Addressing challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome: The use of applied behaviour analysis for assessment and intervention

Page created by Jessica Rivera
 
CONTINUE READING
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
64

     Addressing challenging
     behaviour in children with
     Down syndrome: The use of
     applied behaviour analysis for
     assessment and intervention
     Kathleen M. Feeley and Emily A. Jones
     C.W. Post Campus of Long Island University, USA

     Abstract – Children with Down syndrome are at an increased risk for engaging in challenging
     behaviour that may be part of a behavioural phenotype characteristic of Down syndrome. The
     methodology of applied behaviour analysis has been demonstrated effective with a wide range of
     challenging behaviours, across various disabilities. Applications to children with Down syndrome
     and the examination of behaviourally based strategies to specifically address the unique charac-
     teristics of children with Down syndrome are limited. However, there are several studies in which a
     subset of the participants did have Down syndrome. A handful of these studies are reviewed within
     the context of functional behaviour assessment and Positive Behavioural Supports. Drawing from
     these studies and the behavioural literature, as well as the authors’ clinical experience and research,
     suggestions regarding early intervention for challenging behaviour with children with Down syn-
     drome are provided.

     Keywords: applied behaviour analysis, functional behaviour assessment, Positive Behaviour Support,
     behavioural phenotype, proactive strategies, setting events, antecedent strategies, skill replacement,
     reinforcement systems, consequence strategies.

     Children with Down syndrome have a propensity to                such as “stubborn,” have long been noted in the litera-
     engage in behaviours that can be particularly problem-          ture (Gibson, 1978). More recently, parent and teacher
     atic for family members and caregivers, as well as profes-      ratings indicate that children with Down syndrome
     sionals who work with them. Such behaviour, referred to         show higher rates (than typically developing children) of
     by Doss and Reichle (1991) as challenging behaviour, is         attention problems, social withdrawal, noncompliance,
     defined as that which results “…in self-injury or injury         and compulsions (such as arranging objects and repeat-
     of others, causes damage to the physical environment,           ing certain actions) (e.g., Coe et al., 1999; Evans & Gray,
     interferes with the acquisition of new skills, and/or           2000) and high rates of self-talk (Glenn & Cunningham,
     socially isolates the learner” (p. 215). The presence of        2000). Additionally, behaviours associated with anxiety,
     challenging behaviour, along with characterisations

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
                                                                                                                               65
K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

depression, and withdrawal, have been noted to increase        Archer, 2002; Cunningham, Glenn, Lorenz, Cuckle &
with age (Dykens & Kasari, 1997).                              Shepperdson, 1998). Therefore, interventionists who are
There are specific physical characteristics of Down syn-        likely to encounter behaviour that may be associated with
drome (e.g., those associated with sleep disorders) (Rich-     later negative outcomes must be prepared to address such
dale, Francis, Gavidia-Payne & Cotton, 2000; Stores,           behaviour so that children with Down syndrome benefit
1993) as well as a higher incidence of illness (Roizen,        from learning opportunities and are less likely to be pre-
1996) that may significantly impact the behavioural             cluded from general education/community placements.
repertoires, including increasing the likelihood of chal-      Fortunately, the methodology of applied behaviour
lenging behaviour, in children with Down syndrome.             analysis, demonstrated effective across a wide range of
Researchers have also demonstrated that challenging            populations, with various disabilities, can be utilised to
behaviour, particularly avoidance behaviour, appears           address the unique characteristics of challenging behav-
consistently in very young infants with Down syndrome          iour demonstrated by children with Down syndrome.
(Wishart, 1993a, 1993b). Specifically, when presented           With thousands of studies demonstrating the positive
with task demands just slightly above their current level      impact of interventions based on the principles of behav-
of ability, children with Down syndrome demonstrate            iour analysis on the lives of individuals with disabili-
a unique pattern of behaviour involving opting out of          ties, both researchers and practitioners have long relied
learning opportunities and misusing social behaviours.         on this scientific literature to address the challenging
It appears that the presence of the extra chromosome           behaviour of individuals with developmental disabilities.
associated with Down syndrome affects the likelihood            Recently, the technology of Positive Behaviour Support
of challenging behaviour beginning in infancy. In turn,        (Carr et al., 2002) has emerged as an application of the
these behaviours can have catastrophic effects; inter-          principles of behaviour analysis to not only address skill
fering with learning in children with Down syndrome,           repertoires, but also redesign individuals’ living environ-
not only at the foundational level during infancy, but         ments with the goal of achieving enhanced quality of life
throughout life.                                               and decreases in problem behaviour (Carr et al., 1999).
These characteristics that impact the behaviour rep-           To date, researchers have not specifically applied the
ertoire reflect a unique pattern of strengths and weak-         principles of behaviour analysis and positive behaviour
nesses evident in individuals with Down syndrome and           support to meet the unique behavioural challenges pre-
have been described as a distinct cluster of behaviours        sented by individuals with Down syndrome. However,
termed a behavioural phenotype. Dykens (1995) defined           in an extensive review of the literature, we identified a
behavioural phenotype as “…the heightened probabil-            small number of studies utilising behavioural assessment
ity or likelihood that people with a given syndrome will       strategies, as well as several intervention studies that
exhibit certain behavioural and developmental sequela          addressed severe challenging behaviour in individuals
relative to those without the syndrome” (p. 523). This         with Down syndrome. The focus of these investigations
does not mean that all children with Down syndrome             was not on the unique characteristics of individuals with
will demonstrate all of the characteristic behaviours;         Down syndrome, rather the focus was a particular form
rather, there will be in an increased likelihood.              of challenging behaviour (e.g., aggression, self-injury) or
The early developing avoidance behaviour reported by           a particular intervention strategy (e.g., reinforcement)
Wishart and colleagues along with reports of other chal-       and involved either a sole participant who had Down
lenging behaviour is believed to be part of this phenotype     syndrome or several participants, only one or two of
in very young children with Down syndrome (Fidler,             whom had Down syndrome.
2005). Thus, it is of utmost importance to intervene on        In this paper, we briefly describe applied behaviour
the behaviours characteristic of this behavioural pheno-       analysis and its applications in assessing and addressing
type early on so that they do not result in pronounced         challenging behaviours prevalent in children with Down
deficits within the child’s later development.                  syndrome. We then draw upon the existing literature as
Challenging behaviour not only precludes individu-             well as our clinical experience and ongoing research to
als with Down syndrome from learning opportunities             provide suggestions regarding behaviourally based appli-
within their environment, but also prevents them from          cations to ameliorate specific challenging behaviours
accessing more typical educational and community               associated with Down syndrome in an effort to intervene
environments as they approach preschool and school             as early as possible on this aspect of the behavioural phe-
age. When placement in a general education or commu-           notype characteristic of children with Down syndrome.
nity setting does occur, challenging behaviour is likely
to be the cause of removal from these settings. This is
particularly unfortunate, as general education settings
have been found to lead to more positive outcomes for
children with Down syndrome (Buckley, Bird, Sacks &

                                            © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
                                                                              http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
66
                                                 K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

     Applied behaviour analysis                                        The work of Wishart (1993a; 1993b) and Wishart and
                                                                       Duffy (1990) suggests that certain functions of chal-
     Based upon the seminal work of Baer, Wolf and Risley              lenging behaviour may be more characteristic of chil-
     (1968), applied behaviour analysis emphasises interven-           dren with Down syndrome and part of the characteristic
     tions addressing socially significant age-appropriate              behavioural phenotype. In particular, both escape and
     behaviours with immediate importance to the indi-                 attention seeking functions seem prevalent in Wishart’s
     vidual using precise measurement of those behaviours              reports of performance during assessment tasks. Spe-
     in need of improvement. Generalised behaviours, that              cifically, Wishart and colleagues (Pitcairn & Wishart,
     is, behaviours that are maintained over time, appear in           1994; Wishart, 1986; Wishart & Duffy) report avoidance
     other environments, and extend to other behaviours,               behaviours ranging from disruptive tantrum behaviours
     are targeted. A functional relationship between changes           (e.g., sweeping test items off the table) to charming or
     in behaviour and the intervention being implemented               ‘cute’ behaviours (e.g., clapping, blowing raspberries)
     is demonstrated. Interventions, derived from the basic            that might distract the evaluator during task demands.
     principles of behaviour (e.g., reinforcement, extinction),        The overlapping attention seeking function of such task
     are described so that they can be easily replicated, and          avoidance behaviours was also noted by Kasari and Free-
     their effectiveness is measured by improvement in the              man (2001) who confirmed the higher frequency of these
     individual’s performance.                                         ‘charming’ behaviours in older children with Down syn-
                                                                       drome (6-10 years). In fact, in their study, children with
                                                                       Down syndrome engaged in higher rates of looking to
     Functions of challenging                                          the experimenter during a task situation that may have
                                                                       been related to the significantly longer latency to start
     behaviour                                                         and complete tasks, when compared to typically devel-
                                                                       oping children and children with mental retardation not
     It has long been established that challenging behaviour           related to Down syndrome.
     is directly related to environmental variables (e.g., how         As a result of these findings, it seems that both escape and
     the behaviour is consequated). From a behaviour ana-              attention seeking behaviours may be particularly preva-
     lytic perspective, challenging behaviours are maintained          lent in children with Down syndrome. To determine
     because they are positively reinforced (i.e., result in the       the extent to which an individual child possesses these
     delivery of a preferred item/activity) and/or are nega-           specific characteristics (increased likelihood of engaging
     tively reinforced (i.e., result in the removal of a nonpre-       in escape/avoid or attention motivated problem behav-
     ferred item/activity). O’Neill, Horner, Albin, Storey and         iour) of the behavioural phenotype, interventionists can
     Sprague (1997) described these two major functional               conduct a functional behaviour assessment. Not only
     categories of behaviour as either to obtain or to escape/         will this enable interventionists to explore this aspect of
     avoid. That is, behaviour may function to obtain conse-           the behavioural phenotype, but specific antecedents and
     quences such as attention (e.g., from a parent or teacher)        consequences associated with the challenging behaviour
     and access to objects (e.g., snacks, toys) or activities (e.g.,   can be determined.
     recess, trip to the park). Behaviour may also function to
     escape/avoid consequences such as attention (e.g., inter-
     actions with peers) and objects (e.g., certain foods) or
     activities (e.g., academic tasks).
                                                                       Functional behaviour
     In addition to the functions of obtain or escape/avoid,           assessment
     O’Neill et al., (1997) further categorise the functions of
     challenging behaviours in terms of socially or nonsocially        While certain functions (e.g., escape, obtain attention)
     motivated behaviours. Socially motivated behaviours               may be associated with Down syndrome, challenging
     involve those in which the child seeks to escape/avoid or         behaviour can serve a variety of functions within and
     obtain something from another individual in their envi-           across individual children. Therefore, the foundation
     ronment. Behaviours serving a social function in chil-            of addressing challenging behaviour is determining the
     dren with developmental disabilities are often related to         function a specific challenging behaviour serves for a
     impairments in communication skills (Carr & Durand,               particular child (i.e., to obtain or escape/avoid; socially
     1985; Durand & Carr, 1991; 1992). Nonsocially moti-               or non-socially motivated). This is accomplished via the
     vated behaviours involve those in which the child seeks           functional behaviour assessment process in which the
     to escape/avoid or obtain internal stimuli (i.e., sensory         relationship between events in a person’s environment
     stimulation) and, thus, do not involve consequences               and the occurrence of challenging behaviour is deter-
     related to another individual. These nonsocially moti-            mined in an effort to identify factors maintaining that
     vated behaviours are often referred to as ‘self-stimula-          behaviour. (The reader is referred to O’Neill et al.’s [1997]
     tory’ behaviours.

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
                                                                                                                                67
K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

practical handbook for a detailed discussion of func-           more frequent sleep problems, including restless sleep,
tional behaviour assessment procedures.)                        waking more than once per night, and waking prior to
                                                                5:00 am, in children with Down syndrome compared
                                                                to typically developing children. The higher incidence
Interview                                                       of sleep problems appears to be related to several physi-
                                                                cal features (specifically, upper airway obstruction due
The first step in the functional assessment process con-         in part to relatively small mouths and airway passages,
sists of an interview to identify the behaviour of concern;     enlarged tonsils, and obesity) (Stores & Stores, 1996).
related environmental/medical factors; and when/where/          These sleep problems were also associated with daytime
with whom and during which activities the behaviour             problem behaviour: specifically, irritability, hyperactiv-
occurs most/least. The goal is to identify factors that are     ity, and stereotypies (Richdale et al., 2000; Stores, 1993).
related to the occurrence of challenging behaviour.             Thus, sleep disorders may be one motivating operation
In the past, when addressing challenging behaviour, the         that affects the likelihood of challenging behaviour in
field of applied behaviour analysis concentrated on the          children with Down syndrome.
immediate antecedents and consequences. However, sev-           There is also a high incidence of illness in children with
eral behaviourists have recognised that events occurring        Down syndrome (e.g., recurrent ear infections, gastroin-
more distally in time or not directly related to the imme-      testinal disorders, skin conditions) (Roizen, 1996). The
diate antecedents or consequences affect the likelihood          onset of an illness, its presence, as well as the ending
of challenging behaviour (Michael, 1982; 2000; Wahler           stages, may be motivating operations affecting the behav-
& Fox, 1981). Such variables have been referred to as           iour of children with Down syndrome, as is often the case
motivating operations (Michael, 2000) as well as setting        with typically developing children. As the frequency and
events (Wahler & Fox, 1981). Specifically, motivating            severity of these illnesses is increased in individuals with
operations refer to antecedents that affect an individual’s      Down syndrome, the incidence of challenging behaviour
behaviour by changing the value (increasing or decreas-         is likely to be increased as well.
ing) of a consequence (reinforcer or punisher) which, in
                                                                It is during the interview process that environmental
turn, changes the likelihood the individual will engage
                                                                events including setting events/motivating operations,
in a certain behaviour (either increasing or decreasing
                                                                antecedents, and consequences, as well as target behav-
the likelihood). For example, a child may be motivated
                                                                iours are identified. It is here that interventionists should
to follow the directions (immediate antecedent) of his
                                                                closely examine the extent to which specific motivating
teacher because doing so results in a pleasant interaction
                                                                operations (or setting events) associated with Down syn-
(i.e., the teacher smiles and praises the child; reinforcing
                                                                drome affect the individual’s likelihood of engaging in
consequence). However, if the child has a cold, the rein-
                                                                challenging behaviour. Just as the function of behaviour
forcing value of the smile and praise may be significantly
                                                                is likely to vary across individuals, the specific anteced-
decreased, thus, decreasing the likelihood the child will
                                                                ents and consequences as well as the presence of motivat-
engage in compliant behaviour in response to the teach-
                                                                ing operations (or setting events) are likely to be unique
er’s requests. In this situation, the cold is a motivating
                                                                to each child with Down syndrome and require careful
operation.
                                                                assessment to determine which are relevant for a given
The term motivating operation has been used to describe         child.
the effect a specific event has on the quality of a future
consequence (e.g., reinforcer) and, thus, the likelihood
of the target behaviour occurring. Within the Positive          Direct observation
Behaviour Support literature, the term setting event
is widely used to represent the phenomena described             The second component of the functional behaviour
(Horner, Vaughn, Day & Ard, 1996). Several studies have         assessment process consists of direct observation of the
been conducted describing a relationship between dif-           individual and documentation of events that occur prior
ferent setting events (e.g., illness, sleep problems) in the    to and following the challenging behaviour. One example
lives of individuals with developmental disabilities and        is an antecedent-behaviour-consequence (ABC) analysis,
a propensity to engage in challenging behaviour (e.g.,          in which antecedents and consequences are documented
Dadson & Horner, 1993; McGill, Teer, Rye & Hughes,              as they occur in the natural environment. The child is
2005).                                                          observed during his/her daily routine and the occurrence
We suggest that the propensity of individuals with              of challenging behaviours, events that precede the specific
Down syndrome to engage in challenging behaviour is             behaviour (antecedents), and events that follow the spe-
directly related to several motivating operations (or set-      cific behaviour (consequences) are documented. Analysis
ting events) that are inherently related to the presence        of the pattern of antecedents and consequences results in
of Down syndrome. For example, Stores (1993) found              a hypothesised function of the challenging behaviour.

                                             © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
                                                                               http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
68
                                                 K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

     See Table 1 for an ABC data sheet with guidelines regard-           consequences are delivered by others (alone condition);
     ing information that should be included and Table 2 for             and a condition involving noncontingent delivery of pre-
     an example ABC data sheet with sample behaviours.                   ferred items and attention (free play or control condition).
                                                                         Antecedent conditions can also be manipulated, such as
                                                                         task difficulty or attention provided while engaged in the
     Functional analysis                                                 task. The conditions in which the challenging behaviour
                                                                         occurs are indicative of the factor(s) occasioning and/or
     Often, the function of a particular challenging behav-              maintaining the behaviour. For example, if higher rates
     iour for a child can be identified through the interview             of challenging behaviour occur within the demand con-
     and direct observation process. However, a more sys-                dition than within the other conditions, escape would
     tematic approach may be necessary to confirm observa-                appear to be the function of the challenging behaviour.
     tion findings and/or determine the function, if it has not
                                                                         Once the functional behaviour assessment process is
     been clearly identified. Functional analysis consists of
                                                                         complete, and the function of the challenging behaviour
     the systematic manipulation of controlling variables to
                                                                         identified, intervention procedures can be developed
     demonstrate the function of the behaviour (the reader is
                                                                         that specifically address the challenging behaviour and
     referred to O’Neill et al., 1997 as well as Iwata, Dorsey,
                                                                         the function it serves for that individual. Intervention-
     Slifer, Bauman & Richman, 1982 for explicit procedures).
                                                                         ists should draw from the large number of empirically
     Functional analysis involves measuring challenging
                                                                         validated studies demonstrating the facilitative effects of
     behaviour as a series of conditions are introduced. Conse-
                                                                         behaviour analytic procedures.
     quence conditions include situations in which: attention
     is delivered contingent upon the occurrence of chal-
     lenging behaviour (attention condition); demands are
     removed contingent upon the occurrence of challenging
                                                                         Intervention
     behaviour (demand condition); tangibles are delivered                   An effective means of addressing challenging behaviour,
     contingent upon the occurrence of challenging behav-                    Positive Behaviour Support (Carr et al., 2002), is rooted
     iour (tangible condition); the child is left alone and no               in behaviour analytic strategies. Positive Behaviour Sup-
                                                                                                         port consists of strategies
     Table 1. Sample ABC data sheet                                                                      addressing multiple variables
                                                                                                         influencing the occurrence of
      Date Antecedent                Behaviour               Consequence            Hypothesised
                                                                                    Function
                                                                                                         challenging behaviour. When
                                                                                                         utilised at an individual level,
             Activity                Description of          How did you            Obtain attention,
                                     behaviour(s) that       respond?               tangibles, access to
                                                                                                         Positive Behaviour Support
                                     occurred                                       activity             interventions consist of the
             Location of student     May need to include     How did others         Escape demands,      development of a four compo-
                                     dimensions such         respond?               transition, or       nent approach which includes
                                     as duration and                                attention            strategies to address motivat-
                                     intensity                                                           ing operations/setting events
             Others present                                  What happened to Obtain internal            and immediate antecedents,
             (teachers, students)                            the activity?          stimuli              interventions to teach replace-
             Immediate trigger                                                      Escape internal      ment skills (e.g., communica-
             (demand, removal of                                                    stimuli
                                                                                                         tion, academic, social), and the
             attention)
                                                                                                         development of appropriate
                                                                                                         consequence strategies (e.g.,
     Table 2. Example behaviours documented on an ABC data sheet                                         reinforcement strategies) as
                                                                                                         outlined in Figure 1. To illus-
      Date     Antecedent          Behaviour         Consequence                        Hypothesised
                                                                                        Function
                                                                                                         trate behavioural applications
      2/5/06 Child playing         Child climbs      Caregiver rushes over, swoops Attention             within this four component
               on floor, parent     on table          child up, telling her “No, no,     seeking          framework, we will provide
               attending to                          honey, that’s dangerous”                            a brief description of a select
               sibling                               followed by a hug and a kiss.                       few studies implemented with
                                                     Caregiver puts child down
                                                     with toys and plays with child                      children with Down syn-
                                                                                                         drome, followed by additional
      2/7/06 Teacher presents Child makes a          Teacher laughs with child          Escape and       suggestions for interventions
               nonpreferred        silly face and    and joins in song, delaying        attention
               task (e.g., writing begins to sing    the request to engage in the       seeking
                                                                                                         based upon the research liter-
               activity)           a rhyming         nonpreferred task                                   ature and our clinical experi-
                                   song                                                                  ence and research. The studies

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
                                                                                                                                 69
K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

                                                 Positive Behaviour Support Plan

 Strategies to Address           Antecedent Strategies              Skill Building Strategies      Consequence Strategies
 Setting Events                  Choice                             Functional communication       Differential reinforcement
 Setting event checklist         High probability request           training                       of alternative behaviour
 Presessions of attention        Collaboration                      Tolerance for delay of         Differential reinforcement
 Decrease presence of                                               reinforcement                  of the omission of
                                 Preferred item as distractor                                      behaviour
 antecedents
 Increase available                                                                                Token systems
 reinforcement                                                                                     Extinction

Figure 1: Components of a Positive Behaviour Support Plan with example interventions.

utilising behavioural strategies with individuals with            the functional assessment process), while the challeng-
Down syndrome we will discuss are outlined in Table 3.            ing behaviour of the other child served to escape. Thus,
                                                                  although there is an increased likelihood of some setting
                                                                  events in children with Down syndrome, not all may be
Addressing setting events                                         pertinent to all children with Down syndrome. Addi-
                                                                  tionally, once the functional assessment process has been
(motivating operations)                                           completed (taking into account setting events), interven-
                                                                  tionists should select intervention strategies that directly
Because the term setting event is used in the Positive            correspond to the findings. For example, as illustrated by
Behaviour Support literature, we have chosen to use it            McComas et al., presession attention did not affect indi-
in our discussion of intervention strategies. One setting         viduals who engage in challenging behaviour serving
event that we hypothesise may be specifically associ-              functions other than to obtain attention.
ated with Down syndrome is the tendency toward an
increased desire to escape/avoid demands and to obtain            Other setting events, such as sleep disorders and ill-
attention as suggested by Wishart’s research (1993a,              ness, that warrant attention specifically in children with
1993b). McComas, Thompson and Johnson (2003) uti-                 Down syndrome, may be addressed by developing a
lised a setting event intervention strategy consisting of         mechanism for caregivers to share with school person-
presessions during which attention was provided. Sev-             nel when a child has experienced a particular setting
eral children participated in this study, two of whom             event. Checklists can be designed to specifically note
had Down syndrome (11 and 12 year old boys). In one               setting events influencing the child’s challenging behav-
condition, attention was delivered noncontingently (i.e.,         iour. Alternatively, setting events can be reported via a
regardless of the child’s behaviour) during the 10 min-           note sent to school or a telephone call. School person-
utes prior to engagement in tasks likely to elicit challeng-      nel can then consider several interventions designed to
ing behaviours (i.e., during presessions). In the second          ameliorate the effects of the specific setting event (e.g.,
condition, students were ignored during the 10 minute             Dadson & Horner; 1993; Horner et al., 1996). For exam-
presessions. Although, the presession condition in which          ple, a child’s illness may increase the likelihood that he/
attention was provided had no effect on the challenging            she engages in self stimulatory behaviour (e.g., repetitive
behaviour of the child who was escape motivated, the              hand movements). In this case, the child might be given
child who was attention motivated engaged in challeng-            the opportunity to access materials that would function
ing behaviour during subsequent tasks almost exclusively          as a replacement response (e.g., holding onto a small tex-
when no attention was delivered during the presessions.           tured toy stored within the front pocket of a sweatshirt).
                                                                  Another child who experiences trouble sleeping might be
McComas et al.’s (2003) study demonstrated the effec-              more likely to desire to escape demands. In this case, the
tiveness of an intervention procedure to specifically              child may be given an opportunity to rest at some point
address a setting event, in this case, the increased desire       during the day, or school staff might consider reducing
for attention. It is important to note, attention served          their demands (e.g., requesting fewer academic tasks) on
as a reinforcer in the target situation for only one of the       days following disrupted sleep. The effects of the setting
two children with Down syndrome (as confirmed by

                                              © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
                                                                                http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
70
                                                  K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

     Table 3. Studies examining the use of behavioural interventions to address challenging behaviour in
     children with Down syndrome
      Author/      Subject/Setting           Target Behaviour and           Independent Variable        Results
      Year         Description               Function
      Cole &       7 year old boy with       Throwing/destroying items,     Choice versus no            Challenging behaviour decreased
      Levinson     Down syndrome, PDD,       hitting, and dropping to       choice embedded             from a range of 14.3% to 81.8% in the
      (2002)       and ADD attending         the floor)                      within verbal prompts       no choice condition to 8.3% in the
                   a school for students     Function: No functional        delivered during            choice condition, with an increase in
                   with emotional/           assessment was conducted       instruction of daily        independent task performance
                   behavioural disorders                                    routine
      Davis,       7 year old boy with       Noncompliance, kicking,        The delivery of three       Compliance increased from a
      Brady,       Down syndrome,            screaming, spitting,           to five high-probability     mean of 6.7% during baseline to
      Williams &   enrolled in a life        stereotypic behaviour,         requests, each followed     100% following intervention, with
      Hamilton     skills program on an      emptying cabinets/drawers      by the delivery of verbal   performance generalised to other
      (1992)       elementary school         Function: No functional        or gestural (thumbs up)     adults and was maintained during
                   campus                    assessment was conducted       praise                      follow up probes
      Hall,        9 year old boy with       Aggression: hitting,           Instruction of              Aggressive/destructive behaviour
      Neuharth-    Down syndrome             destroying and throwing        communicative               decreased from 11% (of the 10
      Pritchett    in a self contained       materials                      replacement of raising      second intervals within a 45 minute
      & Belfiore    classroom with            Function: both escape and      hand and saying “done”      session) in baseline to 2% during
      (1997)       inclusive opportunities   attention                                                  intervention
                                                                                                        Performance generalised from his
                                                                                                        special education classroom to a
                                                                                                        general education classroom
      McComas,     Two boys with Down        Dan: throwing materials,       Noncontingent               Challenging behaviour decreased for
      Thompson     syndrome enrolled         hitting, and spitting          attention versus no         Dan (who was attention motivated)
      & Johnson    in special education      Function: attention seeking    attention during 10         when attention presessions were
      (2003)       classes within a public                                  minute presessions          provided
                   school:                                                  prior to engaging in        Challenging behaviour did not
                   Dan, 11 years old         Ari: hitting, pinching,        tasks likely to elicit      change for Ari (who was escape
                                             kicking, throwing items        challenging behaviour       motivated)
                   Ari, 12 years old
                                             Function: escape
      Repp &       9 year old girl with      Tantrums (crying, falling      Extinction, differential     Challenging behaviour decreased
      Karsh        Down syndrome             to floor, kicking, hitting,     reinforcement of            from 41% of the day in baseline to an
      (1994)       placed in a segregated    throwing objects, and          alternative behaviour,      average of 4% during intervention
                   school                    grabbing) and finger            and increased               Maintenance observed at one year
                                             stereotypies (finger            opportunities to engage     follow-up
                                             flexions)                       in social interactions
                                             Function: attention

     events may also be ameliorated through the delivery of                rence of challenging behaviour by altering the antecedent
     higher rates and/or higher quality of reinforcement. This             situations associated with it (e.g., decreasing the aver-
     reinforcement should be delivered before the child is                 siveness of the antecedent stimuli). Thus, these strategies
     likely to engage in the challenging behaviour (Marcus &               are implemented proactively, that is, prior to the point at
     Vollmer, 1996). For example, a child can be greeted at the            which the challenging behaviour is likely to occur (iden-
     bus with high rates of quality attention and, therefore,              tified via the functional assessment process). Four strate-
     may be less likely to engage in attention motivated chal-             gies may be particularly relevant to decrease the escape
     lenging behaviour upon entering the classroom.                        and/or attention motivated challenging behaviours often
     Strategies to address setting events can have substantial             seen in children with Down syndrome: choices, high
     effects on the likelihood of challenging behaviour. How-               probability request sequence, collaboration, and pre-
     ever, in many instances, it may also be necessary to con-             ferred item as distractor.
     sider interventions that target immediate antecedents.                Offering choices (e.g., of tasks, materials, locations, etc.)
                                                                           is a strategy that has been effectively used in a number of
                                                                           studies to decrease challenging behaviour (e.g., Bambara,
     Antecedent based strategies                                           Koger, Katze & Davenport, 1995; Dunlap, et al., 1994;
                                                                           Dyer, Dunlap & Winterling, 1990). In one application
     Intervention strategies targeting stimuli that occur just             with a 7 year old boy with Down syndrome, Cole and Lev-
     prior to the child engaging in challenging behaviour are              inson (2002) demonstrated the effectiveness of embed-
     termed antecedent based strategies (Kern, Choutka &                   ding choices within the daily routine of hand washing,
     Sokol, 2002). These strategies aim to prevent the occur-              a situation during which the boy was likely to engage in

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
                                                                                                                                71
K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

challenging behaviour (e.g., throwing/destroying items,         tract them from the aversiveness of the task that triggers
hitting, and dropping to the floor). Rather than deliver-        challenging behaviour. The preferred item is systemati-
ing a directive regarding how to complete a step within         cally presented prior to, or simultaneous with, the request
the task (e.g., “Rub your hands”), the child was offered         that is likely to occasion the challenging behaviour. For
choices regarding how the step could be completed (e.g.,        example, a child can be given a favourite toy and then
“Do you want to rub them together quickly or slowly?”).         requested to take a walk to another instructional area.
Challenging behaviour decreased while independent               Alternatively, the interventionist may embed the pre-
task participation increased. Thus, while presenting tasks      ferred item (e.g., whistle) into the request (e.g., “Can you
that trigger challenging behaviour, choices can be offered       hold the whistle while we walk in from recess?”).
with respect to a variety of aspects, such as location (e.g.,   For antecedent strategies to be effective, it is essential
“You can work at your desk or at the group table”), peers       for interventionists to be fluent in their use, paying par-
(“Would you like to walk with John or Mary?”), or mate-         ticular attention to implementing the strategies prior to
rials (e.g., “You can use your pencil or my blue pen”) to       presenting the antecedents that are associated with the
decrease escape motivated challenging behaviours.               behaviour. While setting event and antecedent based
Another antecedent strategy, high probability request           intervention strategies serve to proactively decrease
sequence, involves the delivery of a series of requests to      the likelihood that challenging behaviour will occur, it
which the child is highly like to comply (a high probabil-      is important to recognise the function the challenging
ity request) each of which is followed by the delivery of       behaviour serves for a particular child and teach replace-
reinforcers, then a request to which the child is not likely    ment skills for the child to use to obtain what they want
to comply, that is, a low probability request (the one that     through more appropriate means.
triggers the challenging behaviour) is delivered. For
example, Davis, Brady, Williams and Hamilton (1992)
successfully utilised high probability request sequence to      Replacement skills
increase compliance and decrease challenging behaviour
(screaming, hitting, kicking, spitting) in a 7 year old boy     Many individuals with Down syndrome can benefit from
with Down syndrome. Behaviour change generalised                instruction in specific skills (e.g., communication, social,
across classroom staff and was maintained during a series        academic, etc.) that function to replace challenging
of 4 weekly follow up probes. High probability request          behaviour with a more appropriate means of obtaining
sequences have not only been effective in decreasing             reinforcers. One type of skill building strategy, func-
challenging behaviours (Horner, Day, Sprague, O’Brien           tional communication training, involves identifying and
& Heathfield, 1991; Mace & Belfiore, 1990), but also              teaching a more appropriate communicative response
increasing compliance (Mace et al., 1988; Singer, Singer        serving the same function as the challenging behaviour
& Horner, 1987), particularly during transitions (Davis,        (Carr et al., 1994).
Reichle & Southard, 2000), as well as increasing social         Several researchers (e.g., Brooks, Todd, Tofflemoyer &
interactions (Davis, Brady, McEvoy & Williams, 1994;            Horner, 2003; Hetzroni & Roth, 2003) have implemented
Davis & Reichle, 1996) and communicative behaviours             functional communication training with children with
(Davis, Reichle & Johnston, 1998). This strategy may be         Down syndrome. For example, Hall, Neuharth-Pritchet
particularly effective with a child who is not only escape/      and Belfiore (1997) determined that the aggressive and
avoid motivated, but also attention motivated, because,         destructive behaviours of a 9 year old boy with Down
inherent in the high probability request sequence, each         syndrome served the function of both escape (the wait)
compliant response results in social reinforcement (e.g.,       and attention when he was required to wait after com-
praise).                                                        pleting a task. He was taught to indicate when he was
A third antecedent strategy that may be particularly            finished by raising his hand and saying “done” which
effective for children with Down syndrome who not only           resulted in the delivery of a brief, in-seat academic activ-
desire to escape/avoid a task, but also obtain attention,       ity. Acquisition of this replacement response resulted in
is an offer of collaboration (Davis, McEvoy & Riechle,           a decrease in aggressive/destructive behaviours that gen-
2005). This entails sharing the responsibility of the task      eralised from his special education classroom to a gen-
with the child. For example, during a worksheet task, the       eral education classroom.
interventionist tells the child, “You do the first five items     As the speech and language skills of individuals with
and I’ll do the last five.” Over time, the amount of assist-     Down syndrome are specifically impaired, their ability
ance provided by the interventionist can be systemati-          to communicate desires is likely to be compromised and,
cally faded (e.g., “You do the first six, and I’ll do the last   thus, related to the occurrence of challenging behaviour.
four”) until the child is performing independently.             Based on the extensive literature examining the effec-
Lastly, preferred item as a distractor (Davis et al., 2000)     tiveness of functional communication training (e.g.,
involves presenting a child with a preferred item to dis-       Carr & Durand, 1985; Durand & Carr, 1991; 1992), com-

                                             © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
                                                                               http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
72
                                                K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

     municative replacement strategies can be used to address        that is, reinforcement of the alternative behaviour of task
     a variety of communicative functions. For example, a            engagement), along with providing increased opportu-
     child who desires to escape circle time activities can be       nities to engage in social interactions. This intervention
     taught to raise his/her hand to request to leave the area.      package resulted in a decrease in challenging behaviour
     For a child who desires attention, he/she can be taught to      which maintained at one year follow-up. Thus, a combi-
     tap an adult’s shoulder to request attention (e.g., to read     nation of consequence strategies for both the challenging
     a book). A child who tends to tantrum in the presence           behaviour (i.e., extinction) and more appropriate behav-
     of desired objects can be taught to appropriately request       iour (i.e., DRA) were important components of effective
     the desired object (e.g., point to communication symbols        intervention.
     “want” “[item]”).                                               Repp and Karsh’s (1994) study illustrates two points with
     When teaching a child to communicate his/her desires            respect to consequence strategies. First, their findings
     instead of engaging in challenging behaviour, it is cru-        that challenging behaviour, in the presence of demands,
     cial to reinforce appropriate communicative behaviours          may actually serve the purpose of accessing attention, are
     with immediate access to what the child desires (e.g., a        particularly relevant in light of Wishart’s work (Wishart
     break for a child who is escape motivated, attention for        & Duffy, 1990; Pitcairn & Wishart, 1994) demonstrat-
     the child who is attention motivated). Once the commu-          ing the propensity of children with Down syndrome to
     nicative replacement response is acquired, it is possible       engage in what appear to be escape motivated challenging
     to systematically teach the child to wait longer periods        behaviours. They do so, however, in a very social manner
     of time before the child can access what they desire. This      (e.g., engaging in party tricks) which often results in not
     intervention is referred to as teaching tolerance for delay     only escaping/avoiding a task, but also obtaining atten-
     of reinforcement (Carr et al., 1994). For example, once         tion. In many instances, the exact consequences main-
     the child reliably uses the replacement communication           taining challenging behaviour may be unclear, making
     skill (e.g., raising his/her hand), when the child makes        intervention selection difficult. However, systematic
     the appropriate request, increasing amounts of time can         implementation of functional assessment procedures
     be inserted before delivery of the reinforcer (e.g., tell the   in conjunction with carefully chosen interventions that
     child “Just one minute.”).                                      specifically address the escape and attention motivated
     Replacement skills are particularly important as they           functions of behaviour can lead to significant changes in
     provide the child with a communicative response that            the behavioural repertoires of children with Down syn-
     can be used across multiple situations to more appropri-        drome.
     ately obtain desirable outcomes. It is equally important        Second, Repp and Karsh’s (1994) study illustrates the
     for interventionists to also implement effective conse-          use of reinforcement procedures (i.e., DRA) to decrease
     quence strategies for both appropriate and inappropriate        a challenging behaviour. Another reinforcement proce-
     behaviour.                                                      dure, differential reinforcement of the omission of behav-
                                                                     iour (DRO), is particularly effective with stereotypical
                                                                     behaviours (e.g., Repp, Deitz & Speir, 1974). DRO involves
     Consequence strategies                                          delivering reinforcement following an interval of time in
                                                                     which the challenging behaviour did not occur. We have
     Effective interventions addressing challenging behaviour         used this procedure with several children with Down
     typically include reinforcement systems for appropri-           syndrome to successfully decrease self-stimulatory
     ate behaviour as well as consequences for inappropriate         behaviours (e.g., mouthing objects, tongue clicking, and
     behaviour. For example, Repp and Karsh (1994) con-              lip licking). In all cases, functional assessment indicated
     ducted a study with two students, one of whom was a             the behaviours were non-socially motivated. Each child
     9 year old girl with Down syndrome whose challeng-              was initially reinforced on a dense schedule (e.g., every
     ing behaviours consisted of tantrums (crying, falling to        30 seconds). The time interval was then quickly increased
     floor, kicking, hitting, throwing objects, and grabbing)         (to 1, 3, 5, and 10 minute intervals) to the point at which,
     and finger stereotypies (finger flexions) that had resulted        for each of the children, the behaviour completely sub-
     in her removal from an integrated educational set-              sided and continued reinforcement specifically for the
     ting. Direct observation during baseline indicated that,        absence of that self-stimulatory behaviour was no longer
     although she engaged in tantrums within demand situ-            necessary.
     ations, she was consistently consequated with attention
                                                                     Two criticisms of DRO procedures are the interven-
     rather than withdrawal of demands. Intervention con-
                                                                     tion does not leave the individual with a specific skill,
     sisted of placing the tantrum behaviour on extinction
                                                                     rather it only decreases the target behaviour and in some
     (in this case, no longer providing attention) as well as
                                                                     instances, the function of the challenging behaviour is
     increasing the rate of reinforcement for task engagement
                                                                     not honoured (i.e., reinforcement is delivered based on
     (differential reinforcement of alternative behaviour (DRA),
                                                                     the omission of the behaviour with no specific functional

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
                                                                                                                                73
K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

equivalent response being taught). Thus, many interven-         widespread that private and public policy statements spe-
tionists prefer to implement differential reinforcement of       cifically call for their use prior to consideration of reac-
alterative behaviours (DRA), such as that used by Repp          tive or punishment based procedures. For example, in the
and Karsh (1994), in which an alternative response is           United States, federal education legislation (Individuals
consistently reinforced. As another example, consider a         with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004)
child who has a tendency to place objects in his mouth          specifically notes “In the case of a child whose behavior
to get attention. Appropriate responses (e.g., play behav-      impedes the child’s learning or that of others, consider
iours with the objects) can be selected to systematically       the uses of proactive behavioral interventions and sup-
reinforce. If not readily within the child’s repertoire,        ports…” (Sec. 614). However, when positive approaches
the child can be prompted to perform the behaviour              have been systematically applied with little success, the
and then immediately provided with reinforcers. Thus,           use of reactive procedures implemented to decrease
the challenging behaviour (mouthing objects) can be             the future likelihood of challenging behaviours may be
replaced with an appropriate play behaviour. In instances       warranted. For example, consider a child who destroys
in which the alternative response is communicative in           materials to escape/avoid academic demands. A multi-
nature, such as within functional communication train-          component intervention may consist of giving the child
ing, differential reinforcement (DRA) is in effect.               a choice of academic activities, the use of a preferred item
For socially motivated behaviours, such differential rein-       as a distractor (i.e., “You can use your markers to fill in
forcement procedures are often paired with extinction to        these questions.”), as well as teaching the child to request
increase their effectiveness. Extinction involves no longer      assistance when the academic task becomes difficult
delivering reinforcement following the occurrence of            (functional communication training). When the appro-
challenging behaviour. Using the previous example, the          priate behaviour(s) occur, the child is immediately rein-
target behaviour (i.e., mouthing objects) would no longer       forced (DRA). However, in the event the target behaviour
be reinforced, thus, no attention would be delivered fol-       does occur, that is, the child destroys the materials, inter-
lowing its occurrence (i.e., extinction). This would be         ventionists might consider a consequence procedure that
paired with high quality reinforcement (i.e., attention         is likely to decrease the future occurrence of the behav-
would be delivered following appropriate play behav-            iour, such as the removal of a privilege (e.g., no recess)
iours) delivered following the alternative behaviour. The       or an added chore (e.g., cleaning the dry erase board).
function of challenging behaviour must be considered,           In these instances, it is of utmost importance for inter-
not only in consequating challenging behaviour, but             vention teams (including caregivers) to make informed
also in the choice of reinforcers to increase appropriate       decisions, in addition to acquiring consent from all nec-
behaviour. In the previous example, appropriate play            essary parties and monitoring intervention for desired
behaviour resulted in attention. As another example, a          effectiveness (Cooper, Heward & Heron, 1987).
student who is escape motivated can be reinforced for
appropriate task completion with the removal of one or
several demands.
                                                                Summary
Reinforcement systems such as token economies, in which         As the four component approach of positive behaviour
tokens (e.g., stickers, check marks) are delivered for          support has effectively addressed challenging behaviour
appropriate behaviour and then cashed in at some later          in individuals with various developmental disabilities
point for backup reinforcers (e.g., toys, food), can also be    across the age span, it is also likely to effectively meet
used to reinforce appropriate behaviour. Such token sys-        the needs of children with Down syndrome who engage
tems exist in many classrooms, but often need to be indi-       in challenging behaviours. Positive behaviour support
vidualised to meet the unique needs of a particular child       interventions can be used to systematically address char-
with Down syndrome. Token systems can even be used              acteristics specific to Down syndrome (e.g., increased
by caregivers within the home with very young children          likelihood of engaging in avoidant and/or attention
with Down syndrome and may be particularly relevant             motivated behaviour) as well as events that occur on
with escape behaviour. For example, we have used token          an individualised basis. Additionally, as intervention-
systems consisting of cartoon character tokens (e.g., Bar-      ists become versed in the functional assessment process
neyTM, Blues CluesTM) with children as young as two years       and proactive interventions, they will find themselves
of age to sustain performance during tasks where escape         naturally implementing many of these strategies, likely
behaviour was typically displayed.                              resulting in an overall decrease in challenging behav-
With the development of empirically demonstrated                iours emitted by the children with Down syndrome with
behavioural intervention strategies, the systematic use of      whom they work. In turn, increasing the child’s likeli-
consequence strategies designed to decrease behaviour           hood of success in inclusive educational and community
(i.e., the use of punishment procedures) is likely unnec-       environments.
essary. The use of positive interventions has become so

                                             © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
                                                                               http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
Down Syndrome Research and Practice 11(2), 64-77
74
                                               K.M. Feeley and E.A. Jones • Challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome

     Future applications                                             might position toys so they are in reach, thus, pulling the
                                                                     toy closer can be more easily accomplished. Caregivers
     Recent research suggests that the presence of escape and        who are well versed in prompting strategies (e.g., the use
     attention motivated challenging behaviour may be part           of physical guidance or positioning of objects to enhance
     of the behavioural phenotype (Dykens, 1995; Fidler,             performance) as well as shaping procedures (i.e., rein-
     2005) that characterises Down syndrome. That is, the            forcing closer and closer approximations to the target
     disability itself is characterised by a pattern of strengths    response) can be advised to implement such strategies
     and weaknesses that increase the likelihood of valuing          to enhance their child’s performance with a given task.
     certain consequences (i.e., escape and attention). McGill       By using prompting and shaping procedures, adults are
     (1999) presented the idea that disabilities may function        setting up the situation to provide the young child with
     as motivating operations in his discussion of the decrease      Down syndrome with more successful opportunities and
     in the reinforcing value of attention in individuals with       then very gradually leading to more difficult tasks. This
     autism and the increase in the reinforcing value of food        is one application of errorless learning techniques which,
     in individuals with Prader-Willi. Although not specifi-          as suggested by Fidler (2005), may result in children with
     cally mentioned by McGill, the work of Wishart and col-         Down syndrome persisting in tasks during which they
     leagues (Wishart, 1993a; 1993b; Pitcairn & Wishart,             might otherwise demonstrate escape behaviour.
     1994; Wishart & Duffy, 1990), as well as the cluster of          With respect to the attention seeking functions of chal-
     behaviours termed a behavioural phenotype, suggest              lenging behaviour, our clinical experience suggests that,
     Down syndrome should be included in the disabilities            during the toddler and preschool years, attention moti-
     identified by McGill as motivating operations.                   vated behaviours are present and particularly interfer-
     The presence of these challenging behaviours, even within       ing. Consider, for example, getting into a forbidden area
     the first year of life in children with Down syndrome,           within the home (e.g., brick fire place, steep stair case,
     effectively reduces the child’s learning interactions with       bay window), a common behaviour in toddlers. Many
     his/her environment. As part of the behavioural pheno-          caregivers might instinctively go to the child and remove
     type characteristic of Down syndrome, we are challenged         him/her from the area, but, in doing so, inadvertently
     to develop strategies to ameliorate these tendencies at a       deliver attention in the form of verbal feedback (e.g.,
     very young age (Fidler, 2005), by first identifying the          “No, no honey, that can be dangerous.”) as well as physi-
     specific behaviours in which a child engages and then            cal contact (e.g., once the parent has the child in his/her
     teaching caregivers to respond in such a way that these         arms, hugs and kisses are naturally delivered). Because
     behaviours are not systematically reinforced (Gerenser,         of the positive interaction that ultimately takes place, the
     personal communication, June 6, 2004). For example,             child may learn that an effective way to get attention is
     caregivers often make simple requests of a child that are       to move toward the forbidden area. Alternatively, car-
     followed by avoidance behaviour (e.g., turning away).           egivers can systematically respond in a way that is not
     These avoidance behaviours are likely to result in some         reinforcing to the child (e.g., silently go to the child with-
     initial persistence on the part of the caregiver, but, in       out providing eye contact and remove the child from the
     the face of further child resistance, caregivers may ‘give      forbidden area) while at the same time responding in a
     up’ and discontinue the request. In these situations, the       very reinforcing manner when the child is playing in an
     child, who did not want to comply with the request, is          acceptable location (e.g., while sitting on the floor with
     ultimately reinforced; that is, avoidance (e.g., turning        their toys).
     away) to respond results in the removal of the demand.          Given the early presence of these challenging behaviours,
     Alternatively, caregivers can be taught to follow through       it is essential for professionals to involve caregivers during
     with even simple requests. Therefore, when making a             the development and implementation of interventions so
     request, if the child does not respond, he/she is prompted      that caregivers can effectively intervene throughout the
     to do so. Once the response occurs, even if prompted,           day and across multiple environments. Professionals
     reinforcement is immediately delivered. This results            should be skilled at imparting intervention techniques to
     in teaching the child, from an early age, the benefits of        caregivers so they can comfortably implement interven-
     following simple instructions and possibly curtailing a         tions during ongoing interactions with their young child
     pervasive pattern of opting out as identified by Wishart         with Down syndrome.
     (1993a, 1993b).
     Another way to address escape behaviour in young chil-
     dren with Down syndrome, especially in the presence of
     slightly more difficult tasks, may be to teach caregivers to
     manipulate items in the child’s environment to make spe-
     cific tasks less difficult. For example, if the child is having
     difficulty accessing toys in his environment, the parent

     © 2006 The Down Syndrome Educational Trust. All Rights Reserved. ISSN: 0968-7912
     http://information.downsed.org/dsrp/11/02
You can also read