Factors Influencing Rumour Re-Spreading in a Public Health Crisis by the Middle-Aged and Elderly Populations - MDPI

Page created by Dennis Watson
 
CONTINUE READING
Factors Influencing Rumour Re-Spreading in a Public Health Crisis by the Middle-Aged and Elderly Populations - MDPI
International Journal of
 Environmental Research
 and Public Health

Article
Factors Influencing Rumour Re-Spreading
in a Public Health Crisis by the Middle-Aged
and Elderly Populations
Zhonggen Sun 1 , Xin Cheng 1 , Ruilian Zhang 2, * and Bingqing Yang 1
 1 School of Public Administration, Hohai University, Nanjing 211100, Jiangsu, China;
 sunzhonggen@hhu.edu.cn (Z.S.); 1707020105@hhu.edu.cn (X.C.); yangbingqing@hhu.edu.cn (B.Y.)
 2 Sustainable Minerals Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane 4072, Australia
 * Correspondence: ruilian.zhang@uq.edu.au; Tel.: +61-481120982
 
 Received: 10 July 2020; Accepted: 3 September 2020; Published: 8 September 2020 

 Abstract: Due to discrimination and media literacy, middle-aged and elderly individuals have been
 easily reduced to marginalized groups in the identification of rumours during a public health crisis and
 can easily spread rumours repeatedly, which has a negative impact on pandemic prevention and social
 psychology. To further clarify the factors influencing their behaviours, this study used a questionnaire
 to survey a sample of 556 individuals in China and used multiple linear regression and analysis of
 variance to explore influencing factors during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic.
 We found that, first, in the COVID-19 pandemic, middle-aged and elderly adults’ willingness to
 re-spread rumours is positively related to their degree of believing rumours and to personal anxiety
 and is negatively related to their rumour-discrimination ability and to their perception of serious
 consequences to rumour spreading. Second, the degree of believing rumours plays an intermediary
 role in the willingness to re-spread rumours. It plays a partial mediating role in the path of anxiety’s
 influence on behaviour, suggesting that an anxious person will spread a rumour even if he or she does
 not have a strong belief in the rumour. Third, interpersonal communication has a greater credibility
 and a greater willingness to re-spread than does mass communication. This suggests the importance
 of increasing public knowledge expertise and of reducing public panic. This also has important
 implications for the future design of public health policies.

 Keywords: COVID-19; middle-aged and elderly group; rumours spreading; influencing factors;
 social impacts

1. Introduction

1.1. Background
 Middle-aged and elderly groups, defined as people over 46 years old are a very important part
of communication with respect to the process of spreading rumours [1,2]. This is because, first,
they account for 41.38% of the current total population in China according to the 2019 statistical
yearbook [3]. Second, older people have much social experience, and middle-aged people have
a higher family status in China [4]; therefore, other age groups tend to listen to both groups. Third,
facing the flood of pandemic-related information, middle-aged and elderly groups who have low
media literacy and relatively weak cultural qualities have low rumour-discrimination ability [5–8],
which leads to being more susceptible to accepting rumours and to spreading rumours. Media literacy
refers to people’s ability to choose, comprehend, question, evaluate, create, produce, and respond
critically when confronted with information in various media [9]. Cultural qualities are developed
through education in the humanities and arts [10], which is directly proportional to educational

Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542; doi:10.3390/ijerph17186542 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 2 of 14

level, and rumour-discrimination ability refers to the ability to distinguish between right and wrong
information. For example, due to the nuclear leak in Japan, there were rumours that the seawater
contamination had led to salt shortage in China, which caused a salt scramble in 2011. According to
an analysis by the public media, the middle-aged and elderly populations played a leading role in the
spread of rumours and were also the main victims [7]. During coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19),
some widely circulated but unconfirmed news on social media, such as that salt brine mouthwash and
smoked vinegar could prevent viruses, were eventually confirmed as rumours by public media and
experts. According to a research in the US, people aged 45–65 share fake news three times as much as
those aged 18–29 and those over 65 share fake news seven times as much as those aged 18–29 [11].
A study shows that, in the context of the increasingly sound development of new media functions,
the elderly population is enjoying short videos spread through WeChat (a chat software of Shenzhen
Tencent Computer Systems Company Limited, Shenzhen, China), the authenticity of which cannot be
verified. These contents were often identified as rumours [12].
 Fonzo and Bordia point out that the process of spreading rumours usually goes through three
stages: generation, evaluation, and re-spreading [13]. The concept of rumour re-spreading is relative
to the initiation of the rumour. It emphasizes the role of the information audience in the dissemination
process. The focus is on its diffusion behaviour after one hears the rumour [14]. According to the
existing literature, there are many variables affecting re-spreading, and the main research covers the
following four aspects: the information itself, the disseminator, the individual audience, and the social
environment [7,15,16]. Allport and Postman note that the importance and ambiguity of information
will contribute to the spread of rumours [17]. Chours argues that, in addition to the importance and
ambiguity of information that will cause the spread of rumours, individual intelligence, the level
of knowledge, and moral values will also have an impact on the propagation of rumours [18].
In addition, opinion leaders [19], communication channels [20], and trust in the rumour [21] can also
influence re-spreading of the rumours. There are many academic studies on the factors influencing
rumour propagation. However, there is little research on the relationship between anxiety, rumour
discrimination, opinion leaders, and trust in the process of rumour re-spreading as the influencing
factors [14].
 In terms of subjects, the research on rumours of public health events in the existing literature
mainly focuses on aspects of food safety [22] and sudden occurrence [23]. The existing research groups
span across all ages, and there are fewer studies on elderly adults [24]. In addition, the existing research
on the influence of rumour re-spreading of various public health events has different factors [7].
 On 8 December 2019, the first case of a COVID-19 infection was found in Wuhan, Hubei Province.
Subsequently, COVID-19, which is a communicable disease, has spread rapidly throughout Wuhan
and other cities in China. The pandemic is rampant, and it is difficult for people to discern truth and
rumours in a timely and accurate manner under conditions of panic and anxiety [25]. The rumour
that “Shuanghuanglian oral liquid (a famous Chinese traditional medicine, composed of honeysuckle,
scutellarin, and forsythia and an antidote with the functions of relieving fever, cough, and sore throat)
can inhibit COVID-19” has caused citizens across the country to quickly gather in local pharmacies
to stock up on drugs [26]. The spread of rumours related to the COVID-19 pandemic has not only
exacerbated the possibility of the pandemic but also has stimulated public panic and caused chaos
in social order [12].
 In this article, the middle-aged and elderly groups are used as the research object to help the
public understand the rumour re-spreading behaviour of this group, which addresses the gap in the
literature on rumour re-spreading in middle-aged and older populations. Meanwhile, in the process of
rumour re-spreading, existing studies have found that anxiety level has an influential role in rumour
propagation. However, the specific role of anxiety level in rumour propagation has not been discussed.
Therefore, using the brine mouthwash rumour spread in COVID-19 as an example, we found that
anxiety level influences rumour re-spreading behaviour in part through the degree of belief in the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 3 of 14

rumour. These findings complement research on the factors influencing rumour re-spreading and have
important implications for the future design of public health policies.

1.2. Hypothesis Development
 This paper’s theoretical foundations are rumour propagation theory and information
diffusion theory.
 Rumour propagation theory was first proposed by Allport and Postman and argues that rumours
are the result of a combination of event importance and event ambiguity [17]. When the environment
and atmosphere are full of anxiety and uncertainty, it will increase anxiety and rumours are more likely
to spread. Chours subsequently revised the theory, arguing that the spread of rumour is related to
event importance plus event ambiguity and to public critical thinking of crowds [18]. Public critical
thinking is also an important factor that affects the spread of rumours, consisting of an individual’s
intelligence, relevant level of knowledge, and moral values. Public critical thinking represents the
insight into social events that comes from people’s intelligence or other relevant factors. Differences in
personalities have a very different effect on the spread of rumours, which means that different moral
values will affect the possibility that people believe and spread rumours.
 Information diffusion theory suggests that the spread of most information is in the shape of an “S”
curve. In the process of diffusion, early adopters provide necessary help for the subsequent popularity.
These early adopters convince opinion leaders in their communities, often through interpersonal
communication. The opinion leaders then spread their influence to audiences in their interpersonal
communication sphere to allow more people to receive the information [27,28]. Moreover, information
diffusion always takes place with the help of certain social networks. In the process of diffusion of
information to the society, public communication through technology is effective in providing relevant
knowledge and information. However, interpersonal communication is more direct and effective
in persuading people to accept new information [29].
 Based on Chours’s study about critical thinking of crowds, the higher the individual’s intelligence
and relevant knowledge is, the less likely the rumour is to spread; the better the moral accomplishment
is, the less likely the rumour is to spread. In this paper, the ability to discern rumours is used to
measure the individual’s intelligence and knowledge, and the perception of serious consequences of
spreading rumours is used to measure the moral values.
 Previous research has found that, in the face of rumours, the audience’s cognitive ability and
emotional factors will have an impact on redistribution. In particular, anxiety is one of the most
important manifestations of emotional factors [30]. In early experiments, Anthony showed that anxiety
is directly proportional to the number of rumours [31], and in later empirical studies, Onook et al.
also suggested that audience anxiety positively affects their willingness to re-spread [32–34]. Therefore,
the following assumptions are made:
H1: The degree of anxiety of the subjects is significantly positively related to their willingness to re-spread rumours.
 The ability of rumour discrimination is to some extent an innovative concept in this paper.
People’s behaviour relies on their own ability to think and judge, and a weaker ability to discern
rumours limits this ability to some extent [5]. In contrast, when an individual has enough relevant
knowledge, such as if a person knows the origin of the virus and has protection knowledge, then they
are less likely to spread rumours. Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:
H2: Subjects’ ability to discern other rumours is significantly negatively related to their willingness to
re-spread rumours.
 People’s behaviour is regulated by social norms and their own values [35]. If a person of high
moral cultivation knows that what he or she hears is a rumour, then he or she will understand that
the consequences of spreading may have bad consequences for the followers [21], such as causing
physical harm, sending the followers into panic, etc., and the perception of the severity of such
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 4 of 14

consequences will prevent them from spreading the rumour. A related study also corroborated the
idea that moral judgments are conducive to rumour-confrontation behaviour [36]. Thus, we propose
the following hypothesis:
H3: The participants’ perceptions of the serious consequences of spreading rumours are significantly negatively
related to their willingness to re-spread rumours.
 The news value is also known as the heat brought about by the spread of information.
Opinion leaders can also increase news value by increasing the heat of the discussion [37].
Opinion leaders are influential and active people in the process of information spreading and
interpersonal interaction [17]. In information dissemination, they can be nongovernment organizations,
experts and scholars with a certain voice, or personal media accounts with a certain number of
fans. To some extent, opinion leaders are more knowledgeable, talented, credible, and virtuous.
According to the information diffusion model, we think opinion leaders can facilitate the re-spreading
of rumours because they have higher levels of trustworthiness [28]. Therefore, we propose the
following hypothesis:
H4: Having an opinion leader or not has significant differences on participants’ willingness to re-spread rumours.
 It has been shown that, when people believe rumours, they instinctively spread the topics they
believe in. The more people believe a rumour, the more frequently they spread the rumour [38].
Hua [33] argued that the credibility of Internet rumours positively affects the audience’s willingness to
redistribute rumours. In addition, people’s perceptions can have a driving effect on their behaviour.
Trust is a person’s perception of things or opinions, which will have a propulsive effect in the course of
action. In the process of rumour propagation, Liu argued that the degree of user trustworthiness plays
a mediating effect in the influence of network density on the willingness to propagate rumours [22].
Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:
H5: The degree to which subjects believe rumours is significantly positively related to their willingness to spread
the rumours.
H6: Among the above factors that significantly affect participants’ willingness to spread rumours, the degree to
which they believe rumours plays a mediating role.
 Social network theory can infer that mass communication facilitates the spread of rumours,
but interpersonal communication makes people believe them more. The former gets information
from traditional media such as television, newspapers, or new media such as the Internet, and the
latter gets information from familiar family and friends. Some studies have suggested that intimate
social relationships have a positive effect on the willingness to spread rumours [16,20]. This research
proposes the following hypothesis:
H7: The communication channels have significant differences on participants’ degree of approval of rumours and
their willingness to re-spread rumours.
 The overall conceptual model is shown in Figure 1.
gets information from familiar family and friends. Some studies have suggested that intimate social
 relationships have a positive effect on the willingness to spread rumours [16,20]. This research
 proposes the following hypothesis:
 • H7: The communication channels have significant differences on participants’ degree of
 approval of rumours and their willingness to re-spread rumours.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 5 of 14
 The overall conceptual model is shown in Figure 1.

 Figure
 Figure 1.
 1. Conceptual
 Conceptualmodel.
 model.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data Source

2.1.1. Subject Selection
 Most international and domestic studies use 45 years of age as the dividing line between young
individuals and middle-aged individuals. Thus, this article conceptualized the middle-aged and
elderly populations as those aged 46 years and older as the research sample; it was also ensured that
the subjects had not heard of any information about refuting the rumours the utilized to make sure
they did not know that the rumours were fake news. The article designed a questionnaire around
rumours during the COVID-19 pandemic.
 The questionnaire was based on the rumour that “brine mouthwash can prevent COVID-19”
to test the degree to which participants in different middle-aged and elderly groups accepted the
rumours and were willing to re-spread them. This rumour was selected as the case study based on the
consideration that it is simple to operate and easier to implement because saltwater is readily available
and thus has a herd effect. At first, the rumour was found on nearly every social media. Actually,
even though brine mouthwash is a traditional and a common-sense disinfectant in Eastern culture,
according to the WHO, there is no evidence that brine mouthwash can prevent COVID-19 and washing
the mouth is not related to respiratory infections.
 Personal anxiety was composed of three indicators: “the possibility of the virus infecting you
and the people around you”, “the degree of distrust in virus prevention and control”, and “the degree
of personal panic”. Rumour discrimination ability was given by seven uncertain terms related to
knowledge of pandemic protection, measured by participants’ correct scores. A Likert scale was used to
score the severity of the consequences of the spread of rumours and was subjectively measured by the
participants. The influence of opinion leaders was measured by indicating in some questionnaires that
the source of information was a certified “Dr. Returnee”, while in others, it was not clearly indicated.
The level of belief and willingness to spread rumours was based on a Likert scale, with larger values
indicating higher levels of belief or willingness to spread rumours.

2.1.2. Questionnaire
 On 18 February 2020, with the help of an Internet professional platform called “Questionnaire
Star” (Ranxing Information Technology Company, Changsha, China), the questionnaire was created
and geared towards middle-aged and older people over 46 years of age.
 With the help of WeChat (chat software used by over 1.1 billion people in China) chat groups,
we could quickly forward and recall questionnaires. Data collection of the questionnaire survey
lasted for 3 days. A filtered question was set at the beginning of the questionnaire, which meant
that the questionnaire was aimed at people who had not heard the brine mouthwash rumour.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 6 of 14

Only 562 respondents were eligible, and 556 questionnaires remained after eliminating duplicates,
with an effective rate of 98.93%.
 The questionnaire was first administered in Wuhan; then, its use spread all over China, which meant
that the data collected from Wuhan was supplemented by information from other provinces of China.
Wuhan had the earliest outbreak of COVID-19 and the largest number of infected people. Wuhan is
the capital of Hubei province, located in central China (Figure 2), with an urban resident population of
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, x 6 of 13
9.06 million [39].

 Figure 2. Location of Wuhan, Hubei Province.
 Figure 2. Location of Wuhan, Hubei Province.
 Among the valid questionnaires recovered, 381 individuals were located in Wuhan City,
Hubei Among the valid
 Province, questionnaires
 accounting recovered,
 for 68.5% of the 381 individuals
 total wereand
 sample size, located
 the in Wuhan
 other City, Hubei
 approximately
 Province, accounting for 68.5% of the total sample size, and the other approximately 100
100 questionnaires were from other provinces. According to the statistics of the pandemic situation on
 questionnaires
21 February 2020,werethefrom other provinces.
 cumulative number ofAccording to thein
 people infected statistics
 Wuhan of theHubei
 City, pandemic situation
 Province on 21
 accounted
 February 2020, the cumulative number of people infected in Wuhan City, Hubei Province accounted
for 79.1% of the total infected population, with fewer cases in other provinces. The proportion of the
 for 79.1% of the total infected population, with fewer cases in other provinces. The proportion of the
sample in Hubei Province in the valid questionnaire was close to the proportion of the population
 sample in Hubei Province in the valid questionnaire was close to the proportion of the population in
in Hubei Province with COVID-19 infection; thus, the sample is representative.
 Hubei Province with COVID-19 infection; thus, the sample is representative.
2.2. Methods
 2.2. Methods
 This study used the socioeconomic statistical analysis tool SPSS22.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA)
 This study
for analysis. Theused
 linearthe socioeconomic
 regression method statistical
 was used analysis toolthe
 to analyse SPSS22.0 (SPSS
 significant Inc., Chicago,
 influencing IL, in
 factors USA)
 the
 for analysis. The linear regression method was used to analyse the significant influencing
model under the influence of multiple factors. The hierarchical regression method was used to detect factors in
 the model under the influence of multiple factors. The hierarchical regression method
the mediating effect of key factors, which was proposed by Baron and Kenny in 1986 [40] and is often was used to
 detectinthe
used themediating effect of key
 study of influence factors, which was proposed by Baron and Kenny in 1986 [40] and
 factors.
 is often
 This study used personal anxiety, factors.
 used in the study of influence rumour discrimination, perception of the serious consequences
 This study
of spreading usedand
 rumours, personal anxiety,
 the presence of anrumour
 opinion discrimination, perception
 leader as independent of the
 variables. The serious
 degree
 consequences of spreading rumours, and the presence of an opinion leader as
of believing rumours was used as a mediator, and willingness to re-spread rumours was used as independent variables.
aThe degree ofvariable
 dependent believing to rumours was used model.
 build a conceptual as a mediator, and willingness to re-spread rumours was
 used as a dependent variable to build a conceptual model.
 The logistics models were established as follows:
 Model 1: M= + + ⋯ + + (1)
 Model 2: Y= + + ⋯ + + (2)
 Model 3: Y= + + ⋯ + + + (3)
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 7 of 14

 The logistics models were established as follows:

 Model 1 : M = α1 + β1 X1 + · · · + βk Xi + ε1 (1)

 Model 2 : Y = α2 + β2 X1 + · · · + β2k Xi + ε2 (2)

 Model 3 : Y = α3 + β3 X1 + · · · + β3k Xi + δM + ε3 (3)

where Y is the dependent variable; Xi (i = 1, . . . . . . , k) is the model’s independent variable; M is the
mediator; αi , βi , γi (i = 1, 2, 3), and δ are the parameters to be estimated; and εi (i = 1, 2, 3) is the
random perturbation term.
 First, we conducted a regression of X on M, testing the significance of the regression coefficient β1i .
Second, we conducted a regression of X on Y, testing the significance of the regression coefficient β2i .
Third, we conducted a regression of X and M on Y, testing the significance of the regression coefficients
β3i and δ. If the coefficients β1i , β2i , and δ are all significant, it indicates the presence of an intermediate
effect. If the coefficient β3i is not significant, then the mediation effect is full; if the regression coefficient
β3i is significant, but β3i < β1i , then the mediation effect is partial.
 To further clarify the differences in the influence of communication channels, the degree of belief
in rumours and the willingness to spread rumours were set as the dependent variables, and channels
were set as independent variables to build model 4. This study detected the effect of three dummy
variables by means of variance analysis.

2.3. Ethical Approval
 For this study, consent of the University Behavioral and Social Sciences Ethical Review Committee
to which the researcher belongs was obtained (approval number: School of Public Administration
number 20200202). All respondents were given information about the aim of the study, that the data
would be treated as strictly confidential, and that all answers would be anonymous.

3. Results

3.1. Variable Descriptive Statistics

3.1.1. Sample Population Attributes
 As shown in Table 1, women accounted for 57.73% of the sample, men accounted for 42.27%,
individuals aged 46–55 years old accounted for 57.37%, those aged 56–65 years old accounted for
24.46%, and those aged 76 years old and older accounted for 3.96%.
 More than half of the participants had a bachelor’s degree or above. Considering that the outbreak
occurred in the city, the questionnaire was distributed to the urban areas of Wuhan, which has
84 universities, is a major education city, and contains the top three education levels in China in 2019,
with a high proportion of urban residents having an undergraduate-level education.

 Table 1. Descriptive statistics of sample population attributes.

 Variable Content Composition Percentage (%)
 Male 42.27
 Gender
 Female 57.73
 46–55 years old 57.37
 56–65 years old 24.46
 Age
 66–75 years old 14.21
 76 years old and above 3.96
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 8 of 14

 Table 1. Cont.

 Variable Content Composition Percentage (%)
 Uneducated 2.70
 Primary school 7.19
 Junior high school 16.73
 Education level High school and technical
 20.50
 Secondary school
 Bachelor’s degree and college 45.86
 Graduate school and above 7.01

3.1.2. Measurement of Personal Anxiety
 Anxiety levels in the questionnaire were measured on a Likert scale, with 1 indicating no anxiety
at all, 5 indicating being very anxious, and higher scores indicating more panic. Overall, the subjects
were optimistic about the pandemic, had full confidence in virus control, and had low levels of panic
(Table 2).

 Table 2. Sample anxiety measurement results.

 Index Index Content Mean
 Possibility of one’s own infection and
 2.414
 that of people in his/her environment
 Personal anxiety Lack of confidence in virus control 1.529
 Individual panic levels 2.369
 Sum of overall panic 6.311
 Cronbach’s α value 0.773

3.1.3. Rumour Discrimination Ability
 From Table 3, we find that most of the middle-aged and elderly respondents have basic protective
knowledge of topics such as window ventilation and wearing masks but that only 13.13% of
the participants chose all the correct answers. Of the participants, 16.01% and 8.09% believe that
probiotics can improve their immunity to COVID-19 and that Shuanghuanglian oral liquid can prevent
COVID-19, respectively.

 Table 3. Rumour discrimination results.

 Index Option Content Correct Rate (%)
 Need to open windows regularly to keep air flowing (right) 94.42
 Wear a mask when going out (right) 98.38
 COVID-19 can be eliminated within 30 min at 56 ◦ C (right) 62.59
 Rumour discrimination UV disinfection lamps can kill COVID-19 (right) 41.01
 Central air conditioning will promote COVID-19 infection (right) 40.83
 Probiotics can boost immunity against COVID-19 (wrong) 83.99
 Shuanghuanglian oral liquid can prevent COVID-19 (wrong) 91.91

3.1.4. Perceived Severity of the Consequences of Rumour Spreading
 A total of 74.82% of the middle-aged and elderly people respondents negatively evaluated the
consequences of the spread of rumours and think that the consequences are serious or very serious.
Only 2.52% of the respondents think that the spread of rumours does not matter at all (Table 4).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 9 of 14

 Table 4. The results of the measurement of the perceived severity of rumour spreading.

 Index Content Composition Percentage (%)
 Never mind 2.52
 The consequences are not serious 6.29
 Perceived severity of the consequences of
 Uncertain 16.37
 rumour spreading
 The consequences are serious 50.36
 The consequences are very serious 24.46

3.2. Correlation Between Major Variables
 The correlation coefficients between the willingness to re-spread rumours, the degree of belief
in rumours, personal anxiety, rumour discrimination, the perception of the severity of the consequences
of rumour spreading, and opinion leader were all significant at the 0.05 level. Among them,
there is a positive correlation between the willingness to re-spread rumours, the degree of belief
in rumours, and personal anxiety. The willingness to re-spread rumours is negatively related to
rumour discrimination and severity perception and positively related to opinion leaders. Among the
relationships, the most obvious is the relationship between the willingness to re-spread rumours and
the degree of belief in rumours, with the value reaching 0.743. Therefore, H1, H2, H3, and H4 are valid
(Table 5).

 Table 5. Pearson correlation coefficient table.
 Perceived Severity The Presence Degree of Willingness to
 Average Personal Rumour
 Variable Std. of Rumour of an Opinion Believing re-Spread
 Value Anxiety Discrimination
 Spreading Leader Rumours Rumours
 Personal anxiety 6.311 2.105 1
 Rumour
 3.131 1.154 0.023 1
 discrimination
 Perceived severity of
 3.879 0.934 −0.042 0.068 * 1
 rumour spreading
 The presence of an
 0.500 0.500 −0.000 −0.000 0.000 1
 opinion leader
 Degree of believing
 2.310 1.070 0.158 ** −0.069 * −0.088 ** 0.135 ** 1
 rumours
 Willingness to
 1.971 1.056 0.177 ** −0.063 * −0.097 ** 0.097 ** 0.743 ** 1
 re-spread rumours

 * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

3.3. Factors Influencing Rumour Re-Spreading
 First, a linear regression was performed using personal anxiety level, rumour discrimination,
perceived severity of the consequences of rumour spreading, and the presence of an opinion leader
as the independent variables and the degree of believing rumours as the dependent variable to
construct model 1. Model 1 illustrates that the subject’s personal anxiety (t = 5.346, p = 0.000 < 0.01),
rumour discrimination (t = −2.292, p = 0.022 < 0.05), perceived severity of the consequences of rumour
transmission (t = −2.610, p = 0.009 < 0.01), and the presence or absence of opinion leaders in the
message (t = 4.632, p = 0.000 < 0.01) all have a significant effects on the degree of believing rumours.
 Then, model 2 was developed by using willingness to re-spread as the dependent variable and by
using personal anxiety level, rumour discrimination, perceived severity of the consequences of rumour
spreading, and the presence of an opinion leader as the independent variables. The results of the
regression analysis of model 2 show that the subjects’ personal anxiety level (t = 5.980, p = 0.000 < 0.01),
rumour discrimination (t = −2.100, p = 0.036 < 0.05), perceived severity of the consequences (t = −2.926,
p = 0.004 < 0.01), and the presence of opinion leaders (t = 3.320, p = 0.001 < 0.01) all significantly affect
rumour re-spreading behaviour.
 The results of models 1 and 2 show that personal anxiety level and the presence of opinion leaders
can have a significant positive impact on the degree of belief in rumours and willingness to re-spread
rumours. The ability to discern rumours and the perceived severity of the consequences of rumour
spreading have a significant negative impact on the degree of belief in rumours and willingness to
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 10 of 14

re-spread rumours. That is, more anxious people and those who receive information from opinion
leaders are more likely to spread rumours, and the higher the rumour discernment and the clearer the
perception of consequences are, the lower the likelihood that a moral person believes the rumour and
spreads it.
 Model 3 adds the degree of rumour belief to model 2, and this independent variable exhibits
a particularly significant effect (t = 35.481, p = 0.000 < 0.01). The change in F-value shows significance
(p < 0.01), implying that the addition of rumour level has an explanatory significance for the model.
Second, the variables of rumour discrimination, the perceived severity of the consequences of rumour
spreading, and the presence or absence of opinion leaders, which were originally significant, are no
longer significant; third, individual anxiety level still plays a significant role but with reduced
explanatory power; and fourth, the adjusted R-squared increases from 0.067 to 0.596, implying that the
degree of rumour belief can generate 52.9% of the explanatory power for the willingness to re-spread
rumours. The above four points indicate that the degree of belief in rumours plays a mediating role
in the process of the independent variables influencing rumour re-spreading willingness (Table 6).
Therefore, H5 and H6 are supported.

 Table 6. Hierarchical regression analysis results (n = 556).
 Degree of Believing Rumours Willingness to Re-Spread Rumours Willingness to Spread Rumours
 (Model 1) (Model 2) (Model 3)
 Variable
 Std. Std. Std.
 B t p B t p B t p
 Error Error Error
 Constant 2.199 ** 0.185 11.905 0.000 1.867 ** 0.182 10.236 0.000 0.283 * 0.133 2.132 0.033
 Anxiety level 0.080 ** 0.015 5.346 0.000 0.088 ** 0.015 5.980 0.000 0.031 ** 0.010 3.000 0.003
 Rumour discrimination −0.062 * 0.027 −2.292 0.022 −0.056 * 0.027 −2.100 0.036 −0.011 0.018 −0.624 0.533
 Perceived severity of
 −0.088 ** 0.034 −2.610 0.009 −0.097 ** 0.033 −2.926 0.004 −0.034 0.023 −1.489 0.137
 rumour spreading
 The presence of an opinion
 0.290 ** 0.063 4.632 0.000 0.205 ** 0.062 3.320 0.001 −0.004 0.043 −0.086 0.932
 leader
 Degree of believing
 0.721 ** 0.020 35.481 0.000
 rumours
 R2 0.054 0.071 0.619
 Adjust R2 0.052 0.067 0.596
 F value F(5550) = 15.914, p = 0.000 F (5550) = 20.992, p = 0.000 F (6549) = 278.129, p = 0.000

 * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01.

3.4. Impact of Communication Channels on the Degree of Believing Rumours and the Willingness to
Re-spread Rumours
 The channel of spread was used as the independent variable, and the degree of believing rumours
and the willingness to re-spread rumours were analysed as the dependent variables. The kurtosis and
skewness of the model were overall normally distributed. At this time, an analysis of variance was
used. After the F-test, the analysis of variance showed that the type of rumour was significant for both
the degree of believing rumours and the willingness to re-spread rumours (p < 0.01), which means that
the degree of believing rumours and the willingness to re-spread rumours differ among the samples
from different channels. Thus, H7 is supported (Table 7).

 Table 7. Analysis of variance results.

 Types of Rumours (Mean ± SD)
 Model 4 Skewness Kurtosis Mass Communication Interpersonal F p
 (n = 556) Communication (n = 556)
 Degree of believing rumours −0.127 −0.978 2.17 ± 1.00 2.85 ± 1.07 229.736 0.000 **
 Willingness to re-spread
 0.357 −1.244 1.87 ± 1.02 2.43 ± 1.21 138.396 0.000 **
 rumours
 ** p < 0.01.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 11 of 14

4. Discussion
 Based on the findings stated above, we argue that the following aspects are critical for us to
understand the impacts of COVID-19 rumour spread.
 Overall, during the COVID-19 pandemic, the influencing factors of middle-aged and elderly
people’s willingness to re-spread rumours includes their levels of personal anxiety, rumour
discrimination, perceived severity of the consequences, the presence of an opinion leader, and the
degree of believing rumours. That is, the more anxious people are, the weaker their ability to discern
rumours is. In addition, the less consideration they have for others, the more trust they will have
in rumours and the more willingly they will be to spread rumours [41]. This is because people’s
behaviours depend on their own ability to think and judge. First of all, their personal anxiety level
affects their thinking ability. When people are more anxious, it is harder for them to calmly think about
the truthfulness of information [13]. Second, opinion leaders are seen as being more knowledgeable,
talented, credible, and ethical than others. What they say influences the decisions of ordinary people,
leading to a “spiral of silence” (If they see that the views they agree with are widely popular, they will
actively participate in it and become bolder to speak out and spread) and crowd behaviour. Therefore,
the presence of opinion leaders directly affects people’s trust in rumours and their willingness to
re-spread them. Third, rumour discrimination allows people to improve their ability to identify
rumours and to limit their re-spreading. The severity perception of the consequences of a rumour
is based on moral values, which allows the recipients of the rumours to consider the perspective of
others. People refrain from re-spreading rumours in order to reduce the harm they cause to others [36].
 The second aspect is the degree of belief in rumours, that is, how much a person believes the
rumours. The process of re-spreading rumours is a process of acceptance and retransmission, and the
subjective attitude of the recipient towards the rumour influences his or her behaviour. When people
believe rumours, they instinctively spread the topics they believe in [33]. With regard to the mediating
role of the degree of rumour belief [22], this paper innovatively finds that, in the process of anxiety
affecting their willingness to re-spread rumours, the degree of believing rumours plays a partially
mediated role. However, it plays a fully mediated role in the process of rumour discrimination, in the
severity perception of the consequences of rumour spreading, and in the opinion leaders’ influence on
the process path of their willingness to re-spread rumours. This conclusion can be explained by two
points. First, the subjects’ level of anxiety has a significant effect on their behaviour, either through
the level of rumour belief or directly. The latter means that it is not replaced by the role of mediating
variables and that the individual’s anxiety has an extremely important role in rumour re-spreading
behaviour. Therefore, rumour re-spreading behaviour is likely to occur even if the level of rumour
belief is not strong. Second, the effect of factors other than anxiety on rumour re-spreading behaviour
is achieved solely by increasing the individual’s trust in rumours. In other words, the above variables
cannot function if middle-aged and older adults do not believe the rumours. Suppression of rumours
by mitigating panic is suggested. Community and public health departments can proactively and
promptly disclose information about public health crises such as COVID-19 or can invite public health
experts to give public health talks. This can increase the public’s understanding of the expertise and
can reduce the public’s fears. Third, the moderating effect of communication channels is present.
Studies have shown that interpersonal communication has a stronger affinity and credibility than mass
communication [42]. On the one hand, the disseminator outperforms mass media in terms of network
density (the closeness of the relationship between individuals) and the strength of the relationship.
This increases the frequency of personal identification and communication, making the subjective level
of the message more credible [22]. On the other hand, the receiving party will have their own mixed
emotions and thus will be more willing to believe the other’s message.

5. Conclusions
 Middle-aged and older people demonstrate weakness in the areas of discrimination and media
literacy and therefore easily become marginalized groups in the identification of rumours during
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 12 of 14

pandemics. To understand the factors influencing middle-aged and elderly people regarding the
re-spreading of rumours about COVID-19 and to clarify the influencing mechanism of different factors,
we found the following after using multiple linear regression and analysis of variance. (1) In the
COVID-19 pandemic, middle-aged and elderly adults’ willingness to re-spread rumours is positively
related to their degree of believing rumours and personal anxiety, negatively related to their rumour
discrimination ability and the perception of serious consequences, and also related to the presence
of opinion leaders. The presence of opinion leaders has a direct driving effect on the degree of
believing rumours and the willingness to re-spread them. (2) The degree of believing rumours plays
an intermediary role in the influence on the willingness to re-spread rumours. That is, individual
anxiety, rumour discrimination, perception of the severity of the consequences of rumour spreading,
and the role of opinion leaders in rumour re-spreading behaviour are all achieved by increasing
an individual’s trust in rumours. (3) The rumour channel is an important factor influencing the
degree of believing rumours and the willingness to re-spread them. This research addresses the gap
in the literature on rumour re-spreading in middle-aged and older populations and has important
implications for the future design of public health policies.
 The limitations of this study are as follows. First, we need more analysis of the willingness to
re-spread rumours among the middle-aged; second, the sample size surveyed was limited during the
rapid spread of the pandemic. It is hoped that, in subsequent relevant studies, the research on the
spread of rumours among middle-aged individuals can be improved based on an adequate sample size.

Author Contributions: Conceptualization, Z.S. and R.Z.; Methodology, X.C. and B.Y.; Supervision, Z.S. and R.Z.;
Writing—original draft, X.C.; Writing—review and editing, R.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Funding: This article is supported by the Jiangsu Provincial Social Science Fund (18SHB011) and Hohai University’s
Central University Basic Scientific Research Business Expenses Project Funding (2018B32614).
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
1. Wen, Y.; Zong, Z.H.; Shu, X.Y.; Zhou, J.F.; Sun, X.M.; Ru, X.M. Health status, health needs and provision of
 health services among the middle-aged and elderly people: Results from a survey in 12 counties, 5 provinces
 in mid-western China. Popul. Res. 2014, 38, 72–86.
2. Gai, L.T.; Chen, Y.H. research on the development of internet benefiting the elderly: Survey of internet
 behaviours basing on middle-aged and elderly internet users. modern communication. J. Commun.
 Univ. China 2017, 39, 77–81.
3. National Bureau of Statistics of the People’s Republic of China. China Statistical Year Book; China Statistics
 Press: Beijing, China, 2019.
4. Zhang, X.H.; Li, J.X. Modernization and the evolution of the family status of the elderly-an example of the
 economic decision-making power of intergenerational families living together. Popul. Econ. 2019, 4, 94–106.
5. Feng, Q.; Ma, Z. The dissemination of on-line rumours and social class differences: A study on the on-line
 food-safety rumours. J. Commun. Rev. 2019, 72, 28–41.
6. Lou, X.D.; Li, F. An Analysis of internet rumours in communication. Today’s Massmedia 2012, 20, 18–21.
7. Wu, J.; Sun, Y.; Li, H.B. Research on the spread of rumours in the elderly susceptible groups: Information,
 channels and audiences. J. Qingdao Univ. Sci. Technol. (Soc. Sci.) 2019, 35, 81–87.
8. Sun, Z.; Yang, B.; Zhang, R.; Cheng, X. Influencing factors of understanding COVID-19 risks and coping
 behaviors among the elderly population. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5889. [CrossRef]
 [PubMed]
9. Sibel, K. The role of critical media literacy in further development of consciousness of citizenship. Proc. Soc.
 Behav. Sci. 2015, 174, 3039–3043.
10. Yang, S.Z. The reawareness and re-departure of cultural quality education: Commemorating the 20th
 anniversary of cultural quality education in China. China High. Educ. Res. 2015, 6, 7–11.
11. Guess, A.; Nagler, J.; Tucker, J. Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on
 Facebook. Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, 4586. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 13 of 14

12. Zhang, C.Q. Analysis of the current status of online scientific rumors of novel coronavirus pneumonia-taking
 WeChat platform as an example. J. Tangshan Norm. Coll. 2020, 42, 116–119.
13. DiFonzo, N.; Prashant, B. How top PR professionals handle hearsay: Corporate rumors, their effects,
 and strategies to manage them. Publ. Relat. Rev. 2000, 26, 173–190. [CrossRef]
14. Lai, S.Q. Influencing Mechanism of the Online Rumours on Audiences’ Re-spreading Behaviour. J. Intell.
 2014, 5, 153–156.
15. Rosnow, R.L.; Esposito, J.L.; Gibney, L. Factors influencing rumour spreading: Replication and extension.
 Lang. Commun. 1988, 8, 29–42. [CrossRef]
16. Sussman, S.W.; Siegal, W.S. informational influence in organizations: An integrated approach to knowledge
 adoption. Inf. Syst. Res. 2000, 14, 47–65. [CrossRef]
17. Gordon, W.A.; Leo, P. An Analysis of Rumor. Public Opin. Q. 1946, 10, 501–517.
18. Chorus, A. The basic law of rumor. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1953, 48, 313–314. [CrossRef]
19. Liu, Y.M. On the relationship of interpersonal communication and advertising. J. Xinxiang Teach. Coll. 2007,
 8, 131–134.
20. Garret, R.K. Troubling consequences of online political rumouring. Hum. Commun. Res. 2011, 37, 55–74.
21. Rosnow, R.L.; Yost, J.H.; Esposito, J.L. Belief in rumor and likelihood of rumor transmission. Lang. Commun.
 1986, 6, 189–194. [CrossRef]
22. Liu, Y.S.; Xu, Y. Rumour and rumour denials in online social networks: Exploring the influence of structural
 factors and social psychological processes. J. Commun. 2016, 23, 51–69.
23. Chua, A.Y.K.; Tee, C.Y.; Pang, A.; Lim, E.P. The retransmission of rumor and rumor correction messages on
 Twitter. Am. Behav. Sci. 2018, 61, 707–723. [CrossRef]
24. Tang, S.H. Research on the Influencing Factors of Internet Rumor Re-spread Willingness. Master’s Thesis,
 Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China, 2015.
25. Zhu, L.; Guan, G. Dynamical analysis of a rumour spreading model with self-discrimination and time delay
 in complex networks. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Its Appl. 2019, 533, 63–71.
26. Zhang, T.Y. Research on the propagation mechanism of self-media rumors from the perspective of modal
 factors: Taking the "Shuanghuanglian" incident as an example. Audiovisual 2020, 7, 140–141.
27. Euane, Y.; Rand, W.; Eftekhar, M.; Rabinovich, E. Evaluating information diffusion speed and its determinants
 in social media networks during humanitarian crises. J. Operat. Manag. 2016, 45, 123–133.
28. Bhattacharya, D.; Ram, S. Sharing News Articles Using 140 Characters: A Diffusion Analysis on Twitter.
 In Proceedings of the 2012 IEEE/ACM International Conference on Advances in Social Networks Analysis
 and Mining (ASONAM), Istanbul, Turkey, 26–29 August 2012; pp. 966–971.
29. Krackhardt, D. The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations. Netw. Organ. Struct.
 Form Act. 1992, 216, 239.
30. Rosnow, R. Psychology of rumour reconsidered. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 87, 578–591. [CrossRef]
31. Anthony, S. Anxiety and rumor. J. Soc. Psychol. 1973, 89, 91–98. [CrossRef]
32. Oh, O.; Agrawal, M.; Rao, H.R. Community intelligence and social media services: A rumour theoretic
 analysis of tweets during social crises. MIS Q 2013, 37, 407–426. [CrossRef]
33. Hua, S. Research on the Influence Factors of Internet Rumour Spread—Take the Microblog Rumour as
 an Example. Master’s Thesis, Zhejiang Normal University of China, Jinhua, China, 2013.
34. Li, B.M. The Influence of Rumor Credibility and Anxiety Level on Rumor Propagation. Master’s Thesis,
 Qufu Normal University, Qufu, China, 2013.
35. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Attitudinal and normative variables as predictors of specific behaviors. J. Personal.
 Soc. Psychol. 1973, 27, 41–57. [CrossRef]
36. Kohlberg, L. The Philosophy of Moral Development; Harper & Row: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 415–418.
37. Wu, J.; Ma, C. The Rumor Propagation Formula: Traceability, Revision and Development. J. Mass. Commun. Q.
 2015, 13, 20–23.
38. Jaeger, M.E.; Anthony, S.; Rosnow, R.L. Who hears what from whom and with what effect: A study of rumor.
 Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 6, 473–478. [CrossRef]
39. National Bureau of Statistics of Wuhan City. Wuhan Statistical Yearbook; Hubei Province Statistics Press:
 Wuhan, China, 2019.
40. Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research:
 Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6542 14 of 14

41. Hu, Y.; Pan, Q.; Hou, W.; He, M. Rumour spreading model with the different attitudes towards rumours.
 Phys. A Stat. Mech. Its Appl. 2018, 502, 331–344. [CrossRef]
42. Zhang, Z.X. The Communication Analysis of “Zhang Wuben Phenomenon”. Today’s Massmedia 2010, 18,
 76–77.

 © 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
 article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
 (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
You can also read