Interpopulation variation in sexual dichromatism in the Neotropical grasshopper Sphenarium purpurascens (Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae)

Page created by Doris Mcguire
 
CONTINUE READING
Interpopulation variation in sexual dichromatism in the Neotropical grasshopper Sphenarium purpurascens (Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae)
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911. With 4 figures.

Interpopulation variation in sexual dichromatism in
the Neotropical grasshopper Sphenarium purpurascens
(Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae)
RAÚL CUEVA DEL CASTILLO*, , MIGUEL GONZÁLEZ-ZERTUCHE and
VÍCTOR HUGO RAMÍREZ-DELGADO

                                                                                                                               Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
UBIPRO, Laboratorio de Ecología, FES Iztacala, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, A.P. 314,
Tlalnepantla, Estatado de México, CP 54090, Mexico

Received 6 October 2020; revised 15 December 2020; accepted for publication 16 December 2020

Cryptic coloration is an adaptative defensive mechanism against predators. Colour patterns appear cryptic through
general background coloration matching or disruptive coloration. Disruptive coloration might evolve in visually
heterogeneous microhabitats, whereas background matching could be favoured in chromatically homogeneous
microhabitats. In this study, we used digital photography to explore the potential use of disruptive coloration and
background matching in males and females of the Neotropical grasshopper Sphenarium purpurascens in different
habitats. We found chromatic differences in three habitats and sexual dichromatism that might be explained by local
adaptation. Although females and males were sexually dichromatic, interpopulation differences were found in the
magnitude of the sexual dichromatism. In a highly contrasting environment, both males and females seemed to follow
a disruptive strategy, whereas in a heterogeneous environments males and females followed different colour cryptic
strategies, in which males were more disruptive than females, and females exhibited high background matching with
fewer disruptive elements. Selective predation in different microhabitats and differences in mobility between the
sexes might explain the colour pattern divergence between females and males.

ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: background matching – crypsis – disruptive coloration – photographic analysis –
sexual dichromatism – Sphenarium purpurascens.

                    INTRODUCTION                                    because it breaks the outlines of the organisms
                                                                    independently of the variable background patterns,
Cryptic coloration is an adaptative defensive
                                                                    and it can be adaptative in organisms with high
mechanism against predators and is probably the
                                                                    mobility (Stevens et al., 2006a, b). Given that crypsis
most widespread form of concealment (Merilaita &
                                                                    decreases the probability of detection by predators, its
Lind, 2005; Merilaita et al., 2017). Colour patterns
                                                                    variation usually matches the geographical variation
appear cryptic by background matching (colours that
                                                                    in substrate colour (Endler, 1990; Stuart-Fox & Ord,
resemble the general colour of the visual background)
                                                                    2004; Rosenblum, 2006; Marshall et al., 2015; Hantak
or disruptive coloration (patterns that conceal the
                                                                    & Kuchta, 2018). Moreover, if females and males use
body outline of an animal; Norris & Lowe, 1964).
                                                                    different microhabitats owing to their different sexual
Background matching can be favoured in chromatically
                                                                    roles, natural selection towards crypsis can favour a
homogeneous microhabitats (Robledo-Ospina et al.,
                                                                    divergence in cryptic colour patterns between females
2017; Orton & McBrayer, 2019), but it can be ineffective
                                                                    and males (Forsman & Appelqvist, 1999; Medina et al.,
at reducing the risk of detection when animals are
                                                                    2016; Ramírez-Delgado & Cueva del Castillo, 2020).
in motion in heterogeneous environments (Ioannou
& Krause 2009). In contrast, disruptive coloration                    Sexual dichromatism is understudied. Studies have
could evolve in visually heterogeneous microhabitats                focused mainly on vertebrates (Font et al., 2009),
                                                                    and it has been explained by differences in selective
                                                                    pressures imposed by visual predators in only a few
*Correponding author. E-mail: rcueva@ecologia.unam.mx               cases (Orton & McBrayer, 2019). Examples of crypsis

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911                    900
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                        901

mediating the differences in coloration between                  environmental heterogeneity. In the areas surrounding
females and males are found in many bird species,                Mexico City, nymphs emerge mainly at the beginning
although in these cases the females tend to be cryptic           of the rainy season (early June). Adults mostly appear
because of predation pressures, whereas males are                and reproduce during autumn (mid-August to mid-
conspicuous owing to sexual selection (Badyaev &                 December). Oviposition and death occur at the end of
Hill, 2003; Medina et al., 2017). In invertebrates,              autumn and beginning of winter, approximately from
the study of the evolution of sexual dimorphism in               mid-October to mid-December (Cueva del Castillo
colour patterns has focused on arthropods and is                 et al., 1999). However, at low elevations, its life cycle can
poorly documented (Forsman & Appelqvist, 1999; Li                start at the beginning of May and end late in January
et al., 2008; Ramírez-Delgado & Cueva del Castillo,              (R. Cueva del Castillo, pers. obs.). Thus, temperate and
2020). Given that many cryptic species are sexually              dry forests, rain forests and even deserts are found
dichromatic, their study has profound implications               in the distribution range of S. purpurascens, only

                                                                                                                                 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
regarding the evolution of intraspecific chromatic               a short distance away from each other at the same
variation with respect to selective pressures imposed            latitude (Sanabria-Urbán & Cueva del Castillo, 2020),
by visual predators.                                             providing the opportunity to explore local chromatic
   Cryptic coloration is common in grasshoppers                  adaptation to diverse environments in this species.
(Eterovick et al., 1997; Forsman & Appelqvist, 1999;                Sphenarium purpurascens, like most species in
Ahnesjö & Forsman, 2006; Karpestam et al., 2012;                 the genus, exhibits wide colour variation (Sanabria-
Baños-Villalba et al., 2018; Edelaar et al., 2019), and          Urbán et al., 2017), which suggests substantial
yet the evolution of sexual dimorphism in colour                 genetic and/or plastic variation and low phylogenetic
patterns in this group of insects has been poorly                constraints on its chromatic evolution (Blomberg &
documented (Ramírez-Delgado & Cueva del Castillo,                Garland 2002). The dorsal colours of S. purpurascens
2020). Sphenarium purpurascens is an univoltine                  vary from green to shades of brown or grey (Fig. 1).
generalist herbivore; adults are found in herbs, grass           Males and females have longitudinal and transverse
and bush leaves. The species has a broad distribution            bands over the thorax and abdomen, showing
and elevation range in central Mexico, from 800 to               extensive continuous variation. However, males
2700 m a.s.l., from the southern Altiplano to the Sierra         usually exhibit considerably more variation in the
Madre del Sur in the Mexican states of Guanajuato,               patterns and number of bands than females, which
Hidalgo, Mexico, Mexico City, Michoacán, Oaxaca,                 tend to have areas that are coloured more evenly
Puebla, Queretaro, Tlaxcala and Veracruz. The                    (Sanabria-Urbán et al., 2017). Males and females
western and eastern mountain ranges create high                  mate randomly with respect to male and female

Figure 1. Dorsal views of the grasshoppers. A, B, female (A) and male (B) from Morelia. C, D, female (C) and male (D)
from Pedregal. E, F, female (E) and male (F) from Tlaxcala. G, dorsal view of female, indicating the dorsal surface of the
grasshopper (GDS) and the adjacent background surface (BS).

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
902    R. CUEVA DEL CASTILLO ET AL.

colour patterns (Cueva del Castillo & Cano-Santana,             established. It was then collected by hand, placed in a
2001). Females are less mobile than males and can               plastic bag (15 cm × 10 cm) and put into a cooler until it
be found near the ground, where they lay their eggs             was unable to move. Each grasshopper was returned to
(Camacho Castillo, 1999). In contrast, males are                the same spot where it was first seen (usually on leaves
often found on top of plants, where they actively look          or plant stems), and photographs of both the dorsal
for females (R. Cueva del Castillo, pers. obs.). They           view of the grasshopper and the background where
are predated by many vertebrates, including birds,              it was placed were taken (Fig. 1G). After taken the
mammals and reptiles (Kevan, 1977).                             photographs, the grasshoppers were placed temporarily
   Owing to the environmental complexity of the                 in a plastic bag in order to avoid their potential
distribution range of S. purpurascens and the broad             recapture. Grasshoppers that moved or fled in response
variety of potential visual predators of this species, we       to the approaching collectors were discarded from the
expected to find differences in the chromatic patterns          study. In all cases, photographs were taken with a

                                                                                                                                 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
of the three analysed populations and that the patterns         Nikon D3200 camera fitted with an AF-S NIKKOR
of grasshoppers associated with places having higher            18–55 mm, f/3.5–5.6 lens. Camera modifications to
visual complexity would be more diverse. In addition,           allow sensitivity to the ultraviolet spectrum were not
given that males and females behave differently and can         implemented; therefore, our analysis is restricted to
be found in niches with different chromatic composition,        the visible spectrum. However, previous studies have
we expected to find differences in their cryptic strategies.    shown marginal reflectance of ultraviolet light on
                                                                grasshoppers (Tsurui et al., 2010). All photographs were
                                                                taken in field conditions between 11.00 and 14.00 h in
                                                                broad daylight. A white diffuser umbrella was placed
            MATERIAL AND METHODS                                over each grasshopper in order to remove potential
                                                                shadows. All photographs were taken 40–50 cm away
         Study sites and image acquisition                      from the grasshopper and included a ColorChecker card
We took photographs of male and female grasshoppers             (X-rite ColorChecker Passport Photo 2; Munsell Color
from three populations located in Mexico City, specifically     Laboratories) in the same plane as the grasshopper and
in Pedregal de San Ángel (19°19′07.9ʺN, 99°11′33.7ʺW),          its background. After the photographs were taken, all
and in the outskirts of the cities of Tlaxcala (19°23′17.0ʺN,   grasshoppers were released at the same places where
98°12′49.4ʺW) and Morelia (819°70′44ʺN, 101°11′94ʺW).           they were collected. Following the suggestions of Stevens
Images of adult grasshoppers and their backgrounds              et al. (2007) and Troscianko & Stevens (2015) on taking
were acquired in the middle of the rainy season, during         objective measurements from digital photographs, we
the first and second weeks of October 2018, when                took the photographs as follows: focal distance was
most of the individuals in the populations were adults          constant at 55 mm; aperture was set to f-stop: f/5.6; light
and the vegetation was still green. Pedregal de San             sensitivity value (ISO) was set to 400 in all photographs;
Ángel was chosen because of its high environmental              and the shutter speed was adjusted in every shot to
heterogeneity (see Ramírez-Delgado & Cueva del                  prevent overexposure of the pictures. Images were
Castillo, 2020). In contrast, the Tlaxcala location was         stored as RAW images to avoid information loss.
at the border of an oak–pine forest, and the Morelia site
was a grassland, dominated mainly by one grass species.
Given their plant diversity, Pedregal de San Angel and                               Image analyses
Morelia represent the most complex and the simplest             Using the multispectral image calibration and analysis
environments, respectively. These localities allowed us         (MICA) toolbox v.2.0 (Troscianko & Stevens, 2015)
to test the potentially different cryptic strategies in         for I mage J v.1.52 (Schneider et al., 2012) software
males and females of S. purpurascens (Fig. 1) associated        and making use of the ColorChecker included in the
with three contrasting environments.                            photographs, we converted the images into human
  At each of the three collection sites, three people           cone-catch images. This conversion produces images
walked slowly over the area, searching for grasshoppers.        based on the spectral sensitivities of the human visual
Humans can be considered as regular predators of                system (Osorio & Vorobyev, 2005, 2008; Delhey et al.,
S. purpurascens because they have collected and                 2015).
used these grasshoppers as a food resource since
pre-Columbian times (Sanabria-Urbán & Cueva
del Castillo, 2020). Moreover, in certain conditions,           Background matching
humans and birds perform similarly in detection tasks           We measured the background matching of the
(Dukas & Kamil, 2001; Michalis et al., 2017).                   colour and brightness of the morphs against their
  Particular care was taken not to disturb any detected         background using chromatic and achromatic just
grasshoppers. When one was found, its location was              noticeable differences (JNDs) from the cone-catch

                           © 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                     903

images. Low JND values denote high resemblance                   superior to other pattern metrics algorithms tested in
between surfaces, whereas high JND values denote                 humans (see Troscianko et al., 2017). The GabRat tool
low resemblance between two surfaces (Osorio &                   is based on a Gabor band-pass filter (see Troscianko
Vorobyev, 2008; Troscianko & Stevens, 2015).                     et al., 2017; Price et al., 2019). Before using this tool,
                                                                 we converted the cone-catch images into LAB images.
                                                                 LAB images fit roughly with human luminance and
Pattern analysis                                                 colour perception and allow us to measure chromatic
We performed a granularity analysis based on fast                disruption (Troscianko et al., 2017). These images
Fourier band-pass filtering from the cone-catch images           are composed of three channels: L corresponds to
to evaluate the colour patterns. Band-pass filters allow         an achromatic channel (lightness), and A and B to
information at different spatial scales to be separated          chromatic channels (red to yellow and blue to green,
(for details, see Chiao et al., 2009; Stoddard & Stevens,        respectively) (Kim et al., 2000). The GabRat tool

                                                                                                                              Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
2010). Granularity analysis measures the standard                estimates the coherent and false edges of an object in an
deviation of pixel reflectance at different pixel scales,        image. The analysis produces values ranging from zero
known as filter sizes; this measurement is referred              to one, where values close to zero are non-disruptive.
to as energy. The graphical representation of energy
across the different filter sizes generates an energy
spectrum, which is useful to compare energy patterns                               Statistical analyses
between surfaces (Chiao et al., 2009). This analysis             Background colour matching
bears a loose resemblance to the cognitive processing            To explore the chromatic and achromatic JNDs,
of visual information by an animal, with decomposition           we performed a multivariate analysis of variance
of spatial information into different spatial frequencies        (MANOVA) considering locality, sex and locality × sex
(Godfrey et al., 1987; Stevens, 2011; Stoddard &                 interaction. Data were transformed by quadratic
Osorio, 2019). Granularity analysis has been used to             square to meet the test assumptions. Given that
distinguish matches in background patterns (Chiao                the MANOVA was significant (see results below),
et al., 2009; Tyrie et al., 2015) and to mark contrasts,         additional univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
which are typically found in disruptive colour patterns          were performed to detect the significant parameters of
(Robledo-Ospina et al., 2017). Granularity analysis              the analysis.
has been used to measure the pattern markings of
several species of animals, including zebras and lions
(Godfrey et al., 1987), cuttlefish (Barbosa et al., 2008;        Comparisons of grasshopper dorsal surface
Chiao et al., 2009), fish (Tyrie et al., 2015), spiders          pattern between localities and sexes
(Robledo-Ospina et al., 2017) and eggs (Stoddard &               A MANOVA considering locality, sex and locality × sex
Stevens, 2010; Yang et al., 2015), in addition to other          interaction was performed to explore potential
Sphenarium grasshoppers (Ramírez-Delgado & Cueva                 differences between the parameters (size of dominant
del Castillo, 2020).                                             marking, pattern diversity and overall pattern
   We used the average pixel reflectance of red and              contrast of the grasshopper dorsal surface). Before
green channels to calculate the energy spectrum of               running the analysis, the data were ln-transformed
grasshoppers and their background across 15 filters              to meet the test assumptions. Additional ANOVAs
ranging from two to 256 pixels, in increments of                 and Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) test
multiples of √2. We obtained three descriptive variables         were performed to identify the significant parameters
from the energy spectrum: the filter size at which the           of the analysis.
maximum energy peak of the spectrum is reached (size
of dominant marking); the proportion of the energy
peak of the spectrum compared with the rest of the               Comparisons of background surface pattern
spectrum (pattern diversity); and the total energy of            between localities and sexes
the spectrum (overall contrast between patterns).                A MANOVA considering locality, sex and locality × sex
                                                                 interaction was performed to explore potential
                                                                 differences between the parameters (size of dominant
Disruptive coloration                                            marking, pattern diversity and overall pattern contrast
We evaluated the edge disruption of the grasshoppers             against the surfaces of the backgrounds where the
using the GabRat tool implemented in the MICA                    grasshoppers were found). Before analysis, the data
toolbox. We used the GabRat tool to measure the ratio            were ln-transformed to meet the test assumptions.
between false and coherent edges of the surfaces of the          Additional ANOVAs and Tukey’s HSD tests were
grasshoppers. This metric is one of the best predictors          performed to detect the significant parameters of the
of human detection time of disruptive targets, and it is         analysis.

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
904      R. CUEVA DEL CASTILLO ET AL.

Comparison between dorsal grasshopper surface                     were not significant, they were not included in the
and background for females and males                              univariate models.
In order to compare the grasshoppers from the three                 The ANOVA for the chromatic JND detected
localities with their backgrounds, we performed                   significant differences between females and males.
Student’s paired t tests comparing the dorsal surfaces            The background colour matching was higher (lower
(size of dominant markings and pattern diversity) of              values) in females than in males (Table 1A; Fig. 2A).
the grasshoppers with the background surfaces where               Also, the ANOVA for the achromatic JND indicated
they were placed.                                                 that the differences between the sexes were highly
                                                                  significant (Table 1B). The achromatic background
                                                                  matching in male grasshoppers was lower (higher
Disruptive coloration                                             JND) than in females, indicating a low resemblance to
We performed a MANOVA considering locality, sex                   the background (Fig. 2B).

                                                                                                                                      Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
and locality × sex interaction to explore the GabRat
in the different channels of the LAB images. The data
were ln-transformed to meet the test assumptions.                   Comparisons of grasshopper dorsal surfaces
Given that the MANOVA was significant (see results),                          between localities and sexes
additional ANOVAs and Tukey’s HSD tests were                      The MANOVA indicated differences between the three
performed to detect the significant parameters of the             localities and between male and female dorsal surfaces
analysis. Statistical analyses were performed with R              (locality: Wilks’s λ = 0.69, F6,294 = 10.18, P < 0.0001; sex:
v.4.0 (R Core Team, 2020).                                        Wilks’s λ = 0.39, F3,147 = 75.21, P < 0.0001). Nevertheless,
                                                                  the magnitude of the differences between males and
                                                                  females in the three localities was similar (locality × sex:
                                                                  λ = 0.93, F6,294 = 1.79, P = 0.10). Given that the locality × sex
                       RESULTS
                                                                  interaction was not significant, it was not included in
We obtained 155 photographs of the dorsal areas                   the univariate models. There were differences in the
and backgrounds of S. purpurascens from the three                 size of dominant markings of grasshoppers between the
localities. Twenty-six photographs of females and 31              localities and between females and males.
of males were taken in Pedregal de San Ángel, 26 of                  The grasshoppers from Morelia had slightly larger
females and 32 of males in Tlaxcala, and 20 of females            dominant marks than those from the other localities.
and 20 of males in Morelia.                                       Moreover, females had larger dominant marks than
                                                                  males (Table 2A; Fig. 3A). Regarding the diversity
                                                                  of dorsal patterns, the grasshoppers from Morelia
           Background colour matching                             and their backgrounds exhibited greater pattern
The MANOVA indicated significant differences only                 diversity (i.e. pattern heterogeneity). Only males
for sex in chromatic and achromatic JNDs. There were              from Tlaxcala had significantly more patterns than
no differences between localities or population × sex             females in their locality (Table 2B, 2E; Fig. 3B). The
interactions (locality: Wilks’s λ = 0.97, F4,296 = 0.82,          contrast of the overall patterns of the dorsal surfaces
P = 0.52; sex: Wilks’s λ = 0.84, F2,148 = 14.56, P < 0.0001;      of grasshoppers showed highly significant differences
locality × sex: Wilks’s λ = 0.95, F4,296 = 1.92, P = 0.11).       between the three localities and the sexes. The
Thus, background colour matching and resemblance to               grasshoppers from Morelia had more contrasting
the background were similar in the three populations.             patterns than those from the other localities.
Given that locality and locality × sex interaction                Moreover, patterns of males contrasted more than

Table 1. ANOVAs of the chromatic (A) and achromatic (B) just noticeable differences (JNDs) between the dorsal surface
of the grasshoppers and their background

Source                d.f.               Sum of squares               Mean square                F-value               P-value

(A) Chromatic JND
Sex                  1                    1.171                        1.17                        8.26                   0.0046
Residuals          153                   21.61                         0.14
(B) Achromatic JND
Sex                  1                   13.52                        13.52                      25.35                 < 0.0001
Residuals          153                   81.61                         0.53

                             © 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                       905

                                                                      Comparison between grasshopper dorsal
                                                                  surface and background for females and males
                                                                 When we compared female and male dorsal chromatic
                                                                 patterns with their respective backgrounds for the
                                                                 three sampled localities, we found that both the size
                                                                 of the dominant markings and the pattern diversity
                                                                 of the dorsal surface of females matched their
                                                                 background (Table 3A, 3C, 3D), whereas their overall
                                                                 pattern contrast differed from their background. In
                                                                 contrast, the males from the three populations showed
                                                                 significant differences between their dominant

                                                                                                                                  Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
                                                                 markings and the overall contrast of their background
                                                                 (Table 3B, 3D, 3F). Only males from Tlaxcala showed
                                                                 significant differences between pattern diversity and
                                                                 background diversity (Table 3F).

                                                                        Disruptive coloration (GabRat) of the
                                                                       grasshoppers from different localities
                                                                 The MANOVA indicated significant differences for the
                                                                 disruptive coloration between locality, sex and their
                                                                 interaction (locality: Wilks’s λ = 0.62, F6,294 = 13.20,
                                                                 P < 0.0001; sex: Wilks’s λ = 0.74, F3,147 = 17.08, P < 0.0001;
                                                                 locality × sex: Wilks’s λ = 0.86, F6,294 = 3.94, P = 0.0008).
                                                                 The GabRat values of the three grasshopper localities
                                                                 were relatively low (maximum GabRat values: L = 0.37,
                                                                 A = 0.51 and B = 0.46; minimum GabRat values:
                                                                 L = 0.5, A = 0.10 and B = 0.07; and mean GabRat
                                                                 values: L = 0.10, A = 0.21 and B = 0.14), suggesting
                                                                 a relatively low disruptivity (Troscianko et al., 2018).
                                                                 Nevertheless, we found significant differences between
                                                                 localities. The GabRat for the L channel (achromatic
                                                                 channel) showed significant differences between the
                                                                 localities; grasshoppers from Morelia had the highest
                                                                 disruptive achromatic values (Table 4A; Fig. 4A). The
                                                                 chromatic GabRat channels (A and B) also showed
                                                                 significant differences between the localities (Table 4B,
                                                                 4C); grasshoppers from Pedregal de San Ángel had the
                                                                 lowest values for channel A, whereas grasshoppers
Figure 2. Box plots of chromatic (A) and achromatic (B)
                                                                 from Tlaxcala had the lowest levels for channel B.
just noticeable differences (JNDs) between the dorsal
surfaces of female and male grasshoppers and their                  Interestingly, the disruptive properties of both sexes
respective backgrounds. In both cases, there are significant     in Morelia were similar in achromatic and chromatic
differences according to Tukey’s HSD tests. Data were            channels, although males in Pedregal and Tlaxcala had
plotted using square root transformations.                       more disruptive patterns than females. Moreover, we
                                                                 found high and significant differences in the magnitude
those of females against their background (Table 2C;             of the disruptive coloration between females and males,
Fig. 3C). The dominant markings in the background                although only in channel A did we find the highest
of the localities and places where females and males             and most significant differences in the magnitude of
were found were similar (Table 2D), although the                 the disruptive coloration between females and males
pattern diversity was higher in Morelia (Table 2E;               in Pedregal de San Ángel and Tlaxcala (locality × sex
Fig. 3B), and the overall pattern contrast differed              interaction), with males being more disruptive than
between the localities and places where females and              females. Interestingly, in Pedregal de San Ángel,
males were located. Contrast was higher in Morelia,              perhaps the most diverse chromatic environment, the
but the places where the males were found exhibited              disruptive differences between females and males for
higher contrast than where the females were found                GabRat channel A were higher than in the other two
(Table 2F; Fig. 3C).                                             localities (Table 4B, 4C; Fig. 4B, C).

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
906      R. CUEVA DEL CASTILLO ET AL.

Table 2. ANOVAs of the size of the dominant marking, diversity and overall pattern contrast of the dorsal surface of the
grasshoppers (A–C) and of their background surfaces (D–F)

Source                d.f.               Sum of squares               Mean square                F-value              P-value

(A) Size of dominant marking of dorsal surface of grasshoppers
Locality                  1               1.82                         0.91                       3.21                  0.043
Sex                       2              22.75                        22.75                      80.27                > 0.0001
Residuals              151               42.8                          0.28
(B) Pattern diversity of dorsal surface of grasshoppers
Locality                  2               0.015                         0.0077                    0.855                 0.427
Sex                       1               0.054                         0.0543                    6.05                  0.015

                                                                                                                                   Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
Residuals              151                1.35                          0.009
(C) Overall pattern contrast of dorsal surface of grasshoppers
Locality                  2               8.1                           4.05                     31.23                > 0.0001
Sex                       1               9.269                         9.27                     71.48                > 0.0001
Residuals              151               19.58                          0.13
(D) Size of dominant marking of background surface
Locality                  2               0.88                          0.442                     0.92                  0.399
Sex                       1               0.12                          0.122                     0.25                  0.614
Residuals              151               72.21                          0.478
(E) Pattern diversity of background surface
Locality                  2               0.139                         0.070                     7.57                  0.0007
Sex                       1               0.0027                        0.0026                    0.29                  0.59
Residuals              151                1.38                          0.0092
(F) Overall pattern contrast of background surface
Locality                  2               6.27                          3.135                     8.92                  0.0002
Sex                       1               3.31                          3.314                     9.42                  0.0025
Residuals              151               53.1                           0.352

                    DISCUSSION                                    on achromatic signals (Osorio et al., 1999). Also,
                                                                  chromatic signals could be useful in searching tasks
Our results show that females bear a closer
                                                                  in variable light conditions (Schaefer et al., 2006;
resemblance to the background (colour and patterns)
                                                                  Cazetta et al., 2009). Perhaps, the main predators of
than males. However, males have a higher disruptive
                                                                  these grasshoppers are chromatically oriented, and
coloration than the females, showing smaller marks
                                                                  this might favour chromatic background matching,
with more contrasting and diverse patterns than
                                                                  whereas the achromatic signals could be less reliable
females. Interestingly, we found a different pattern in
                                                                  if there are large fluctuations in daily light conditions.
Morelia. Although females bear a closer resemblance
                                                                     The differences in colour pattern between the
to the background than males, the marking diversity
                                                                  populations of S. purpurascens can be attributed to
of males and females is similar. The grasses generate
                                                                  local adaptation to different environmental conditions.
a background composed of contrasting lights and
                                                                  The differences in the selective pressures in the three
striped shadows that might favour similar disruptive
                                                                  populations of S. purpurascens seem to favour the
properties in males and females. Nonetheless, despite
                                                                  evolution of two cryptic strategies: disruptive markings
interpopulation differences, we found a relatively high
                                                                  and matching coloration. Disruptive coloration could
chromatic background matching and resemblance
                                                                  evolve in visually heterogeneous microhabitats because
to the background in grasshoppers from the three
                                                                  it breaks the outline of an organism despite the variable
localities, although the achromatic background
                                                                  background patterns, whereas background matching
resemblance was lower.
                                                                  could be favoured in chromatically homogeneous
   The differences between chromatic and achromatic
                                                                  microhabitats (Robledo-Ospina et al., 2017; Orton &
channel background matching could be associated
                                                                  McBrayer, 2019). The lights and striped shadows of the
with different levels of success in predator searching
                                                                  grassland outside Morelia could explain the similarity
distances. Predators that search over short distances
                                                                  between female and male disruptive patterns. This
or hunt large targets might rely more on chromatic
                                                                  environment contrasts with Pedregal de San Ángel,
signals, whereas those that search over relatively
                                                                  a place that offers a complex environment with a
larger distances or hunt small targets might rely
                                                                  wide diversity of plants, which might lead to a wide

                             © 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                   907

                                                                 heterogeneity increases the possibility that both
                                                                 females and males are found in different background
                                                                 patterns. In contrast, the grasshoppers from the oak
                                                                 forest border in Tlaxcala show intermediate levels
                                                                 of sexual dichromatism, which could be associated
                                                                 with a more homogeneous and less visually complex
                                                                 environment than Pedregal de San Ángel.
                                                                    The marking elements associated with females
                                                                 and males can be cryptic if they reduce the risk of
                                                                 boundary detection by potential predators (Merilaita,
                                                                 1998; Cuthill et al., 2005; Schaefer & Stobbe, 2006) and
                                                                 can be adaptative in organisms with high mobility in

                                                                                                                              Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
                                                                 heterogeneous environments (Stevens et al., 2006a, b).
                                                                 This strategy is more evident in females and males
                                                                 from Morelia, and is principally followed by males
                                                                 from Pedregal de San Ángel and Tlaxcala. We could
                                                                 expect high mobility in males because usually they
                                                                 search actively for females, especially in protandrous
                                                                 species (Thornhill & Alcock, 1983). Interestingly, in
                                                                 Pedregal de San Ángel, the males of S. purpurascens
                                                                 are protandrous (Cueva del Castillo & Núñez-Farfán,
                                                                 1999), and they are also more mobile than females
                                                                 (Camacho Castillo, 1999).
                                                                    In Pedregal de San Ángel and Tlaxcala, females
                                                                 exhibit less disruptive coloration than males, but
                                                                 their background matching is higher than in males. In
                                                                 these two localities, the payoff for this strategy could
                                                                 be higher for females owing to the environmental
                                                                 homogeneity of the places where they can be found.
                                                                 The colour matching with their background could
                                                                 reduce their detectability if their mobility is reduced or
                                                                 if they can place themselves where the colour match
                                                                 is high (Endler, 1978; Merilaita et al., 2017; Michalis
                                                                 et al., 2017). However, in a heterogeneous environment
                                                                 it would depend on the ability of individuals to stay in a
                                                                 highly matching microhabitat or reduce their mobility
                                                                 (Merilaita et al., 1999; Bond, 2007), as occurs in the
                                                                 population of S. purpurascens in Pedregal de San Ángel
                                                                 (Camacho Castillo, 1999). The large size of females
                                                                 (Cueva del Castillo et al., 1999) and the increase in
                                                                 their weight owing to egg maturation could explain
                                                                 their lack of mobility. In any case, we interpreted
                                                                 these results considering the human visible spectrum.
                                                                 The spectral sensitivity could be very different in
                                                                 other possible predators, such as birds or mice. Their
                                                                 prey detectability could involve elements that we did
Figure 3. The different marking patterns measured in
                                                                 not consider in this study (Théry & Gomez, 2010).
both sexes and localities. A, size of dominant marking. B,
                                                                 However, humans and other potential predators of
diversity. C, overall pattern contrast. Values are presented
as the mean (SEM). Different symbols denote differences
                                                                 these grasshoppers, such as mice, share part of their
between the organisms and their backgrounds according to         visible spectrum (green and red) to detect potential
Table 3.                                                         prey (Denman et al., 2018), and in certain conditions,
                                                                 birds and humans perform similarly in detection tasks
                                                                 (Dukas & Kamil, 2001; Michalis et al., 2017).
variety of visually complex patterns (see Dimitrova                 In S. purpurascens, chromatic polymorphism and
& Merilaita, 2010) and where high levels of sexual               sexual dichromatism could represent stable specialist
dichromatism in S. purpurascens can be found. Visual             strategies (polymorphic crypsis) that have evolved

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
908      R. CUEVA DEL CASTILLO ET AL.

Table 3. Student’s paired t test comparisons of pattern parameters of dorsal surface of the different populations of
Sphenarium purpurascens grasshoppers and their background

Locality and sex                 Pattern variable                          d.f.               t                    P-value

Morelia, female                  Size of dominant marking                  19                 −1.22                  0.2371
                                 Pattern diversity                         19                  0.74                  0.4664
                                 Overall contrast                          19                 −2.89                  0.0092*
Morelia, male                    Size of dominant marking                  19                  2.91                  0.009*
                                 Pattern diversity                         19                  1.41                  0.1733
                                 Overall contrast                          19                 −7.3                 < 0.0001*
Pedregal, female                 Size of dominant marking                  25                 −1.57                  0.1273

                                                                                                                                 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
                                 Pattern diversity                         25                  1.12                  0.2747
                                 Overall contrast                          25                 −2.51                  0.0187*
Pedregal, male                   Size of dominant marking                  30                  5.55                < 0.0001*
                                 Pattern diversity                         30                 −1.74                  0.0917
                                 Overall contrast                          30                 −6.64                < 0.0001*
Tlaxcala, female                 Size of dominant marking                  25                 −0.77                  0.4489
                                 Pattern diversity                         25                 −0.26                  0.7953
                                 Overall contrast                          25                 −3.26                  0.0031*
Tlaxcala, male                   Size of dominant marking                  31                  4.54                < 0.0001*
                                 Pattern diversity                         31                 −2.83                  0.0081*
                                 Overall contrast                          31                 −3.99                  0.0003*

*P-value of P < 0.05.

Table 4. ANOVAs of the GabRat values

Source                    d.f.            Sum of squares               Mean square                F-value            P-value

(A) GabRat of L channel (acromatic channel)
Locality                    2            3.015                         1.5074                     27.80              0.0001
Sex                         1            0.134                         0.134                       2.47              0.118
Locality × sex              2            0.046                         0.023                       0.42              0.655
Residuals                149             8.08                          0.0542
(B) GabRat of A channel
Locality                    2            0.88                          0.44                        6.99              0.0012
Sex                         1            2.18                          2.18                       34.60              0.0001
Locality × sex              2            1.23                          0.61                        9.74              0.0001
Residuals                149             9.41                          0.063
(C) GabRat of B channel
Locality                    2            1.52                          0.76                        8.51              0.0003
Sex                         1            1.71                          1.71                       19.16              0.0001
Locality × sex              2            0.37                          0.19                        2.09              0.13
Residuals                149            13.28                          0.089

as a result of the diversity of predators and hunting           camouflage that is not perfectly background matched to a
strategies associated with environmental heterogeneity.         single habitat but instead offers a degree of resemblance
Nevertheless, the differences in spectral sensitivity and       to multiple backgrounds and/or using camouflage that
visual acuity of potential predators and the differences        works somewhat independently of background matching
in mobility of the grasshoppers could constrain the             (Hughes et al., 2019).
evolution of crypsis (Caves et al., 2018; Hughes et al.,          Our results might have important implications
2019). The chromatic patterns of the grasshoppers could         regarding the origin and maintenance of intraspecific
be generalist (or compromise) strategies that might             colour variation with respect to selection pressures
match several backgrounds to some extent, but none              imposed by visual predators. We are currently
closely. These grasshoppers might be adopting a form of         conducting studies on predation and escape behaviour

                           © 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                             909

                                                                                ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
                                                                 This research was supported by the project PAPIIT-
                                                                 UNAM IN211617. Víctor Hugo Ramírez-Delgado
                                                                 acknowledges the Posgrado en Ciencias Biológicas
                                                                 of the Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
                                                                 (UNAM) and Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología
                                                                 (CONACyT) for a doctoral scholarship (no. 330551).
                                                                 The authors wish to thank M. A. Serrano-Meneses
                                                                 and Salomón Sanabria-Urbán for their help in the
                                                                 field, and Judith X. Ponce-Wainer for her valuable
                                                                 suggestions to improve the quality of the manuscript.

                                                                                                                                       Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
                                                                 Valuable suggestions on the manuscript were made by
                                                                 two anonymous reviewers and the editor.

                                                                                        REFERENCES
                                                                 Ahnesjö J, Forsman A. 2006. Differential habitat selection
                                                                   by pygmy grasshopper color morphs; interactive effects of
                                                                   temperature and predator avoidance. Evolutionary Ecology
                                                                   20: 235–257.
                                                                 Badyaev AV, Hill GE. 2003. Avian sexual dichromatism in
                                                                   relation to phylogeny and ecology. Annual Review of Ecology,
                                                                   Evolution, and Systematics 34: 27–49.
                                                                 Baños-Villalba A, Quevedo DP, Edelaar P. 2018.
                                                                   Positioning behavior according to individual color variation
                                                                   improves camouflage in novel habitats. Behavioral Ecology
                                                                   29: 404–410.
                                                                 Barbosa A, Mäthger LM, Buresch KC, Kelly J, Chubb C,
                                                                   Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT. 2008. Cuttlefish camouflage: The
                                                                   effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle
                                                                   or disruptive body patterns. Vision Research 48: 1242–1253.
                                                                 Blomberg SP, Garland T Jr. 2002. Tempo a mode in
                                                                   evolution: phylogenetic inertia, adaptation and comparative
                                                                   methods. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 15: 899–910.
                                                                 Bond AB. 2007. The evolution of color polymorphism:
                                                                   crypticity, searching images, and apostatic selection. Annual
                                                                   Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 38: 489–514.
                                                                 Camacho Castillo E. 1999. Demografia y movilidad de
                                                                   Sphenarium purpurascens (Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae) en
                                                                   la Reserva del Pedregal de San Angel. Unpublished Bachelor
                                                                   Thesis, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.
                                                                 Caves EM, Nicholas C, Brandley NC, Johnsen S. 2018.
                                                                   Visual acuity and the evolution of signals. Trends in Ecology
                                                                   & Evolution 33: 358–372.
                                                                 Cazetta E, Schaefer HM, Galetti M. 2009. Why are fruits
                                                                   colorful? The relative importance of achromatic and
Figure 4. The different GabRat values for both males and           chromatic contrasts for detection by birds. Evolutionary
females at the three localities. A, achromatic L channel. B,       Ecology. 23: 233–244.
C, the chromatic channels A and B, respectively. Values are      Chiao CC, Chubb C, Buresch K, Siemann L, Hanlon RT.
presented as the mean (SEM). According to Tukey’s HSD              2009. The scaling effects of substrate texture on camouflage
tests, the letters indicate significant differences; if two        patterning in cuttlefish. Vision Research 49: 1647–1656.
means share a letter, there are no significant differences.      Cueva del Castillo R, Cano-Santana Z. 2001. Variación
                                                                   de la coloración corporal de Sphenarium purpurascens
to test the effectiveness of background matching and               (Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae) en función del sexo y su
disruptive coloration cryptic strategies in different              relación con la formación de parejas en un ambiente
chromatic environments.                                            heterogéneo. Folia Entomologica Mexicana 40: 297–309.

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
910     R. CUEVA DEL CASTILLO ET AL.

Cueva del Castillo R, Núñez-Farfán J. 1999. Sexual                   Ioannou CC, Krause J. 2009. Interactions between background
  selection on maturation time and body size in Sphenarium             matching and motion during visual detection can explain why
  purpuracens (Orthoptera: Pyrgomorphidae): correlated                 cryptic animals keep still. Biology Letters 5: 191–193.
  response to selection. Evolution 53: 209–215.                      Karpestam E, Merilaita S, Forsman A. 2012. Reduced
Cueva del Castillo R, Núñez-Farfán J, Cano-Santana Z.                  predation risk for melanistic pygmy grasshoppers in post-
  1999. The role of body size in mating success of Sphenarium          fire environments. Ecology and Evolution 2: 2204–2212.
  purpurascens in Central Mexico. Ecological Entomology 24:          Kevan DK. 1977. The American Pyrgomorphidae (Orthoptera).
  146–155.                                                             Revista de la Sociedad Entomológica Argentina 36: 3–28.
Cuthill IC, Stevens M, Sheppard J, Maddocks T,                       Kim SC, Kim DW, Hong JP, Rah DK. 2000. A quantitative
  Párraga CA, Troscianko TS. 2005. Disruptive coloration               evaluation of pigmented skin lesions using the L*a*b* color
  and background pattern matching. Nature 434: 72–74.                  coordinates. Yonsei Medical Journal 41: 333–339.
Delhey K, Delhey V, Kempenaers B, Peters A. 2015. A                  Li J, Lim MLM, Zhang Z, Liu Q, Liu F, Chen J, Li D. 2008.

                                                                                                                                         Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
  practical framework to analyze variation in animal colors            Sexual dichromatism and male colour morph in ultraviolet-B
  using visual models. Behavioral Ecology 26: 367–375.                 reflectance in two populations of the jumping spider Phintella
Denman DJ, Luviano JA, Ollerenshaw DR, Cross S,                        vittata (Araneae: Salticidae) from tropical China. Biological
  Williams D, Buice MA, Olsen SR, Reid RC. 2018. Mouse                 Journal of the Linnean Society 94: 7–20.
  color and wavelength-specific luminance contrast sensitivity       Marshall KLA, Philpot KE, Damas-Moreira I, Stevens M.
  are non-uniform across visual space. eLife 7: e31209.                2015. Intraspecific colour variation among lizards in distinct
Dimitrova M, Merilaita S. 2010. Prey concealment: visual               island environments enhances local camouflage. PLoS ONE
  background complexity and prey contrast distribution.                10: e0135241.
  Behavioral Ecology 21: 176–181.                                    Medina I, Delhey K, Peters A, Cain KE, Hall ML,
Dukas R, Kamil AC. 2001. Limited attention: the constraint             Mulder RA, Langmore NE. 2017. Habitat structure is
  underlying search image. Behavioral Ecology 12: 192–199.             linked to the evolution of plumage colour in female, but not
Edelaar P, Baños-Villalba A, Quevedo DP, Escudero G,                   male, fairy-wrens. BMC Evolutionary Biology 17: 35.
  Bolnick DI, Jordán-Andrade A. 2019. Biased movement                Medina I, Losos JB, Mahler DL. 2016. Evolution of dorsal
  drives local cryptic coloration on distinct urban pavements.         pattern variation in Greater Antillean Anolis lizards.
  Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286:         Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 120: 427–435.
  20191343.                                                          Merilaita S. 1998. Crypsis through disruptive coloration in
Endler JA. 1978. A predator’s view of animal color patterns.           an isopod. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
  In: Hecht MK, Steere WC, Wallace B, eds. Evolutionary                Sciences 265: 1059–1064.
  Biology. Boston: Springer, 319–364.                                Merilaita S, Lind J. 2005. Background-matching and
Endler JA. 1990. On the measurement and classification                 disruptive coloration, and the evolution of cryptic coloration.
  of colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biological           Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 272:
  Journal of the Linnean Society 41: 315–352.                          665–670.
Eterovick PC, Figueira JEC, Vasconcellos-Neto J. 1997.               Merilaita S, Scott-Samuel NE, Cuthill IC. 2017. How
  Cryptic coloration and choice of escape microhabitats by             camouflage works. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
  grasshoppers (Orthoptera: Acrididae). Biological Journal of          Society B: Biological Sciences 372: 20160341.
  the Linnean Society 61: 485–499.                                   Merilaita S, Tuomi J, Jormalainen V. 1999. Optimization
Font E, Pérez I, de Lanuza G, Sampedro C. 2009.                        of cryptic coloration in heterogeneous habitats. Biological
  Ultraviolet reflectance and cryptic sexual dichromatism              Journal of the Linnean Society 67: 151–161.
  in the ocellated lizard, Lacerta (Timon) lepida (Squamata:         Michalis C, Scott-Samuel NE, Gibson DP, Cuthill IC. 2017.
  Lacertidae). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 97:           Optimal background matching camouflage. Proceedings of
  766–780.                                                             the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 284: 20170709.
Forsman A, Appelqvist S. 1999. Experimental manipulation             Norris KS, Lowe CH. 1964. An analysis of background color-
  reveals differential effects of colour pattern on survival           matching in amphibians and reptiles. Ecology 45: 565–580.
  in male and female pygmy grasshoppers. Journal of                  Orton RW, McBrayer LD. 2019. Resolving tradeoffs among
  Evolutionary Biology 12: 391–401.                                    crypsis, escape behavior, and microhabitat use in sexually
Godfrey D, Lythgoe JN, Rumball DA. 1987. Zebra stripes and             dichromatic species. Oecologia 189: 91–104.
  tiger stripes: the spatial frequency distribution of the pattern   Osorio D, Mikló A, Gonda Z. 1999. Visual ecology and
  compared to that of the background is significant in display and     perception of coloration patterns by domestic chicks.
  crypsis. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 32: 427–433.      Evolutionary Ecology 13: 673–689.
Hantak MM, Kuchta SR. 2018. Predator perception across               Osorio D, Vorobyev M. 2005. Photoreceptor spectral
  space and time: relative camouflage in a colour polymorphic          sensitivities in terrestrial animals: Adaptations for
  salamander. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 123:           luminance and colour vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society
  21–33.                                                               B: Biological Sciences 272: 1745–1752.
Hughes A, Liggins E, Stevens M. 2019. Imperfect                      Osorio D, Vorobyev M. 2008. A review of the evolution of
  camouflage: how to hide in a variable world? Proceedings of          animal colour vision and visual communication signals.
  the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286: 20190646.              Vision Research 48: 2042–2051.

                              © 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN A GRASSHOPPER                           911

Price N, Green S, Troscianko J, Tregenza T, Stevens M. 2019.        Stevens M, Cuthill IC, Windsor AMM, Walker HJ. 2006b.
  Background matching and disruptive coloration as habitat-           Disruptive contrast in animal camouflage. Proceedings of the
  specific strategies for camouflage. Scientific Reports 9: 7840.     Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273: 2433–2438.
R Core Team. 2020. R: a language and environment for                Stevens M, Párraga CA, Cuthill IC, Partridge JC,
  statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical         Troscianko TS. 2007. Using digital photography to study
  Computing. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/                 animal coloration. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society
Ramírez-Delgado VH, Cueva del Castillo R. 2020.                       90: 211–237.
  Background matching, disruptive coloration, and differential      Stoddard MC, Osorio D. 2019. Animal coloration patterns:
  use of microhabitats in two neotropical grasshoppers with           linking spatial vision to quantitative analysis. The American
  sexual dichromatism. Ecology and Evolution 10: 1401–1412.           Naturalist 193: 164–186.
Robledo-Ospina LE, Escobar-Sarria F, Troscianko J,                  Stoddard MC, Stevens M. 2010. Pattern mimicry of host eggs
  Rao D. 2017. Two ways to hide: predator and prey                    by the common cuckoo, as seen through a bird’s eye. Proceedings

                                                                                                                                        Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biolinnean/article/132/4/900/6127663 by guest on 14 May 2021
  perspectives of disruptive coloration and background                of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 1387–1393.
  matching in jumping spiders. Biological Journal of the            Stuart-Fox DM, Ord TJ. 2004. Sexual selection, natural
  Linnean Society 122: 752–764.                                       selection and the evolution of dimorphic coloration and
Rosenblum EB. 2006. Convergent evolution and divergent                ornamentation in agamid lizards. Proceedings of the Royal
  selection: lizards at the White Sands ecotone. The American         Society B: Biological Sciences 271: 2249–2255.
  Naturalist 167: 1–15.                                             Théry M, Gomez D. 2010. Chapter 7. Insect colours and
Sanabria-Urbán S, Cueva del Castillo R. 2020. The evolution           visual appearance in the eyes of their predators. In: Casas J,
  and diversification of Neotropical generalist herbivores: the       Simpson SJ, eds. Advances in insect physiology. London:
  evolutionary history of the grasshopper genus Sphenarium            Academic Press, 38: 267–353.
  Charpentier, 1842. In: Núñez-Farfán J, Valverde PL,               Thornhill R, Alcock J. 1983. The evolution of insect mating
  eds. Evolutionary ecology of plant-herbivore interaction.           systems. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  Switzerland AG: Springer International, 277–292.                  Troscianko J, Skelhorn J, Stevens M. 2017. Quantifying
Sanabria-Urbán S, Song H, Oyama K, González-                          camouflage: how to predict detectability from appearance.
  Rodríguez A, Cueva del Castillo R. 2017. Integrative                BMC Evolutionary Biology 17: 7.
  taxonomy reveals cryptic diversity in neotropical                 Troscianko J, Skelhorn J, Stevens M. 2018. Camouflage
  grasshoppers: taxonomy, phylogenetics, and evolution of             strategies interfere differently with observer search images.
  the genus Sphenarium Charpentier, 1842 (Orthoptera:                 Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 285:
  Pyrgomorphidae). Zootaxa 4274: 1–86.                                20181386.
Schaefer HM, Levey DJ, Schaefer V, Avery ML. 2006. The              Troscianko J, Stevens M. 2015. Image calibration and
  role of chromatic and achromatic signals for fruit detection        analysis toolbox – a free software suite for objectively
  by birds. Behavioral Ecology 17: 784–789.                           measuring reflectance, colour and pattern. Methods in
Schaefer HM, Stobbe N. 2006. Disruptive coloration                    Ecology and Evolution 6: 1320–1331.
  provides camouflage independent of background matching.           Tsurui K, Honma A, Nishida T. 2010. Camouflage effects
  Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273:        of various colour-marking morphs against different
  2427–2432.                                                          microhabitat backgrounds in a polymorphic pygmy
Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. 2012. NIH Image                grasshopper Tetrix japonica. PLoS ONE 5: e11446.
  to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nature Methods 9:          Tyrie EK, Hanlon RT, Siemann LA, Uyarra MC. 2015.
  671–675.                                                            Coral reef flounders, Bothus lunatus, choose substrates on
Stevens M. 2011. Avian vision and egg colouration: concepts           which they can achieve camouflage with their limited body
  and measurements. Avian Biology Research 4: 168–184.                pattern repertoire. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society
Stevens M, Cuthill IC, Alejandro Párraga C, Troscianko T.             114: 629–638.
  2006a. The effectiveness of disruptive coloration as a            Yang C, Hu Y, Ma M, Liang W, Møller AP. 2015. Absence
  concealment strategy. In: Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL,             of egg rejection in an Asian population of house sparrow
  Martinez LM, Alonso JM, Tse PU, eds. Progress in Brain              (Passer domesticus), a conspecific brood parasite in Europe.
  Research. Oxford: Elsevier, 155: 49–64.                             Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 69: 723–727.

                                                        SHARED DATA
The data of the study are available from the Zenodo repository: https://zenodo.org/record/4330018#.X9uvmGm73qs.

© 2021 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2021, 132, 900–911
You can also read