et al.

ly (Kazdin, 1979). The within-subject        ior of other students, and calculate in-
research design allows for the elimi-        terobserver agreement percentages.
nation of reactivity (if so desired by the
experimenter) in data-collection pro-             HOW TO CONDUCT THE
cedures as a plausible alternative hy-                DEMONSTRATION
pothesis to independent-variable ef-            Begin by explaining to your students
fects. This is achieved through the ac-      that they will be participating in a
quisition of a stable baseline measure       classroom demonstration in which they
of the behavior before intervening and       will encounter three important con-
maintenance of the measurement sys-          cepts. Although these are independent
tem through the treatment phase (Baer,       concepts, this one activity will provide
Wolf, & Risley, 1968).                       a good demonstration of each. Next,
   When reactivity is an uncontrolled        present a short lecture on the defini-
source of variation in the data, it is un-   tions, uses, and examples of self-mon-
wanted. When reactivity is produced          itoring, reactivity, and interobserver
under controlled conditions, however,        agreement.
it can also be a positive and useful phe-
nomenon to the behavior analyst. Self-       Self-Monitoring
monitoring was originally an attempt
to have the client simply collect data          Instruct students to count the fre-
regarding his or her own behavior.           quency of their own eye-blinks for 2
However, its reactive effect on behav-       min. Students can mark a simple tally
ior has led it to become a very popular      of the number of eye-blinks on a sheet
treatment because most behavior              of paper. (You may want to develop a
change as a result of self-monitoring is     recording sheet to hand out to the class
in the desired direction (Cooper et al.,     beforehand to record the self-monitor-
1987).                                       ing, reactivity, and interobserver agree-
   Interobserver agreement refers to         ment scores.) Use a stopwatch, time
the extent to which two or more inde-        the 2-min period, and cue students
                                             when to begin and stop self-monitor-
pendent observers agree on the occur-        ing. Do not attempt to develop an op-
rence of some dependent measure              erational definition of an eye-blink at
(Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1991). Baer         this time. If students should inquire
(1977) notes that rather than envision-      about the definition of an eye-blink,
ing some "true reliability" (p. 117)         tell them to use what they consider to
that has an existence somewhere in re-       be an eye-blink. After the 2 min are up,
ality but has to be estimated appropri-      have the students calculate the rate by
ately by scientists, applied behavior        dividing their total frequency by two.
analysts rely on the very practical no-         Inform the students that the average
tion of how often "two observers             person blinks between 20 and 30 times
watching one subject, and equipped           per minute (Schiffman, 1990), and if
with the same definitions of behavior,       their data fall within this range, it is
see it occurring or not occurring at the     possible that self-monitoring did not
same standard times" (p. 118). Inter-        change their behavior. Scores outside
observer agreement is one of the most        of this range, however, might have
important methodological procedures          been affected by the self-monitoring
in applied behavior analysis because in      procedure. Ask the students to raise
order for the data to be believable, they    their hands if they felt that the proce-
must be reliable (Johnston & Penny-          dure did, in fact, change their eye-blink
packer, 1992).                               frequency (i.e., if their scores were
   The following classroom demonstra-        above or below the average). Chances
tion provides each student with an op-       are you will have students who report
portunity to monitor his or her own be-      that they felt awkward monitoring their
havior, observe and record the behav-        own behavior, that they blinked their
CLASSROOM DEMONSTRATION                                   143

eyes much faster than normal, or they       two students recorded the third stu-
experienced an almost total suppres-        dent's eye-blinks as 40 and 50, the in-
sion of eye-blinking. This provides a       terobserver agreement would be 80%
chance to discuss the implications of       [(40 . 50)100%]. If a recording form
such changes in the target behavior for     has been provided to students, appro-
self-monitoring as an assessment meth-      priate visual prompts may be given on
od or as a treatment procedure.             the form to guide calculation.
                                               The purpose of interobserver agree-
Reactivity                                  ment when collecting behavioral data
   Ask the students to form groups of       is to answer the question, "Is that what
three, turning their desks so that each     I would have seen if I had been there?"
student is facing toward the other two      (Hawkins & Fabry, 1979, p. 545). Dis-
students in the group with approxi-        cuss the utility of interobserver agree-
mately equal spacing between the           ment measures in this exercise and in
desks. Have each group choose one          applied behavioral research. Compare
student whose eye-blinks will be           the students' interobserver agreement
counted by the other two. After the two    results with the conventional standard
students observe and tally the eye-        of 80% for the acceptance of agree-
blink frequency of the third for 2 min     ment percentages (Hersen & Barlow,
(using the same method of timing as         1976). Problems in getting acceptable
described for self-monitoring), have       agreement percentages may be due to
the students rotate twice so that each     observer drift, complexity of the ob-
student is observed by the other two.      servation system, and observer expec-
After all three students in each group     tancies and feedback (Kazdin, 1977).
have been observed, ask the students       These influences can be discussed in
to compare the scores recorded by their    relation to problems encountered by
peers to the previous self-monitoring      the students. The possibility that stu-
session, as well as to the approximate     dents who recorded the same frequen-
norm (20 to 30). Once again, ask stu-      cy may have counted different in-
dents to raise their hands if they ex-     stances of the target behavior should
perienced a behavior change in this        also be discussed.
reactivity session. Discuss the impli-         Students frequently report that prob-
cations of this change as they relate to   lems with interobserver agreement oc-
the implementation of obtrusive and        curred due to the ambiguous definition
unobtrusive recording procedures.          of an eye-blink, the blocking of some-
(Discussion of other variables that        one's vision, or the observer momen-
might account for changes in the fre-      tarily looking away. Most will also re-
quency of eye-blinks, such as fatigue      port a feeling of uneasiness in looking
or practice effects, may also be war-      at someone, or being looked at, for a
ranted.)                                   2-min interval. Further activities could
                                           include an individual or group assign-
Interobserver Agreement                    ment to develop a clear operational def-
                                           inition of an eye-blink and a descrip-
   In this phase of the demonstration,     tion of how to better arrange the ob-
have the students calculate the inter-     servation to minimize other problems
observer agreement percentages from        (e.g., using smaller observation inter-
the reactivity phase of data collection.   vals). Students can be asked to apply
Interobserver agreement for frequency      what they have learned about opera-
data is calculated by dividing the         tional definitions, self-monitoring reac-
smaller number by the larger number        tivity, and interobserver agreement to
and multiplying by 100% (for a dis-        a real-world situation, such as con-
cussion of other common methods of         ducting observations in an elementary
calculation, see Repp, Deitz, Boles,       classroom. This could be followed up
Deitz, & Repp, 1976). For example, if      with assignments outside of class in the
144                            JAMES E. CARR et al.

                                      TABLE 1
Frequency of student eye-blinks during each 2-min phase of the demonstra-
                                   tion
                                                                 Reactivity
                             Self-monitoring                                     Inter-
                                                                                observer
                                    Change from                  Change from   agreement
 Student     Baseline Frequency baseline (%) Frequency baseline (%)               (%)a
      1         43         78          +35 (81)         67          +24 (56)        96
     2          79         63          -16 (20)         54          -25 (32)       100
     3          20         24           +4 (20)         14           -6 (30)       100
     4          83         45          -38 (46)         13          -50 (60)        86
     5          35         20          -15 (43)         25          -10 (29)        92
     6          27         29           +2 (7)          25           -2 (7)         79
     7          28         27           -1 (4)          23           -5 (18)        97
     8          34         33           -1 (3)          29           -5(15)         73
     9          33         33            0 (0)          47          +14 (42)        59
    10          44         17          -27 (61)         14          -30 (68)        75
    11          67         16          -51 (76)         30          -37(55)         88
    12          20         17           -3 (15)         11           -9 (45)       100
    13          56         33          -23 (41)         30          -26(46)         94
    14          40         35           -5 (13)         30          -10 (25)        77
 aPercentage agreement between the observations made by two other students.

application of these procedures and re-      Self-Monitoring
porting of the results.
                                                In the self-monitoring phase, three
               RESULTS                       students experienced an increase in
                                             eye-blink frequency from baseline. The
   We have conducted this demonstra-         average increase was 36% (range, 7%
tion with two undergraduate classes:         to 81%). Ten students experienced a
Introduction to Applied Behavior             decrease in eye-blink frequency from
Analysis and Business Psychology.            baseline. The average decrease was
The students had previously been pre-        32% (range, 3% to 76%). One student
sented lectures and read textbook chap-      experienced no change from baseline.
ters on behavioral observation and           Overall, self-monitoring decreased the
measurement methodology. As an ad-           eye-blink frequencies of the students.
ditional component of the demonstra-         However, there were a few students
tion, we selected 14 students from the       whose eye-blink frequencies increased.
two classes and videotaped them dur-         This difference would make a good
ing a lecture. Both classes had been         class discussion of the role of reactivity
videotaped previously and the students       in self-monitoring.
were not told what the camera was for.
Later, the eye-blink frequencies of the      Reactivity
14 target students were recorded from
a 2-min videotape sample. These re-             In the reactivity phase, two students
sults are presented in Table 1 (base-        experienced an increase in eye-blink
line). The standard demonstration does       frequency from baseline. The average
not include this videotaped baseline         increase was 49% (range, 42% to
component, because it is a very labor-       56%). Twelve students experienced a
intensive extension. We thought, how-        decrease in eye-blink frequency from
ever, that it would be useful to report      baseline. The average decrease was
the actual student baselines instead of      36% (range, 7% to 68%). All students
using the approximate norm.                  experienced a behavior change due to
CLASSROOM DEMONSTRATION                                             145

reactivity. However, there were still         complete and should prove to be a use-
students who deviated from the nor-           ful adjunct to instruction on behavioral
mative behavior decrease. Once again,         observation and measurement meth-
this difference would make a good             odology.
class discussion of the role of reactivity
in promoting and assessing behavior                          REFERENCES
change.
                                              Baer, D. M. (1977). Reviewer's comment: Just
Interobserver Agreement                   because it's reliable doesn't mean that you can
                                          use it. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
   The average interobserver agree-       10, 117-119.
ment score was 87% (range, 59% to Baer,        D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R.
                                          (1968). Some current dimensions of applied
100%). This could be discussed in         behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behav-
class as a publishable agreement score,   ior Analysis, 1, 91-97.
although 5 of the 14 students reported Broden, M., Hall, R. V., & Mitts, B. (1971).
interobserver agreement percentages       The effect of self-recording on the behavior
                                          of two eighth-grade students. Journal of Ap-
below the accepted 80% standard. A        plied Behavior Analysis, 4, 191-199.
class discussion of these low agree- Cooper, J. O., Heron, T E., & Heward, W. L.
ment percentages and why they oc-         (1987). Applied behavior analysis. Colombus,
curred would aid students' understand-    OH: Merrill.
ing of interobserver agreement.         Hawkins, R. P., & Fabry, B. D. (1979). Applied
                                          behavior analysis and interobserver reliability:
                                                A commentary on two articles by Birkimer
Student Opinions and Evaluations           and Brown. Journal of Applied Behavior
                                           Analysis, 12, 545-552.
   The students who participated in this Hersen, M., & Barlow, D. H. (1976). Single
demonstration were asked to evaluate       case experimental designs: Strategies for
the demonstration. Overall, there were     studying behavior change. New York: Perga-
                                           mon Press.
no negative comments about the dem- Johnston,        J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (1992).
onstration. Comments included, "This       Strategies and tactics of behavioral research
was very helpful. After doing the ac-      (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tual calculations, I understood how to Kazdin, A. E. (1974). Self-monitoring and be-
find agreement much better. I believe      havior change. In M. J. Mahoney & C. E.
                                           Thoresen (Eds.), Self-control: Power to the
this example was great for showing         person (pp. 218-246). Monterey, CA: Brooks/
that self-monitoring can have a signif-    Cole.
icant impact on a certain behavior." Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and com-
"Demonstrations in class that are real     plexity of assessment: The ABCs of reli-
                                           ability. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
life make information stay in my mem-      10, 141-150.
ory." "I really liked the experiment. I Kazdin, A. E. (1979). Unobtrusive measures in
understand the concepts better since I     behavioral assessment. Journal ofApplied Be-
was an observer and an observee."          havior Analysis, 12, 713-724.
"Being monitored certainly impressed Kneedler,      R. D., & Hallahan, D. P. (1981). Self-
                                           monitoring of on-task behavior with learning
upon me that watching/observing            disabled children: Current studies and direc-
someone may alter their behavior."         tions. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 2(3),
                                                 73-82.
            CONCLUSION                        Lipinski, D. P., Black, J. L., Nelson, R. O., &
                                                Ciminero, A. R. (1975). Influence of moti-
   This simple classroom demonstra-              vational variables on the reactivity and reli-
tion can help to teach the concepts of          ability of self-recording. Journal of Consult-
                                                 ing and Clinical Psychology, 43, 637-646.
self-monitoring, reactivity, and inter-       Lovitt, T. C. (1973). Self-management projects
observer agreement by engaging stu-             with children with behavioral disabilities.
dents in observational behaviors that           Journal of Learning Disabilities, 6, 138-150.
produce effects they can analyze. The         Maletzky, B. M. (1974). Behavior recording as
                                                a treatment: A brief note. Behavior Therapy,
activity is easy to conduct, and stu-           5, 107-111.
dents seem to enjoy it. The entire dem-       McFall, R. M. (1977). Parameters of self-mon-
onstration takes less than 20 min to            itoring. In R. B. Stuart (Ed.), Behavioral self-
146                               JAMES E. CARR et al.
  management (pp. 196-214). New York: Bru-        Spiegler, M. D., & Guevremont, D. C. (1993).
  ner/Mazel.                                         Contemporary behavior therapy (2nd ed.). Pa-
Repp, A. C., Deitz, D. E., Boles, S. M., Deitz,     cific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
  S. M., & Repp, C. F (1976). Technical arti-     Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1991). Be-
  cle: Differences among common methods for         havior analysis for lasting change. Fort
  calculating interobserver agreement. Journal
  of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 109-113.         Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Schiffman, H. R. (1990). Sensation and percep-    Thoresen, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Be-
  tion: An integrative approach (3rd ed.). New      havioral self-control. New York: Holt, Rine-
  York: Wiley.                                      hart, & Winston.
You can also read