Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA

Page created by Megan Cortez
 
CONTINUE READING
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
Towards
Positive
 Animal
Welfare

  16
        ..
   Vol.

        2015
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
II    WAZA                        16 (2015)

                                                                      Contents
                                                                      Markus Gusset & Gerald Dick
                                                                      Editorial.................................................................. 1

                                                                      Georgia J. Mason
                                                                      Using Species Differences in Health
                                                                      and Well-being to Identify Intrinsic Risk
                                                                      and Protective Factors........................................... 2

                                                                      David Shepherdson & Nadja Wielebnowski
                                                                      The Power of Multi-institutional and
                                                                      Longitudinal Studies for Zoo
                                                                      Animal Welfare Research.......................................6

                                                                      Kathy Carlstead & Janine Brown
                                                                      Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant
                                                                      Welfare: Evaluation of Ovarian Cyclicity and
                                                                      Keeper–Elephant Relationships........................... 10

                                                                      Lance J. Miller, Randall S. Wells, Rita Stacey,
                                                                      F. William Zeigler, Jessica C. Whitham
                                                                      & Michael Adkesson
                                                                      Animal Welfare Management of Bottlenose
                                                                      Dolphins at the Chicago Zoological
                                                                      Society’s Brookfield Zoo...................................... 14

                                                                      David Orban, Hani Freeman, Catharine Wheaton,
                                                                      Jill Mellen, Joseph Soltis & Katherine Leighty
                                                                      Use of Science to Enhance Animal Welfare
                                                                      at Disney’s Animal Kingdom................................ 18

                                                                      Zjef Pereboom & Jeroen Stevens
                                                                      Applied Animal Welfare Research in Zoos:
                                                                      The More the Better............................................. 22

                                                                      Ron Kagan, Stephanie Allard & Scott Carter
                                                                      Exotic Animal Welfare – A Path Forward..............26
     Imprint
                                                                      Christopher W. Kuhar, Andi M. Kornak
     Editors: Markus Gusset & Gerald Dick                             & Kristen E. Lukas
     WAZA Executive Office                                            Beyond Animal Welfare Science........................... 30
     IUCN Conservation Centre
     Rue Mauverney 28                                                 Vicky Melfi & Geoff Hosey
     CH-1196 Gland                                                    Evidence-based Zoo Animal Welfare:
     Switzerland                                                      From Principles to Practice.................................. 34
     Phone: +41 22 999 07 90
     Fax: +41 22 999 07 91                                            Jason Watters, Kate Sulzner,
                                                                      Debbie Marrin, Sandy Huang, Corinne MacDonald,
     Layout and typesetting: michal@sky.cz                            Susan Ostapak, Andrew Poole & Heather Hayle
     Drawings: Kimio Honda (WAZA thanks for his generous donation!)   Assessing Quality of Life in
     Print: Agentura NP, Staré Město, Czech Republic                  Geriatric Zoo Animals........................................... 37

     Edition: 600 copies | © WAZA 2015                                Terry L. Maple
                                                                      Four Decades of Psychological Research
     This edition of WAZA Magazine is also available on               on Zoo Animal Welfare......................................... 41
     www.waza.org.

     Printed on FSC paper.

     ISSN: 2074-4528
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
WAZA                      16 (2015)                                                                                                       1
Markus Gusset1 & Gerald Dick2

Editorial
Whilst conservation of wildlife is the
core purpose of modern zoos and
aquariums, animal welfare is our core
activity. This year, WAZA is going
to release strategic guidance docu-
ments for both animal conservation
and welfare: the third iteration of the
World Zoo and Aquarium Conserva‑
tion Strategy and the first‑ever World
Zoo and Aquarium Animal Welfare
Strategy. This gives testimony to the
equal value and emphasis that WAZA
places on wildlife conservation and
animal welfare.

Animal welfare is understood to vary
on a continuum from very poor to
                                          The Five Domains model for understanding animal welfare, divided into physical/functional
very good. The promotion of positive      and mental components, provides examples of how internal and external conditions give rise
animal welfare states requires differ-    to negative (aversive) and positive (pleasant) subjective experiences, the integrated effects
ent approaches to minimising nega-        of which give rise to an animal’s welfare status (modified from Mellor & Beausoleil 2015;
                                          Anim. Welf. 24: 241–253).
tive welfare states. Modern zoos and
aquariums should work to minimise
the occurrence of negative states        As welfare is a state within an                    Reference to the first four domains
in their animals and, concurrently,      animal and is understood in terms                  enables systematic consideration
should make efforts to promote posi-     of what the animal experiences                     of a wide range of conditions that
tive states.                             subjectively, this model identifies the            may give rise to a range of subjective
                                         two main sources of those mental                   experiences found within the fifth
 The “Five Domains” model presents       experiences. The first is the feelings            “mental” domain. The net impact of
 a useful framework for undertaking      and sensations (collectively known as              all of these experiences is assessed
 systematic and structured assess-      “affects”) that motivate animals to un-             as representing the animal’s welfare
 ments of animal welfare in these        dertake behaviours considered to                   status. Modern zoos and aquariums
 terms. This model outlines four physi- be essential for their survival. These              should provide opportunities for the
 cal/functional domains of “nutrition”,  include thirst motivating an animal to             animals in their care to experience
“environment”, “physical health” and     drink, hunger motivating it to eat and             positive welfare states.
“behaviour”, and the fifth domain,       pain indicating things to avoid. These
 which is the animal’s “mental” state    and other survival‑related factors are             In this edition of the WAZA Magazine,
 (see figure). The Five Domains model    typically covered within the domains               we have compiled various conceptual
 forms the basis of the World Zoo and    of “nutrition”, “environment” and                  and practical approaches to how
 Aquarium Animal Welfare Strategy.      “physical health”.                                  progress towards positive animal
                                                                                            welfare can be achieved by zoos and
                                          The fourth domain of “behaviour”                  aquariums. This includes articles on
                                          captures the second source of sub-                taxa that are challenging and contro-
                                          jective experiences, which can be                 versial to keep in human care, such
                                          negative or positive, and relates to              as elephants, dolphins, polar bears
                                          animals’ perception of their external             and great apes. Collectively, these
                                          circumstances. Negative examples                  articles demonstrate the value of sci-
                                          include: threat eliciting fear, isola-            entific research, often multi‑institu-
                                          tion leading to loneliness and low                tional and longitudinal, to provide an
                                          stimulation to boredom; and positive              evidence base for ensuring that the
                                          examples include: security engender-              animals kept in our care experience
                                          ing confidence and pleasure giving                positive welfare states – in accord-
                                          rise to a sense of reward.                        ance with the Five Domains model.
                                                                                            We hope that this edition of the
                                                                                            WAZA Magazine will substantially
                                                                                            strengthen our community’s efforts
                                                                                            to make caring about animals in zoos
1
    WAZA Chief Conservation Officer                                                         and aquariums continuous with car-
2
    WAZA Executive Director                                                                 ing for them in the wild.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
2       WAZA                    16 (2015)

    Georgia J. Mason1,*

    Using Species Differences in Health
    and Well‑being to Identify Intrinsic
    Risk and Protective Factors

    Summary                                        Species Differences                        Using Species
                                                   in Animal Welfare                          Differences to Test
    The diverse species living in zoos vary                                                   Hypotheses about Intrinsic
    in their propensities for good captive         The 10,000+ species kept in zoos           Risk and Protective Factors
    health and welfare. This variation             are not all managed in fully evi-
    yields opportunities to answer ap-             dence‑based, optimised ways. Con-          What biological characteristics
    plied and fundamental research ques-           sequently, although zoo animals are        predict how well different species
    tions via comparative methods. These           typically healthier, longer‑lived and      adjust to captive life? These can be
    methods can harness this variation to          more fecund than their free‑living         identified empirically via comparative
    statistically identify intrinsic risk and      conspecifics, in some species captive      methods, in which statistical rela-
    protective factors influencing how             individuals often survive and breed        tionships between species‑typical
    well species of different niches adjust        less well than might be expected, and      traits and outcomes of interest are
    to captivity. Known or potential risk          display physiological and behavioural      investigated using data from mul-
    factors include certain foraging styles,       signs of stress. Even taxonomically        tiple species (each being one unit
    and being wide‑ranging or migratory,           close species can vary. Ring‑tailed le-    of replication). Such techniques are
    timid and/or vulnerable to extinction.         murs, for example, typically show few      common in evolutionary and eco-
    Empirically identifying which specific         veterinary, breeding or behavioural        logical research (Mason 2010; Mason
    traits help predict captive welfare            problems, while gentle and black           et al. 2013). However, only recently
    provides an objective way to specify           lemurs are harder to breed success-        have they been used to investigate
    behavioural needs; suggests how to             fully and more prone to stereotypic        animal welfare, perhaps because
    improve husbandry; and could help              behaviour (Mason 2010). Similar            while their logic is simple, the need to
    decide which species to prioritise for         contrasts exist within many genera         control for three potential confounds
    conservation in zoos. Such data could          (Clubb & Mason 2007; Müller et al.         can make implementation complex
    also be used to test certain funda-            2012; Pomerantz et al. 2013).              (Clubb & Mason 2007; Mason 2010;
    mental evolutionary hypotheses.                                                           Müller et al. 2012; Pomerantz et al.
                                                   Differences between captive and an-        2013). One of these potential con-
                                                   cestral environments help explain such     founds is inherent similarity between
                                                   variation: in captivity, the forms of      closely related species, making them
                                                   food and shelter provided may be           statistically non‑independent (Fig. 1).
                                                   evolutionarily new; climates may be        The second is that intrinsic differ-
                                                   different; there is close contact with     ences in pace of life and reproductive
                                                   humans; and abilities to range, explore    strategy must be factored in, when
                                                   and make choices are constrained.          using dependent variables related
                                                   Species’ natural lifestyles may help       to life history (e.g. reproductive
                                                   determine whether any of these             variables or average lifespan) to infer
                                                   differences are obstacles to success.      the effects of captivity. The third is
                                                   In free‑living wild animals adjusting to   husbandry (e.g. relative exhibit size or
                                                   novel habitats (e.g. colonising cities),   management related to studbooks),
                                                   species thrive best if pre‑adapted to      which may systematically vary with
                                                   cope well with change, and/or when         taxon; to pinpoint true influences of
                                                   mismatches between new and ances-          natural lifestyle, any species differ-
                                                   tral environments are relatively small     ence in husbandry must therefore be
                                                   (reviewed by Mason 2010; Mason et          controlled for.
                                                   al. 2013). The same principles seem
                                                   applicable to animals brought into
                                                   captivity: differences between captive
    1
        Department of Animal Science,              and ancestral environments will be
        University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada   minor and/or easily adjusted to for
    *   E‑mail for correspondence:                 some species, but large and/or hard to
        gmason@uoguelph.ca                         accommodate for others.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
WAZA                  16 (2015)                                                                                                             3

  Figure 1    a                                                                   b

                                         The plot in (a) shows hypothetical data from 10 carnivore species, showing an apparent
                                         relationship between natural reliance on burrowing and infant mortality in captivity, which
                                         could indicate that certain species should be allowed to construct their own burrows.
                                         However, this conclusion is premature: (b) reveals that half the hypothetical data come from
                                         Panthera (circles) and half from mustelids (stars). Because each group shares traits inherited
                                         from a common ancestor, the Panthera are inherently similar, likewise the mustelids.
                                         The non‑independence of these taxonomically similar data points makes it inappropriate
                                         to draw a regression line through them as in (a). Custom statistical techniques that take
                                         relatedness into account are therefore needed.

What Potential Risk and
Protective Factors May                     Figure 2
Operate in Zoos?

Being intrinsically wide‑ranging or
migratory. Ros Clubb and I investi-
gated whether the degrees to which
captivity constrains hunting, ranging
or other aspects of natural lifestyle
predict stereotypic behaviour and
captive infant mortality in carnivores
(e.g. Clubb & Mason 2007). Being
large and wide‑ranging, and travel-
ling relatively long distances each      Stereotypic behaviour in carnivores is more time consuming in large, naturally wide‑ranging
day in the wild, emerged as risk         species: across 20 species, body weight and minimum home‑range sizes extracted from field
                                         studies predicted this behaviour’s severity in affected individuals – PB = polar bear, L = lion,
factors (Fig. 2). A decade after these   AF = arctic fox, AM = American mink (Clubb & Mason 2003; figure reprinted with permission from
data were collated, Jeanette Kroshko     Nature 425: 473–474). The relationship became more significant if only pacing was considered
re‑did the work for her MSc thesis,      (Clubb & Mason 2007). Note that the figure is a simple cross‑species plot; final analyses
                                         controlled for phylogenetic non‑independence.
adding additional data, new species
and more stringent controls for small
sample sizes. She could not replicate
the infant mortality result. However,    Pomerantz et al. (2013) found similar              lifestyle, such as more opportunities
she confirmed that species‑typical       effects in primates: across 24 species,            to disperse or explore, multiple den
natural home‑range sizes and daily       average natural day journey length                 sites and/or greater day‑to‑day envi-
travel distances predict the intensity   tended to predict levels of pac-                   ronmental variability, might be good
of stereotypic behaviour (irrespec-      ing and route‑tracing (while these                 ways to improve exhibits, especially
tive of body size); newly found that     animals’ other common abnormal                     to tackle stereotypic route‑tracing.
average “prey chase” distance does       behaviour, hair‑pulling, was pre-                  They also suggest that being natu-
too, suggesting that certain hunt-       dicted by natural group size). To-                 rally sedentary and little‑ranging
ing styles place species at risk; and    gether, these studies suggest that                 pre‑adapts species to captivity.
showed that pacing and route‑trac-       offering the correlates of a ranging
ing were the specific stereotypic
activities best predicted by these
variables.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
4    WAZA                   16 (2015)

    Looking to future work on other            As for future work, bird species that      Conservation status in the wild. Con-
    taxa, seeking similar patterns in birds    readily develop new foraging modes         servation breeding has been said to
    could be fruitful because in the           in the wild cope well when translo-        be more challenging for endangered
    wild, “resident” avian species (those      cated or exposed to other evolution-       birds than their non‑endangered
    staying in one location year‑round)        arily new environments (reviewed           close relatives, while species that
    are typically better than migrants at      by Mason 2010; Mason et al. 2013),         breed well in research laborato-
    adapting to new environments (e.g.         and dietary generalists have been          ries (e.g. Norway rats and rhesus
    establishing self‑sustaining popula-       proposed to adjust well to captivity.      monkeys) are often invasive when
    tions when translocated; reviewed          Together, this suggests that species       living free. Furthermore, one study
    by Mason 2010; Mason et al. 2013).         with flexible diets might be particu-      suggested that across captive canids,
    Restricted ranging has also been           larly easy to keep in zoos; a simple       endangered species had higher pup
    suggested to be a welfare risk factor      hypothesis to test. Focussing on           mortality than unthreatened species.
    for some cetaceans and callitrichids;      abnormal behaviour, oral stereo-           Potential reasons for these possible
    further testable hypotheses for fu-        typic behaviours in hoofstock (e.g.        links could be that species‑typical
    ture research.                             tongue‑twirling) and parrots (e.g.         risk factors predicting vulnerability
                                               feather‑plucking) have been suggest-       in the wild are similar to those likely
    Natural diets or foraging style. As        ed to derive from redirected natural       to predict vulnerability in captivity:
    mentioned, “prey chase” distances          foraging. Once again, this hypothesis      being migratory or wide‑ranging
    predict pacing in carnivores, sug-         is testable; if correct, natural food      rather than sedentary; specialist
    gesting that mimicking this aspect         handling times or daily foraging time      rather than generalist; and timid
    of foraging could be an effective          budgets should predict tendencies to       rather than bold (reviewed by Mason
    enrichment. However, this analysis         develop these abnormal behaviours.         2010; Mason et al. 2013).
    was based on few species and needs
    replication. More robust and impres-       Timidity. Timid species are fearful and    So do “weed species” succeed eve-
    sive are findings from Müller et al.       stressed even when faced with famil-       rywhere, while others are vulnerable
    (2012) who analysed data from 78           iar threats, showing little habituation.   both in the wild and in zoos? Some
    ruminant species to relate natural         This is problematic when eliciting         preliminary evidence supports this.
    foraging niche to captive survivor-        stimuli are actually harmless. Heini       Jonathan Jeschke and David Strayer
    ship. They cleverly controlled for         Hediger suggested decades ago              collected data on 99 vertebrate spe-
    species differences in normal intrinsic    that differential fearfulness could        cies moved between North America
    life expectancy by calculating the         help explain why zoo life is stress-       and Europe, cataloguing the fates of
    ratios of mean lifespan in captivity       ful for some species but not others.       translocated populations. Together,
    to the maxima ever recorded. Across        Furthermore, recent avian research         Jonathan Jeschke and I investigated
    females (for whom sample sizes             shows that in the wild, species that       whether species coping well with
    were larger and intra‑specific aggres-     are particularly scared of humans          such translocations also apparently
    sion not an issue), “relative lifespans”   have shown the greatest population         adjust readily to captivity. We clas-
    negatively co‑varied with spe-             declines in Europe and are least likely    sified each species as to whether or
    cies‑typical percentages of browse         to colonise cities (reviewed by Mason      not being “commonly captive” (e.g.
    (vegetation from woody plants)             2010; Mason et al. 2013). It thus          often farmed, used in research, held
    in the natural diet, showing that the      seems plausible that timid species
    needs of grazing species have been         may be relatively hard to keep well
    more readily accommodated by zoos          in captivity, while bold species are
    than those of browsers.                    pre‑adapted to succeed; a fascinat-
                                               ing hypothesis to test.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
WAZA                    16 (2015)                                                                                             5

 in zoos or kept by private individuals).   Conclusions                               References
“Common captives” proved signifi-
 cantly more likely to establish after      The diversity of species kept in zoos     • Clubb, R. & Mason, G. J. (2007)
 translocation than species rarely kept     can yield insights into animal welfare      Natural behavioural biology as
 captive, suggesting that similar traits    that are both practically and funda-        a risk factor in carnivore welfare:
 predispose animals to cope with both       mentally important. It is recognised        how understanding species dif-
 types of novel environment (Mason          that not all aspects of natural life        ferences could help zoos improve
 et al. 2013).                              need to be mimicked in captivity, but       enclosures. Applied Animal Be‑
                                            that some – those meeting a spe-            haviour Science 102: 303–328.
However, there are some counter‑ex-         cies’ behavioural needs – should be.      • Mason, G. J. (2010) Species differ-
amples (Mason et al. 2013), and             Comparative approaches can provide          ences in responses to captivity:
Jeanette Kroshko’s research found           objective ways to identify such             stress, welfare and the compara-
no relationships between IUCN Red           needs. They also allow the test of          tive method. Trends in Ecology
List status and captive carnivores’         hypotheses that would be challeng-          and Evolution 25: 713–721.
vulnerability to infant mortality or        ing to test experimentally, and make      • Mason, G., Burn, C. C., Dallaire,
stereotypic behaviour. This could           good, cost effective use of existing        J. A., Kroshko, J., McDonald
reflect differences between taxa; the       data. Practically, their outcomes can       Kinkaid, H. & Jeschke, J. M.
many complex reasons behind threat          help suggest new ways to improve            (2013) Plastic animals in cages:
status; or that differential manage-        husbandry. They could also help             behavioural flexibility and
ment may mask such relationships:           zoos make population management             responses to captivity. Animal
Müller et al. (2012) showed that            decisions. As several recent analy-         Behaviour 85: 1113–1126.
ruminant species managed with               ses reveal, keeping 10,000+ species       • Müller, D. W. H., Bingaman
international studbooks achieve             comes at a price: population sizes          Lackey, L., Streich, W. J., Fickel,
longer relative lifespans, suggest-         are tiny for most. Keeping larger           J., Hatt, J.-M. & Clauss, M. (2011)
ing that threatened taxa receive            populations of fewer species seems          Mating system, feeding type and
more assiduous care. More research          advisable, but which to select is de-       ex situ conservation effort deter-
investigating the hypothesised links        bated. Considering intrinsic abilities      mine life expectancy in captive
between vulnerability in the wild and       to fare well in captivity seems wise        ruminants. Proceedings of the
in zoos is therefore needed, especial-      in such discussions, in order to favour     Royal Society B 278: 2076–2080.
ly given that threatened species are        those inherently most likely to form      • Pomerantz, O., Meiri, S. & Terkel,
under‑represented in zoos; could this       healthy, self‑sustaining populations.       J. (2013) Socio‑ecological factors
perhaps reflect that these species are                                                  correlate with levels of stereo-
intrinsically harder to keep?               The types of comparative approach           typic behavior in zoo‑housed
                                            described here could also yield novel       primates. Behavioural Processes
                                            insights into the evolution of behav-       98: 85–91.
                                            iour and stress responses. For exam-
                                            ple, they could reveal which aspects
                                            of behaviour are flexible, to test
                                            hypotheses about the evolution of
                                            phenotypic plasticity; help elucidate
                                            why species’ responses to anthropo-
                                            genic disturbance vary; and provide
                                            insights into historical domestica-
                                            tions. Data from the diverse species
                                            held in zoos are thus a potentially
                                            superb, largely untapped resource for
                                            fundamental biologists.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
6       WAZA                 16 (2015)

    David Shepherdson1,* & Nadja Wielebnowski1

    The Power of Multi‑institutional
    and Longitudinal Studies for Zoo
    Animal Welfare Research

    Summary                                  Introduction

    Zoo animal welfare research is often     Traditionally, sample size has been       approach. Another study by Wieleb-
    compromised by small sample sizes        the bane of zoo research. For obvious     nowski et al. (2002) on husbandry and
    and a multitude of key variables         reasons, studies tend to have small       exhibit variables related to clouded
    not usually under the control of the     sample sizes and are restricted to        leopards provides another example
    researcher. However, careful study       one or two institutions. These kinds      of the impact this approach can have
    design and the use of multi‑institu-     of studies limit the degree to which      for welfare management of a species.
    tional comparative approaches and/or     findings can be applied or generalised    The primary challenge encountered
    consistent long‑term data collection     to the larger zoo population, either      with the multi‑institutional approach
    on even a small number of individuals    because the circumstances of the          is the logistical complexity and costs
    over time can provide valuable scien-    subjects or the institutions are unique   of working with multiple institutions.
    tific data, which can in turn be used    or because the full range of variables    Data security and ownership can also
    to impact management and care            are not adequately represented in the     be a challenge.
    decisions to improve animal welfare.     dataset.
    We highlight these two approaches                                                  A second approach, perhaps less
    and provide specific examples of their   Here, we explore and give examples        common, is to collect detailed
    impact on animal care and welfare.       of two increasingly popular meth-         and consistent records on smaller
                                             odologies for improving the predic-       numbers of individuals over long
                                             tive power of zoo research. The           periods of time and correlate changes
                                             obvious answer to the problem of          in individuals with changing events
                                             limited sample size is to increase the    over time. Oregon Zoo has been col-
                                             number of animals and institutions        lecting physiological and behavioural
                                             involved in a given study. The study      data from its Asian elephant herd
                                             of stereotypic behaviour in polar         for over 30 years, and Glaeser et al.
                                             bears conducted by Shepherdson            (2012) recently published an analysis
                                             et al. (2013) is one example of this      of female reproductive physiology
                                                                                       in Asian elephants based on this
                                                                                       extensive dataset. A major challenge
                                                                                       with this approach is maintaining
                                                                                       consistency in data collection over
                                                                                       long time periods in spite of changes
                                                                                       in personnel, exhibits, analytical
                                                                                       tools and techniques. In spite of their
                                                                                       respective challenges, both types of
                                                                                       studies illustrate nicely the benefits
                                                                                       of these approaches to allow for solid
                                                                                       scientific research in the zoo environ-
                                                                                       ment that also benefits animal care
                                                                                       and welfare.

    1
        Oregon Zoo, Portland, OR, USA
    *   E‑mail for correspondence:
        david.shepherdson@oregonzoo.org
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
WAZA                   16 (2015)                                                                                               7

                                                                                   Figure 1 © Michael Durham/Oregon Zoo
                                                                                   Polar bear enrichment: working
                                                                                   to obtain food items frozen in ice.

                                                                                     tapes. A challenge of multi‑institu-
                                                                                     tional studies is consistency of data
                                                                                     collection and analysis. In this study,
                                                                                     behavioural data were collected via
                                                                                     video tape at each institution and
                                                                                     subsequently analysed at Oregon
                                                                                     Zoo by a small group of highly
                                                                                     trained observers. Faecal samples,
                                                                                     collected every two weeks, were
                                                                                     sent to Saint Louis Zoo’s endocrine
                                                                                     laboratory for analysis.

                                                                                     Using these techniques, we found
                                                                                     that on average polar bears spend
                                                                                     11% of their time engaged in ste-
                                                                                     reotypic behaviour, but that was
                                                                                     highly variable from institution to
                                                                                     institution. We found that stereotypic
                                                                                     behaviour was negatively correlated
                                                                                     with enrichment, number of bears
                                                                                     in the exhibit and whether bears
                                                                                     could see out of their exhibit. Also,
                                                                                     zoos that practiced positive rein-
                                                                                     forcement training were linked to
                                                                                     lower rates of pacing. Further, we
                                                                                     found that higher faecal glucocor-
                                                                                     ticoid concentrations were associ-
                                                                                     ated with higher rates of pacing and
Multi‑institutional Study                                                            smaller dry land exhibit area.
of Stereotypy in Polar Bears
                                                                                     These results suggest a link between
Polar bears are infamous for their        A total of 20 North American zoos          stress physiology and stereotypic
propensity to display repetitive, ap-     (including Canada and the USA)             behaviour in zoo polar bears. These
parently stereotypic pacing behav-        agreed to partner with us on this          findings also suggest that some eas-
iour in zoo exhibits. The goals of this   year‑long study. Input variables           ily available tools, namely environ-
study were to assess the magnitude        consisted of exhibit, husbandry and        mental enrichment (Fig. 1) and posi-
of the problem, attempt to identify       climate factors. Output variables          tive reinforcement training, may be
environmental correlates of the           were performance of stereotypic            effective at reducing the incidence of
behaviour and investigate poten-          behaviour and faecal glucocorticoid        these behaviours. Exhibit designers
tial links to reduced well‑being and      levels. Input variables were collected     should take note that providing polar
faecal glucocorticoid concentrations      by direct observation and measure-         bears with a view out of their exhibit
(Shepherdson et al. 2013).                ment and by questionnaire surveys,         and larger land areas are associated
                                          with the exception of enrichment           with both behavioural and physi-
                                          that was assessed directly from video      ological benefits.
Towards Positive Animal Welfare - WAZA
8    WAZA                   16 (2015)

    Multi‑institutional Study
    of Adrenal Activity,
    Husbandry and Exhibit
    Variables in Clouded
    Leopards

    Clouded leopards in zoos are known
    to display a variety of behavioural
    problems; for example, fur‑plucking,
    tail‑biting, excessive pacing or hid-
    ing, and severe mate incompatibility.
    These problems generally indicate
    poor animal well‑being and severely
    hinder successful population man-
    agement. In this study, we collected
    hormone and behaviour data for
    several weeks on 74 clouded leopards
    at 12 zoos (Wielebnowski et al. 2002).

    Using non‑invasive faecal hormone
    monitoring, we found that some of
    the observed behavioural problems
    were associated with an increase
    in faecal glucocorticoid concentra-
    tions. This increase, in turn, was cor-                                            Figure 2 © Michael Durham/Oregon Zoo
    related with the following variables:                                              Multi‑generational herd of Asian elephants
    exhibit height, predator visibility                                                at Oregon Zoo: social interactions and
    and public display. For example, the      Three Decades                            soft substrates are important for
                                                                                       elephant well‑being.
    higher the exhibit the lower the fae-     of Physiological and
    cal glucocorticoid concentrations;        Behavioural Monitoring
    and, if predators were visible and ani-   of Asian Elephants
    mals were on constant public display,     at Oregon Zoo
    faecal glucocorticoid concentrations
    were higher.                              Oregon Zoo has a long‑standing           Asian elephants (Glaeser et al. 2012).
                                              history of contributing substantially    It showed that cycle length, while
    In a follow‑up study, we tested the       to the global knowledge on Asian         relatively consistent over an individ-
    impact of changes in exhibit features     elephant reproduction. For example,      ual’s lifespan, can vary substantially
    experimentally (Butterworth et al.        the Asian elephant reproductive cycle    between individual elephants. The
    2011). Available exhibit height was       was first characterised at Oregon Zoo    average Asian elephant cycle is about
    altered through adding or removing        (Hess et al. 1981). Indeed, Oregon       16 weeks long.
    climbing structures, and the amount       Zoo has the longest continuous
    and type of available hiding spaces       dataset on female and male Asian         Because of our long‑term hormone
    was changed. Data showed that             elephant reproductive hormones ever      monitoring of several successful
    adding both climbing structure and        collected and analysed, and data col-    pregnancies over time, we can also
    hiding space resulted in a decline of     lection continues to this date.          predict the due date for an upcoming
    faecal glucocorticoid concentrations                                               birth within a few days of the event
    in the study animals. Once published,     Based on the long‑term physiologi-       and we can measure signs of impend-
    these data were also used to inform       cal data obtained through weekly         ing birth using hormonal indicators.
    the husbandry guidelines for the          blood sample collection on all of        In addition, this in‑depth physi-
    management of this species.               our elephants, it was found that         ological monitoring together with
                                              zoo elephants were able to become        behavioural observations is used to
                                              mature and ready to conceive much        inform the social management of our
                                              earlier than their wild counterparts.    elephant herd (currently seven ele-
                                              For example, males can breed suc-        phants, three males and four females;
                                              cessfully as early as five years of      Fig. 2). Female cycles are monitored
                                              age in zoos, while males in the wild     to ensure that they are with a breed-
                                              become sexually mature at around         ing male only when appropriate and
                                              11 years. A recent publication summa-    desirable.
                                              rised 30 years of data on eight female
WAZA                   16 (2015)                                                                                              9

Monitoring the bulls helps us to           Conclusions                                References
establish the musth cycles and bulls
can also be managed accordingly            Multi‑institutional comparative as        • Butterworth, A., Mench, J. A. &
to avoid unwanted breeding and             well as longitudinal study designs          Wielebnowski, N. (2011) Practi-
aggression. Based on our hormone           have yielded much valuable informa-         cal strategies to assess (and
studies, we know that a strong surge       tion and are certainly key tools for        improve) welfare. In: Animal
of testosterone occurs during musth.       the study of zoo animal welfare. To         Welfare, 2nd ed. (ed. by Appleby,
Since we have been monitoring male         succeed with both of these types of         M. C., Mench, J. A., Olsson, I. A.
elephants of all ages, we were able        studies, the zoo community needs to         S. & Hughes, B. O.), pp. 200–214.
to document the onset of musth             be actively supporting and funding          Wallingford: CABI Publishing.
in young males and correlate the           projects of this type. Zoos, espe-        • Glaeser, S. S., Hunt, K. E., Martin,
hormonal data with onset of sexual         cially medium‑ to larger‑sized zoos,        M. S., Finnegan, M. & Brown, J. L.
and other social behaviours. Fur-          need to make an effort to establish         (2012) Investigation of individual
thermore, we found that our adult          an active “on grounds” research             and group variability in estrous
males tend to experience their musth       department with qualified research          cycle characteristics in female
cycles around the same time. Indeed,       staff to lead such study efforts.           Asian elephants (Elephas maxi‑
one male coming into musth appears         Also, engaging the academic com-            mus) at the Oregon Zoo. Theriog‑
to trigger other bulls to start their      munity that most zoo researchers            enology 78: 285–296.
musth cycle. It is not known yet what      are already linked to is an impor-        • Hess, D. L., Schmidt, M. J. &
exactly triggers this synchronisation      tant part of ensuring that valuable         Schmidt, A. M. (1981) Endocrine
and why it is beneficial for bulls to      scientific data that can be published       and behavioral comparisons dur-
synchronise musth. We are in the           in peer‑reviewed journals is properly       ing the reproductive cycle in the
process of analysing the long‑term         collected, analysed and disseminated        Asian elephant Elephas maximus.
dataset on males and hope to learn         in a timely fashion.                        Biology of Reproduction 24 (Suppl.
even more details about musth                                                          1): 93A.
cycles and male reproduction during        A recently completed, large‑scale         • Shepherdson, D., Lewis, K. D.,
the next few years.                        multi‑institutional study exemplifies       Carlstead, K., Bauman, J. &
                                           what can be accomplished if exten-          Perrin, N. (2013) Individual and
Most recently we have started analy-       sive collaboration and zoo support          environmental factors associated
ses of past and current samples for        is garnered. The elephant welfare           with stereotypic behavior and
cortisol and we plan to look at these      project, described in more detail by        fecal glucocorticoid metabolite
data in combination with behaviour         Carlstead & Brown (this issue), paves       levels in zoo housed polar bears.
and correlate them with various            the way for integrating previous            Applied Animal Behaviour Science
life events and husbandry variables.       approaches in zoo welfare research.         147: 268–277.
A benefit of taking part in the el-        North American zoo researchers            • Wielebnowski, N. C., Fletchall,
ephant welfare project (see below) is      from different fields came together         N., Carlstead, K., Busso, J. M. &
that we can now add movement and           to address a welfare science need           Brown, J. L. (2002) Noninvasive
recumbence using GPS devices and           (addressing the question of welfare         assessment of adrenal activity
accelerometers inserted into ankle         status and associated variables             associated with husbandry and
bracelets. Our long‑term monitoring        in elephants in North American zoos)        behavioral factors in the North
approach has therefore not only pro-       and engaged with prominent welfare          American clouded leopard popu-
vided us with the ability to effectively   scientists at several universities. The     lation. Zoo Biology 21: 77–98.
manage our herd with regards to            Association of Zoos and Aquariums
reproductive and social health and         (AZA) supported the approach and
welfare, but has also yielded much         helped to ensure wide participation
valuable scientific data over the          of its member institutions. Indeed,       Moving forward, a study of this
years that contribute to the general       almost all AZA zoos holding el-           type integrates both approaches for
knowledge base of Asian elephant           ephants participated in this study of     the future: multi‑institutional and
biology, physiology and behaviour          unprecedented scale and scope. Due        longitudinal monitoring on a major
worldwide.                                 to the fact that the study contained      sector of a zoo animal population
                                           almost the entire North American          to establish a baseline of welfare
                                           zoo population of African and Asian       indicators and answer questions of
                                           elephants, it was decided that data       the relative impact of various man-
                                           collected in this study can serve         agement variables on welfare. We
                                           to provide a baseline of AZA zoo          hope that these examples will spark
                                           elephant welfare similar to epidemio-     additional welfare research on small
                                           logical models provided by human          and large scales and lead to increased
                                           medicine studies.                         global networks of zoo and university
                                                                                     welfare researchers working jointly
                                                                                     to advance the emerging field of zoo
                                                                                     animal welfare science.
10       WAZA                     16 (2015)

     Kathy Carlstead1,* & Janine Brown2

     Using Science to Understand
     Zoo Elephant Welfare:
     Evaluation of Ovarian Cyclicity and
     Keeper–Elephant Relationships

     Summary                                        Asking the Elephants

     Significant attention has been                 The science of zoo animal welfare is       to a broad range of facility‑based
     directed towards the housing and               developing into a field that guides        measures (input variables). Data for
     care of Asian and African elephants            improvements in animal husbandry           individual elephants were collected
     in zoos, and many questions have               through research that is multi‑dis-        on‑site by zoo personnel, including
     been raised about whether they                 ciplinary and multi‑institutional.         photographs, video recordings, blood
     can meet elephants’ environmental              A team of zoo scientists, academic         and faecal samples, veterinary ex-
     and social needs in a manner that              consultants and elephant managers          aminations and technology‑assisted
     promotes good welfare. Four years              set up a study to evaluate Asian and       observations of elephant movement.
     ago, a research team set out to evalu-         African elephant welfare in Associa-
     ate the welfare of elephants in zoos           tion of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA)-ac-       The research made extensive use
     accredited by the Association of Zoos          credited zoos (Mellen et al. 2008) by      of survey data obtained from staff
     and Aquariums (AZA). Data were                 analysing animal‑based data on a va-       members at each zoo. Elephant man-
     collected on elephants’ daily lives,           riety of welfare indicators. In a sense    agers were asked to provide detailed
     how they interact with each other              we were asking the elephants what          descriptions of facilities and manage-
     and their keepers, and their physi-            environmental and husbandry factors        ment practices (e.g. exhibit space,
     ological function. An epidemiological          are the most important for enhanc-         social groupings, flooring, feeding
     approach was then taken to evaluate            ing their welfare. With a 94% com-         methods, training and handling pro-
     husbandry and management factors               mitment from AZA zoos holding              cedures, veterinary care programme,
     impacting welfare by looking at asso-          elephants, the team collected a large      enrichment programme) at each zoo
     ciations between resource‑based en-            amount of data on 266 elephants            for each elephant, since not all indi-
     vironmental/life‑history variables and         in 70 zoos. The study was funded by        viduals at a zoo experience the same
     animal‑based indicators of welfare.            a grant from the Institute of Museum       housing and/or procedures. Keepers
     We present a brief overview of two of          and Library Services.                      provided information about their
     our findings: factors associated with                                                     own attitudes towards and opinions
     reproductive cycle status in females,          An epidemiological approach was            of elephants, and their relationships
     and the quality of relationships be-           used to examine multiple input varia-      with elephants.
     tween keepers and elephants.                   bles that predict indicators of welfare
                                                    (Carlstead et al. 2012). The welfare       Here, we review some of the study
                                                    framework we used for elephants            results relevant to reproductive
                                                    consisted of “gold standard” welfare       function in female elephants, and
                                                    indicators, as well as some new meas-      the quality of relationships between
                                                    ures that were developed and vali-         keepers and elephants. More results
                                                    dated. This multi‑factorial approach       of the study have been submitted
                                                    to welfare was intended to provide         as a series of papers for concurrent
                                                    a more holistic view of best manage-       publication in the open‑access journal
                                                    ment practices for elephants. All          PLoS ONE.
                                                    welfare indicators (outcomes) were
                                                    animal‑based (e.g. body condition,
     1
         Honolulu Zoo Society, Honolulu,            foot and joint health, walking rates
         HI, USA (formerly)                         and recumbence, stereotypy, ovarian
     2
         Center for Species Survival, Smithsonian   cycling, prolactin, cortisol) and were
         Conservation Biology Institute,            statistically analysed, using univariate
         Front Royal, VA, USA                       and multivariable models, in relation
     *   E‑mail for correspondence:
         kcarlstead@honzoosoc.org
WAZA                 16 (2015)                                                                                               11

  Figure 1                                                                       Examples of concentrations
                                                                                 of serum progesterone in
                                                                                 (a) normal cycling, (b) irregular cycling
                                                                                 and (c) non‑cycling elephants.
         a

                                                                                  making a positive appraisal of the
                                                                                  quality of the zoo environment when
                                                                                  diverse enrichment provides dynamic,
         b                                                                        changing surroundings, stimulating
                                                                                  investigation and other behaviours.
                                                                                  In wild savannah elephants, similar
                                                                                  positive appraisals of a changing en-
                                                                                  vironment, particularly with respect
                                                                                  to cues signalling incipient water and
                                                                                  forage availability, are important for
                                                                                  stimulating ovarian cycling when
                                                                                  conditions are favourable for concep-
         c                                                                        tion and pregnancy (Wittemyer et
                                                                                  al. 2007).

                                                                                  Univariate results indicated that
                                                                                  social experience was also associ-
                                                                                  ated with ovarian cycling for female
                                                                                  African elephants. Females that
                                                                                  are socially separated, even if they
                                                                                  have access to one or more other
                                                                                  elephants through a barrier, have
                                                                                  a decreased chance of cycling. We
                                                                                  know that sociality is important for
                                                                                  elephants behaviourally, and this sug-
Management Factors                                                                gests that it also may be important
Associated with Ovarian                                                           physiologically in terms of supporting
Cycles of Female Elephants                                                        normal ovarian function.

Reproductive success contributes        Rates of normal cycling, non‑cycling      Although a larger percentage of
to the sustainability of zoo elephant   and irregular cycling were 73.2, 22.5     female Asian elephants had normal
populations. Lack of reproductive       and 4.2% for Asian elephants, and         ovarian cycles, univariate analyses
function has long been considered an    48.4, 37.9 and 13.7% for African          indicated that more time spent
indicator of reduced welfare. Ques-     elephants, respectively, all of which     in spaces with free choice of being
tions were asked about the impacts      differed between species (Brown et        indoors or outdoors was associated
that zoo environments may have on       al. 2015). Age was strongly associated    with an increased likelihood of nor-
the biological systems regulating       with an increased risk of non‑cycling     mal cycles. Time spent in spaces with
reproductive activity. Some female      for both species. In addition, three      free indoor/outdoor choice was also
elephants in AZA zoos fail to show      management factors were found to          negatively correlated with faecal cor-
normal 13–17 weeks ovarian cycles,      be related to ovarian cycle status:       tisol concentrations for female Asian
which may be influenced by social       enrichment and sociality for African      elephants, and older, non‑cycling
and/or environmental management         elephants, and choice of space for        females had higher serum cortisol
practices. For this study, bi‑weekly    Asian elephants.                          values compared to cycling females
blood samples were collected from                                                 of the same age. While not conclusive
75 Asian and 93 African female          We used a diversity index of enrich-      until further experimental research is
elephants of reproductive age (8–55     ment that takes into account the type     conducted, this is a suggestion that
years) over a 12-month period for       of enrichment used and the frequen-       being able to move freely between
analysis of serum progestogens and      cy of its use. Multivariable modelling    the indoor and outdoor areas of
prolactin (Fig. 1).                     indicated that enrichment diversity       exhibits might reduce stress that
                                        increases the chance that a female        interferes with normal reproductive
                                        African elephant will cycle normally.     function in elephants.
                                        This is possibly due to elephants
12    WAZA                     16 (2015)

     Figure 2 © Manny Dayao
     A Honolulu Zoo elephant keeper makes a shower more enriching
     for Mari and Vaigai by spraying them from a novel location.

                                                                                              Keeper–Elephant
                                                                                              Relationships

     In addition to ovarian cycling, we             Using multivariable modelling, we         The quality of relationships between
     also evaluated prolactin as another            identified four input variables related   humans and animals can be consid-
     welfare indicator. Prolactin is a pi-          to the manifestation of this endo-        ered positive, neutral or negative for
     tuitary hormone that has over 300              crine disorder among female African       animal welfare. Negative impacts of
     known functions involving multiple             elephants. Age was positively associ-     human–animal relationships are well
     systems, including reproduction and            ated with the chance that a female        known for farm animals, with studies
     stress. In other species, prolactin has        will be hyperprolactinemic, as was        showing that caretakers’ attitudes
     direct and indirect effects on follicle        being exposed to a higher number          and behaviour towards their ani-
     development, and is finely regulated           of social groups. The latter may be       mals can cause fear and stress in the
     by positive and negative feedback              related to social instability, a known    animals (reviewed by Waiblinger et al.
     mechanisms. Too much prolactin,                stressor for elephants. We also found     2006). Therefore, we used elephant
     called hyperprolactinemia, is a com-           that increased enrichment diversity       stress responses to keepers as an
     mon cause of infertility in women,             and feeding diversity (food presented     indicator of welfare. We character-
     and is associated with a disruption            in a foraging device, hidden or hang-     ised the quality of the relationships
     in normal ovarian cycle activity.              ing) is associated with a female being    between elephants and their caretak-
     Hyperprolactinemia was absent                  less likely to have hyperprolactine-      ers by comparing keeper attitudes
     in female Asian elephants, but 53% of          mia. Again, seeing these links with       towards and opinions of elephants
     non‑cycling female African elephants           environmental enrichment indicates        with varying levels of elephant serum
     were hyperprolactinemic. By contrast,          that having more things to do in ex-      cortisol measured in response to
     no cycling females exhibited consist-          hibits, including having to look or       the blood sampling procedure by
     ently elevated prolactin, so there             work harder for food, may promote         keepers. Keepers’ perceptions about
     appears to be a relationship between           more normal physiological function        and attitudes towards elephants
     this condition and compromised                 in elephants.                             were assessed via a questionnaire
     ovarian activity in female African
     elephants.
WAZA                   16 (2015)                                                                                                 13

                                           Zoo Elephant Welfare,                      References
                                           Going Forward
                                                                                      • Brown, J. L., Paris, S., Pra-
from which their answers were then         From our study of elephant welfare           do‑Oviedo, N., Meehan, C.,
grouped into a smaller number of fac-      in North American zoos we are gain-          Hogan, J. N., Morfeld, K. &
tors that describe their interactions      ing tools to help improve conditions         Carlstead, K. (2015) Reproduc-
with elephants. The keeper factors         for our elephants. The results re-           tive health assessment of female
were averaged for all keepers at a zoo     ported here suggest that zoos should         elephants in North American
and then entered into a multivari-         commit to robust environmental               zoos and association of husband-
able model predicting serum cortisol       enrichment and feeding programmes            ry practices with reproductive
values of individual elephants.            that increase both the types of meth-        dysfunction in African elephants
                                           ods employed and the frequency               (Loxodonta africana). PLoS ONE,
Lower stress responses in African          of implementation, and continue              in review.
elephants were associated with             careful selection of elephant keepers      • Carlstead, K., Mench, J. A.,
having keepers who reported a high         with positive attitudes and empathy          Meehan, C. & Brown, J. L. (2013)
degree of interactions with elephants      towards elephants.                           An epidemiological approach
that are positive (versus neutral or                                                    to welfare research in zoos: the
negative). Keepers also answered           It is important to remember that             elephant welfare project. Journal
questions on the Davis Interpersonal       welfare is complex, especially for           of Applied Animal Welfare Science
Reactivity Index, a widely used re-        elephants, and many of the factors           16: 319–337.
search tool to assess human‑directed       that we evaluated are interrelated.        • Mellen, J. D., Barber, J. C. & Miller,
empathy. Higher scores of keepers          Additional experimental studies are          G. W. (2008) Can we assess the
for “empathic concern” were corre-         needed to tease apart what input fac-        needs of elephants in zoos? Can
lated with lower cortisol concentra-       tors are most important for specific         we meet the needs of elephants
tions in African elephants. For Asian      welfare outcomes. Hopefully, using           in zoos? In: Elephants and Ethics:
elephants, the more the keeper             science to provide valid evidence            Toward a Morality of Coexist‑
believes in the importance of positive     of welfare impacts will guide our            ence (ed. by Wemmer, C. M. &
physical contact between keeper and        decisions around next steps to con-          Christen, C. A.), pp. 307–324.
elephant, such as rubbing, touching        tinue our focus on the welfare of all        Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
and petting, the lower elephants’          elephants in zoos.                           University Press.
stress responses. This was irrespec-                                                  • Waiblinger, S., Boivin, X., Peders-
tive of whether contact with the           Acknowledgements                             en, V., Tosi, M.-V., Janczak, A. M.,
elephants was under protected or                                                        Visser, E. K. & Jones, R. B. (2006)
free‑handling conditions. Although         Project principal investigators: Anne        Assessing the human–animal
we were not able to directly assess        Baker, Janine Brown, Kathy Carlstead,        relationship in farmed species:
keeper behaviours towards elephants,       Candice Dorsey, Mike Keele, Jill Mel-        a critical review. Applied Animal
we did find that self‑reports of talking   len, Joy Mench, David Shepherdson            Behaviour Science 101: 185–242.
to elephants casually or using verbal      and Nadja Wielebnowski. Project            • Wittemyer, G., Ganswindt, A. &
commands when training are be-             manager: Cheryl Meehan. Academic             Hodges, K. (2007) The impact of
haviours correlated to these positive      consultants: Georgia Mason, Anne             ecological variability on the re-
keeper attitudes.                          Marie de Passillé and Jeff Rushen. Ad-       productive endocrinology of wild
                                           ditional researchers: Christie Alligood,     female African elephants. Hor‑
Therefore, these results suggest that      Mary Bonaparte‑Saller, Brian Greco,          mones and Behavior 51: 346–354.
keepers shape the quality of their         Jennifer Hogan, Mathew Holdgate,
relationships with elephants, with the     Katherine Leighty, Lance Miller, Kari
opportunity to enhance elephant wel-       Morfeld, Natalia Prado‑Oviedo and
fare with their perceptions, attitudes     Joseph Soltis.
and behaviours towards them (Fig. 2).
Continued study of keeper–elephant
relationships could eventually help
inform keeper selection and training.
14       WAZA                     16 (2015)

     Lance J. Miller1,*, Randall S. Wells1, Rita Stacey1, F. William Zeigler1,
     Jessica C. Whitham1 & Michael Adkesson1

     Animal Welfare Management of
     Bottlenose Dolphins at the Chicago
     Zoological Society’s Brookfield Zoo

     Summary                                  Preventative Health                      Animal Training
                                              Programme
     In 1961, the Chicago Zoological Soci-                                            Positive reinforcement training is the
     ety’s Brookfield Zoo became the first    As part of our ongoing animal welfare primary tool, coupled with strong
     inland facility to exhibit bottlenose    management, a robust preventative       trainer–animal relationships, used
     dolphins for public viewing. Since       health programme plays a critical       to assist with animal husbandry and
     then, dolphin welfare has improved       role in maintaining optimal animal      veterinary procedures. Our preventa-
     due to increases in knowledge about      welfare. Bottlenose dolphins at the     tive health programme sampling is
     day‑to‑day husbandry, advances           Chicago Zoological Society’s Brook-     accomplished through conditioning
     in medical technology and research       field Zoo are assessed daily by animal the animals to voluntarily partici-
     on veterinary care, husbandry and        care staff and receive semi‑annual      pate. Having the animals participate
     wild dolphins. These efforts allow the   preventative veterinary exams. Diag-    in their own care gives them more
     Chicago Zoological Society to take       nostic testing is performed quarterly   choice and control within their en-
     a holistic approach to the animal        and includes comprehensive blood        vironment, two aspects thought to
     welfare management of dolphins.          tests, ultrasound examinations and      enhance animal welfare. Training ani-
     This involves a collaboration of many    cytological examinations of chuff       mals for these different procedures
     departments (animal care, veterinary     (breath exhalate), gastric and faecal   also allows for better development of
     services, behavioural husbandry,         samples. Weekly measurements of         human–animal relationships that can
     nutrition, animal welfare research       weight and blubber thickness as well    benefit animal welfare.
     and field research) working to-          as monthly length and girth measure-
     gether to ensure that each individual    ments are collected to ensure that      In addition to the dolphins participat-
     dolphin thrives. From operating the      animals fall within body condition      ing in their preventative health pro-
     world’s longest‑running study of         norms developed from wild bottle-       gramme, they also participate in our
     a wild dolphin population in Sarasota    nose dolphins. This extensive data-    “Dolphins in Action” programme.
     Bay, FL and the extensive experience     base of medical information specific    This educational programme allows
     gained through the management            to each dolphin provides a power-       visitors to see the dolphins engaged
     of animals at Brookfield Zoo, the        ful tool for assessing any changes      in species‑appropriate behaviours
     Chicago Zoological Society has an        in animal health or welfare, which we   with the goal of connecting them
     unmatched wealth of knowledge on         consider best practice in monitoring    to nature. Previous research has
     the care and welfare of bottlenose       dolphins in zoos and aquariums.         demonstrated that these types of
     dolphins.                                                                        programmes are potentially reward-
                                                                                      ing for the animals by giving them
                                                                                      more stimulation, choice and control
                                                                                      within their environment (Miller et al.
                                                                                      2011). Thus, positive reinforcement
                                                                                      training can provide many animal
                                                                                      welfare benefits.

     1
         Chicago Zoological Society,
         Brookfield, IL, USA
     *   E‑mail for correspondence:
         lance.miller@czs.org
WAZA                       16 (2015)                                                                                                      15

Figure 1 © Chicago Zoological Society
Three generations of bottlenose dolphins at the Chicago Zoological Society’s Brookfield Zoo.

Social Enrichment                                                                              Nutrition

While other enrichment opportuni-                 Maintaining stability of the group is        Research on the diets of bottlenose
ties are provided (ice blocks, balls,             important for social dynamics. This,         dolphins under professional care have
etc.), it is likely that a species‑ap-            in turn, aids in improving the learning      been found to differ from wild bottle-
propriate social environment is the               experience through familiarity and           nose dolphins (Slifka et al. 2013). This
best form of enrichment for dolphins.             acceptance of each animal within the         has led to further research examin-
As social mammals, they engage                    social grouping. For example, expe-          ing the impact of those diets on the
in a wide variety of interactions, and            rienced females raising young act as         health and welfare of the dolphins at
the number of potential interactions              role models for daughters, nieces            Brookfield Zoo. Iron storage dis-
increases exponentially with increas-             and grand‑daughters that have yet            ease is recognised in dolphins under
ing group size. We make every effort              to reproduce. Inexperienced females          professional care. It was thought that
to try to recreate naturalistic social            often share in the parenting process,        differences in the diets of managed
groupings based on findings from                  providing them with valuable lessons         versus wild dolphins could affect the
our long‑term field research (e.g.                they will need later in life.                incidence of this disease. However,
Wells 2014). Typical composition of                                                            examination of the iron levels in the
the dolphins at Brookfield Zoo over                                                            species of fish commonly fed to the
the past two decades has included                                                              dolphins at Brookfield Zoo suggests
nursery groups of mothers and their                                                            that they receive equal to, or less
offspring from multiple generations,                                                           than, their wild counterparts. Further
along with individual or paired breed-                                                         research is needed to understand
ing males (Fig. 1).                                                                            how iron storage disease and differ-
                                                                                               ences in diets may impact the health
                                                                                               and welfare of dolphins. The Chicago
                                                                                               Zoological Society is continuing ef-
                                                                                               forts to optimise animal welfare and
                                                                                               has altered diets to better reflect the
                                                                                               diversity, type and seasonal changes
                                                                                               of species common to bottlenose
                                                                                               dolphins in Sarasota Bay, FL.
You can also read