Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility

Page created by Ralph Tyler
 
CONTINUE READING
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility
Please do not remove this page

Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of
Cognitive Flexibility
Kupis, Lauren
https://scholarship.miami.edu/discovery/delivery/01UOML_INST:ResearchRepository/12381228840002976?l#13381228830002976

Kupis. (2021). Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility [University of Miami].
https://scholarship.miami.edu/discovery/fulldisplay/alma991031583588402976/01UOML_INST:ResearchR
epository

Open
Downloaded On 2021/12/09 11:10:49 -0500

Please do not remove this page
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility
UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI

BRAIN DYNAMICS UNDERLYING THE DEVELOPMENT OF COGNITIVE
                     FLEXIBILITY

                                 By

                           Lauren Kupis

                            A THESIS

                       Submitted to the Faculty
                     of the University of Miami
            in partial fulfillment of the requirements for
                  the degree of Master of Science

                       Coral Gables, Florida

                            August 2021
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility
©2021
   Lauren Kupis
All Rights Reserved
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility
UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI

                       A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of
                           the requirements for the degree of
                                   Master of Science

    BRAIN DYNAMICS UNDERLYING THE DEVELOPMENT OF COGNITIVE
                         FLEXIBILITY

                                     Lauren Kupis

Approved:

Lucina Uddin, Ph.D.                                   Aaron Heller, Ph.D.
Professor of Psychology                               Professor of Psychology

Manish Saggar, Ph.D.                                  Guillermo Prado, Ph.D.
Stanford University                                   Dean of the Graduate School
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development of Cognitive Flexibility
LAUREN KUPIS                                                           (M.S., Psychology)
Brain Dynamics Underlying the Development                                 (August 2021)
of Cognitive Flexibility

Abstract of a thesis at the University of Miami.

Thesis supervised by Professor Lucina Uddin.
No. of pages in text. (68)

       Cognitive flexibility, or the ability to mentally switch according to changing

environmental demands, supports optimal outcomes across development. Despite the

importance of cognitive flexibility for development, little is known regarding the neural

mechanisms underlying it. The goal of the current study was to uncover developmental

differences in the neural systems supporting cognitive flexibility using a functional MRI

task designed to elicit switching mechanisms in both children and adults and by using a

novel method called co-activation pattern (CAP) analysis. The current study examined

neural differences in brain activation and dynamic brain states between children and

adults during a cognitive flexibility task, and the relationships between brain dynamics

and behavior. The CAP analysis revealed that children as compared with adults dwelled

longer in brain states consisting of hybrid brain states consisting of between-network

coupling. Additionally, in both children and adults, more frequent occurrence of a brain

state consisting of coupling between the default and central executive networks was

associated with better cognitive flexibility as measured by the Behavior Rating Inventory

of Executive Function. This study provides the first evidence of the developmental

changes associated with brain dynamic changes during cognitive flexibility and links

brain function with a real-world measure of cognitive flexibility, thereby paving the way
for future research of neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by atypical cognitive

flexibility.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

                                                                                                                                  Page

LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................................................. iv

LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................................... v

Chapter

     1     BACKGROUND ......................................................................................................... 1
           Cognitive flexibility… ................................................................................................. 1
           Executive functions needed to implement cognitive flexibility… .................................. 6
           Development of cognitive flexibility ............................................................................ 11
           Brain network findings in cognitive flexibility.............................................................. 19
           Specific aims and hypotheses ....................................................................................... 23

     2     METHODS ................................................................................................................. 26
           Behavioral measures ................................................................................................... 27
           Data acquisition .......................................................................................................... 30
           Data preprocessing ...................................................................................................... 30
           Analytic plan............................................................................................................... 32

     3     RESULTS................................................................................................................... 34
           Brain activation........................................................................................................... 34
           Co-activation pattern analysis (CAP)........................................................................... 38
           Age differences in CAPs ............................................................................................. 38
           Brain-behavior relationships with CAPs ...................................................................... 39

     4     DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................. 41
           Brain activation........................................................................................................... 41
           Co-activation pattern analysis (CAP)........................................................................... 43
           Limitations and future directions ................................................................................. 48
           Conclusion .................................................................................................................. 49

FIGURES ................................................................................................................................ 50

REFERENCES ......................................................................................................................... 56

                                                                    iii
LIST OF FIGURES
FIGURE 1……………………………………………………………………………..   50

FIGURE 2……………………………………………………………………………..   51

FIGURE 3……………………………………………………………………………..   52

FIGURE 4……………………………………………………………………………..   53

FIGURE 5……………………………………………………………………………..   54

                       iv
LIST OF TABLES

TABLE 1……………………………………………………………………………..   26

TABLE 2……………………………………………………………………………..   35

                       v
CHAPTER 1: BACKGROUND

       Flexible cognition and behavior are required to adaptively respond to changing

environmental demands. This process is enabled by cognitive flexibility, the mental

readiness to switch to initiate flexible behavioral responses (Dajani & Uddin, 2015).

Cognitive flexibility is a core feature of executive functioning (Diamond, 2013; Logue &

Gould, 2014) and is associated with positive life outcomes including the successful

transition into adulthood (Burt & Paysnick, 2012), resilience to negative life events

(Genet & Siemer, 2011), and better quality of life (Davis et al., 2010). Cognitive

flexibility is also critical for developmental outcomes, including reading and math skills

(Yeniad et al., 2013), social competence (Ciairano et al., 2006), and overall academic

achievement (Titz & Karbach, 2014). Despite the importance of cognitive flexibility

across the lifespan, little is known regarding the neural mechanisms supporting the

development of this ability.

Cognitive flexibility

       Examples of cognitive flexibility include the ability to think differently about a

situation, and quickly switch between tasks or strategies to solve a problem. Being

flexible ultimately aids creativity (Lu et al., 2017, 2019), problem-solving (Ionescu,

2012), learning (Kehagia et al., 2010), and resilience to negative life events (Genet &

Siemer, 2011). Cognitive flexibility benefits adaptation via the ability to quickly

transition from different activities or change perspective. Therefore, cognitive flexibility

is an important feature of daily functioning.

       Cognitive flexibility is also associated with positive life outcomes including

academic achievement such as reading and math skills (Yeniad et al., 2013), and the

                                                1
2

successful transition into adulthood (Burt & Paysnick, 2012). Conversely, cognitive

inflexibility may be a risk factor for repetitive or ruminative thoughts (Deveney &

Deldin, 2006; Genet et al., 2013; Whitmer & Banich, 2007), which underlie

psychological disorders such as anxiety, depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder

(OCD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Keenan et al., 2018; McDougle et al.,

1995; van Steensel et al., 2011). Ultimately, cognitive flexibility is important during

childhood and adolescence because these periods are accompanied by learning,

susceptibility to psychological disorders (e.g., anxiety and depression; (Côté et al., 2009),

and increased substance use (Rose et al., 2019). In all cases, cognitive flexibility may

buffer against negative effects.

       Although cognitive flexibility contributes to positive adaptation and learning

across development, the underlying brain regions supporting developmental changes of

cognitive flexibility are not fully understood. Characterizing the brain regions involved in

cognitive flexibility across development may clarify the mechanisms underlying the

cognitive and behavioral changes observed.

       The lateral frontoparietal network (FPN) has been found to be important in the

development of executive function broadly (Dajani & Uddin, 2015). Further, flexible

interactions among brain regions and neural networks may contribute to greater cognitive

flexibility (Nomi, Bolt, et al., 2017). However, these neuroimaging findings are primarily

in adults, and reflect the neural processes associated with mature cognitive flexibility. A

more complete understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility

may lead to the creation of targeted treatments for neurodevelopmental disorders, the
3

        updating of classroom curricula, and the creation of individualized preventative

measures for psychological disorders or substance use (Lucina Q. Uddin, 2021).

Terminology

         A variety of terminologies exist to describe tasks that have been developed to

study cognitive flexibility. Cognitive flexibility is first considered to be the ‘umbrella’

term describing a cognitive construct that is investigated using ‘set-shifting’ and ‘task

switching’ tasks (Konishi et al., 1998; Monsell, 2003). Relatedly, cognitive flexibility is

referred to as ‘shifting’ in latent models of components of executive function (A. Miyake

et al., 2000).

Task switching and set shifting

        Two types of tasks commonly used to assess cognitive flexibility are ‘task

switching’ and ‘set-shifting’ paradigms. Task switching paradigms involve alternating

between tasks, and performance is measured by a ‘switch cost’, or the difference in task

performance (e.g., reaction time) in switching versus non-switching task blocks (Monsell,

2003; Vandierendonck et al., 2010). An example of task switching are rule-switching

tasks, which require participants to switch their response selection (or task) based on the

presented rule (Wendelken et al., 2012). Set-shifting, on the other hand, involves shifting

or switching within a task (Dajani & Uddin, 2015). For example, a commonly used set-

shifting task requires participants to shift attention between color and shape dimensions

and choose the unique attribute (Casey et al., 2004). Although these tasks differ in terms

of switching within versus between tasks, they both are thought to rely on cognitive

flexibility. However, these subtle differences may have important implications in the

neural processes underlying the different types of shifting processes occurring.
4

        There are different types of task switching and set-shifting paradigms used to

study cognitive flexibility in developmental neuroimaging studies. Broadly, the

categories of these various tasks include: attention-shifting, complex set-shifting, rule-

switching, and performance-based switching tasks. These tasks are often adaptations of

common neuropsychological test batteries that assess cognitive flexibility including the

Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS; Zelazo, 2006), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task

(WCST; Heaton RK et al., 1993), and shifting tasks within the Delis-Kaplan Executive

Function System (D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2001). In the DCCS, usually given to children

between 3 and 5 years, children are presented with two cards (e.g., blue rabbit and red

boat) and are asked to match a series of cards to the two cards based on one dimension

such as color or shape (e.g., blue boat is matched with the blue rabbit) (Zelazo, 2006).

The WCST can be given to individuals from 6.5 to 89 years of age, and requires cards to

be sorted based on one of three dimensions (color, shape, or number) and update the

sorting criteria based on the experimenter’s feedback (Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley

JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G, 1993). Lastly, the D-KEFS consists of several tasks (color-word

interference, card sorting, design fluency, and verbal fluency tasks) that test shifting

abilities.

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging cognitive flexibility tasks

        Four general categories of fMRI tasks that have been used to study cognitive

flexibility include: attention shifting, complex set-shifting, rule-switching, and

performance-based switching tasks. These tasks range in difficulty in that some have only

one task demand (e.g., attention) or include many demands (e.g., attention, working

memory, and switching). The fMRI cognitive flexibility tasks also differ greatly in the
5

type of switching required (e.g., attention versus rule-based switching). The following

section describes in more detail these categories of tasks.

Attention Shifting

       In attention shifting tasks, participants shift attention between stimuli dimensions

(e.g., color, shape) within a trial. This type of task has been primarily utilized in

developmental studies, as it is generally easy for young children to understand the simple

instructions (Casey et al., 2004; Dirks et al., 2020; Morton et al., 2009; Yerys et al.,

2015). Attention-shifting tasks are often an adaptation of the DCCS test battery (Ezekiel

et al., 2013; Morton et al., 2009), and therefore, can be given to children as young as 3

years (Hanania & Smith, 2010).

Complex Set-Shifting

       In complex set-shifting tasks, participants are still required to shift within a trial,

but utilize more cognitive abilities than simply shifting attention (Yasumura et al., 2015).

One example of a complex set-shifting task is the flexible item selection task (FIST;

Jacques & Zelazo, 2001). Participants are presented with three cards and are instructed to

select 2 cards that match on one dimension (e.g., number, color, size) and then shift by

selecting another matching pair based on a different dimension (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001).

Moreover, the FIST is similar to the DCCS and WCST, but can vary in difficulty by

including more abstract/complex dimensional shifts, which can be further altered by

increasing dimensions or shifts within a trial (Dajani et al., 2020; Dick, 2014).

Additionally, other cognitive processes such as working memory may be recruited to

complete the task depending on the number of switches needed to complete the trial.

Recruitment of other cognitive processes such as working memory and inhibition in
6

addition to shifting are thought to support cognitive flexibility (Dajani & Uddin, 2015),

and may better represent real-world scenarios. Because the FIST can be altered to make it

more difficult, and may be more representative of real-world scenarios, children tend to

perform less well compared with adults (Dick, 2014). Therefore, complex set-shifting

tasks may require greater cognitive demands to complete the task.

Rule-Switching

       Rule-switching tasks require participants to flexibly respond to a stimulus based

on the given rule (e.g., respond based on color or direction) (Wendelken et al., 2012).

Similar to complex set-shifting tasks, rule switching tasks sometimes require higher order

processes (i.e., working memory) when participants must mentally maintain the rules

(Dajani & Uddin, 2015).

Performance-Based Switching

       Performance-based switching tasks typically involve: 1) an adaptation of the

WCST, where participants have to switch between three possible response rules (Crone et

al., 2008), and 2) a probabilistic task (Hauser et al., 2015; van den Bos et al., 2012),

where participants have to switch their stimulus choice based on probabilistic feedback

(e.g., choosing stimulus A receives positive feedback 80% of the time whereas choosing

B results in only 20% positive feedback).

Executive functions needed to implement cognitive flexibility

       Cognitive flexibility is a key aspect of executive functioning, or the higher order

processes that enable goal-oriented behaviors. However, other executive functions may

be involved in cognitive flexibility, and are thought to be distinct yet correlated

constructs (Akira Miyake & Friedman, 2012). The executive functions involved in
7

cognitive flexibility processes may include working memory and inhibition, along with

other processes such as salience detection and attention (Dajani & Uddin, 2015).

Common behavioral cognitive flexibility tasks, including the Wisconsin Card Sorting

Test (WCST; Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G, 1993), the

Dimensional Card Sort (DCCS; Zelazo, 2006), and various tasks of the Delis Kaplan

Executive Functioning (D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2012), require multiple executive functions

to complete the tasks (Coulacoglou & Saklofske, 2017; Lange et al., 2016; A. Miyake et

al., 2000). For example, during the WCST, participants classify cards differing on color,

shape, or number of designs. Participants need to be able to orient their attention to

salient features of the cards, utilize working memory to recall previously used responses

(Lange et al., 2016), inhibit prior responses (Diamond et al., 2005), and ultimately switch

their response. Therefore, it is theorized that cognitive flexibility is orchestrated in

accordance with other crucial executive functions of working memory and inhibition

(Diamond et al., 2005), and salience/attention processes (Chen et al., 2016).

Salience detection

        Salience detection is the process of determining which stimuli are salient and

subsequently drawing attention to the stimuli (Lucina Q. Uddin, 2015). Salience detection

is supported by the mid-cingulo insular network (M-CIN; Salience network) (Lucina Q.

Uddin et al., 2019), including the anterior insula (AI), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex

(dACC), and subcortical and limbic structures (Seeley et al., 2007). The M-CIN

integrates external sensory information and internal emotional and body states that are

used to guide behavior. The M-CIN additionally plays a role as a flexible hub, facilitating

brain network interactions, and allowing salient information to be processed and
8

responded to (Sridharan et al., 2008). Specifically, the posterior and mid-insula integrate

and transmit interoceptive signals, whereas the AI orchestrates the brain network

dynamic interactions (Dajani & Uddin, 2015). The flexible variability in the M-CIN is

thought to be an important contributor to flexible behavior, and it has been previously

shown to predict individual differences in cognitive flexibility (Chen et al., 2016).

Bottom-up/top-down attention

       Attention processes can be partitioned into dorsal and ventral pathways in the

brain, contributing to goal-oriented (top-down) and stimulus-driven (bottom-up) attention

(Chica et al., 2011). The dorsal-attention network (DAN), comprised of the intraparietal

sulcus (IPS), superior parietal lobule, and frontal eye fields (FEF), supports top-down

processing, whereas the ventral attention network (VAN) comprised of the ventrolateral

prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and temporoparietal junction, supports bottom-up processing

(Corbetta et al., 2008). The DAN is therefore important for spatial orienting of attention

and feature-based attention (Vossel et al., 2014), useful in cognitive flexibility tasks such

as during attention-shifting.

Working memory

       Working memory, or the ability to mentally maintain information needed for an

ongoing task or process (Oberauer, 2019), is largely supported by the lateral-frontal

parietal network (L-FPN; central executive network) or areas of the dorsolateral PFC,

vlPFC, premotor and parietal cortices (Thomason et al., 2009). The L-FPN broadly

supports goal-directed systems and control of information flow in the brain (Niendam et

al., 2012). Working memory is an important aspect in cognitive flexibility tasks, and a

potential confounder if not properly accounted for. The Flexible Item Search Task (FIST)
9

is a cognitive flexibility task that requires participants to match stimuli based on a shared

dimension and flexibly switch to make a new dimensional match (Dick, 2014). Further,

as more matches are required, greater working memory is needed to prevent repeating

prior matches. Greater working memory load is also indicative of task performance,

primarily in children as they typically perform worse on tasks requiring high working

memory loads, compared with older children and adults (Cowan et al., 2010). However,

in the FIST, working memory was found not to impact cognitive flexibility performance

when the working memory load was age-appropriate. Overall, working memory is an

important aspect of performing cognitive flexibility tasks, but age-related confounds need

to be accounted for.

Inhibition

       Inhibition is regarded as an important function for cognitive flexibility (Davidson

et al., 2006) because prior tasks or responses need to be inhibited prior to switching.

Inhibition is most commonly supported by the brain regions of the right vlPFC (Aron et

al., 2014), AI, and the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) (Aron & Poldrack, 2006). The right

vlPFC plays a role primarily in response inhibition (Sebastian et al., 2016), the rAI plays

a role in detecting behaviorally relevant events and an important region in the M-CIN,

and the IFJ plays a role in detecting behaviorally relevant stimuli (Sebastian et al., 2016).

Inhibition therefore allows for the previous task set to be inhibited prior to switching.

Isolating cognitive flexibility from other executive functions in neuroimaging studies

       A challenge with studying cognitive flexibility involves isolating cognitive

flexibility from other core executive functions (i.e., inhibition and working memory).

Difficulty in isolating the process may impact our understanding of the brain regions
10

underlying cognitive flexibility across development. Executive functions enable goal-

oriented behaviors and extant literature suggests core executive functions are independent

constructs yet highly related (A. Miyake et al., 2000). Therefore, isolating executive

functions from each other becomes difficult, and maybe impossible, because many of

these processes are related and in some cases may even depend on each other. For

example, many of the flexibility tasks described above involve working memory to keep

the task rules in mind, and inhibition to prevent repetition of previously answered

responses. These more complex tasks, however, are thought to be more representative of

real-world applications as real world situations integrate multiple processes (e.g., visual,

sensorimotor, attention, salience, various executive functions) (Matusz et al., 2019;

Bottenhorn et al., 2019). Additionally, in adult studies of the neural substrates underlying

cognitive flexibility, there are brain regions activated solely during cognitive flexibility

tasks and brain regions activated across tasks of executive functions. Because cognitive

flexibility tasks sometimes involve many executive functions, and has been seen to

impact the findings in adult fMRI cognitive flexibility studies, studies during childhood

and adolescence may not always be testing pure cognitive flexibility. However, study of

‘pure’ cognitive flexibility may not be representative of real-world switching (Bottenhorn

et al., 2019). Because of this, multiple considerations need to be made when assessing the

results from the developmental fMRI studies of cognitive flexibility including the type of

task used, executive functions required to complete the task, and developmental stage of

the participants since different components of executive function have different

developmental trajectories.
11

Development of cognitive flexibility

       Behavioral Findings

       Cognitive flexibility and its associated executive functions exhibit different

developmental trajectories across childhood (Anderson, 2002). In general, these skills

begin to emerge during preschool in children as young as 3 years (Hughes, 1998).

Cognitive flexibility skills in preschool children represent lower order forms, such as

shifting, evidenced by children’s ability to perform well on shifting tasks such as the

DCCS (Diamond et al., 2005). Further, integration of other executive functions such as

working memory and inhibition with cognitive flexibility are not yet fully developed in

preschool-aged children, revealed by poor performance on tasks requiring the integration

of various executive functions (Diamond et al., 2005). For example, 4.5 year-old children

successfully perform the DCCS task but perform poorly on the WCST (Chelune & Baer,

1986; Dick, 2014), suggesting cognitive flexibility can be altered by task difficulty, and

higher order flexibility takes longer to develop. Through elementary school age,

cognitive flexibility takes a protracted development compared with other executive

functions (Davidson et al., 2006), and reaches similar behavioral performance as adults

by 10 years of age (Chelune & Baer, 1986; Dick, 2014; Rosselli & Ardila, 1993; Welsh

et al., 1991). However, cognitive flexibility skills continue to develop into adolescence

and adulthood (Anderson, 2002), peaking between 21 and 30 years (Cepeda et al., 2001).

Brain Regions in neurotypical adults

       Greater insight into the neurodevelopmental processes associated with cognitive

flexibility can be gleaned from studies of the adult brain. Findings from functional MRI

studies of adults using age-adjusted versions of cognitive flexibility tasks have shown
12

core brain regions and networks underlying cognitive flexibility (Dajani et al., 2016), as

well as task-dependent brain regions (C. Kim et al., 2011; Chobok Kim et al., 2012)

(Figure 3). Brain regions found to be involved in core cognitive flexibility across all task

types include the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC), dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC),

anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), right anterior insula (AI), inferior frontal junction (IFJ),

premotor cortex, inferior and superior parietal cortices, inferior temporal cortex, occipital

cortex, and subcortical structures such as the caudate and thalamus (Dajani & Uddin,

2015; Chobok Kim et al., 2012; Niendam et al., 2012). Brain regions involved in

cognitive flexibility further differentiate depending on the type of task (C. Kim et al.,

2011; Chobok Kim et al., 2012). More abstract switching recruits anterior-PFC regions;

moderately abstract switching recruits mid-PFC; and constrained switching recruit

posterior-PFC regions (C. Kim et al., 2011). Results from Kim et al., (2012) meta-

analysis further revealed region-specific activation during attention-shifting tasks in the

dorsal portion of the premotor cortex, and during set-shifting tasks in the frontopolar

cortex. Overall, the findings from adult neuroimaging studies of cognitive flexibility

reveal brain regions associated with cognitive flexibility that may differentiate depending

on the task.

Neural correlates in developmental cohorts

       Developmental neuroimaging studies suggest cognitive flexibility takes a

protracted developmental trajectory, as paralleled in behavioral findings. A prolonged

development among the brain regions supporting cognitive flexibility, mainly the

frontoparietal network (Barber & Carter, 2005; Cole & Schneider, 2007), is also observed

in structural findings suggesting the gray matter in those regions mature later than other
13

regions (Casey et al., 2000; Giedd et al., 1999). Similarly to adult findings, certain brain

regions are commonly activated in children across switching tasks among the

frontoparietal network such as the dlPFC and the pre-SMA (Morton et al., 2009;

Wendelken et al., 2012), and the basal ganglia (Casey et al., 2004; Crone, Wendelken, et

al., 2006; Rubia et al., 2006). However, activation among the frontoparietal network and

insula increases in activation strength across development and most strongly in adults,

suggesting as these regions develop, cognitive flexibility strengthens (Rubia et al., 2006;

Taylor et al., 2012; Wendelken et al., 2012).

Attention-shifting

       Cognitive flexibility can also be observed differentially depending on the

switching/shifting task, as observed in adult studies of cognitive flexibility (Chobok Kim

et al., 2012). Attention-shifting, shifting attention between stimuli dimensions (e.g., color,

shape), has been primarily studied across development (Casey et al., 2004; Dirks et al.,

2020; Morton et al., 2009; Yerys et al., 2015). Children and adults share common brain

activation regions during attention-shifting including the superior parietal cortex, dlPFC,

IFJ, pre-SMA region (Morton et al., 2009), and the caudate nucleus (Casey et al., 2004).

Key developmental differences between children and adults are also seen during

attention-shifting. In Morton et al., (2009) children (11-13 years) had unique activation

among the R superior frontal sulcus, whereas adults had unique activation in the L

superior parietal cortex and R thalamus. This finding implies children may have different

switching strategies compared to adults resulting in differing brain activation patterns. In

another study, Casey et al., (2004) observed more prefrontal and parietal regions in adults

compared to children (7-11 years) suggesting greater recruitment of these cortical regions
14

across development. In a study of typically developing (TD) children compared with

children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), utilizing the same task from Casey et al.,

(2004), TD children (7-12 years) had brain activation in the L posterior supramarginal

gyrus/angular gyrus during shift trials (Dirks et al., 2020). The discrepancies in attention-

shifting findings may be due to small participation sample sizes and the shifting task,

previously shown to be a poor indicator of cognitive flexibility (Dirks et al., 2020).

Set-shifting

       Complex set-shifting tasks require different brain regions to complete the task

compared with lower level shifting tasks. Attention-shifting studies primarily utilize the

DCCS, shown to be relatively easy for children at young ages, but the WCST (set-

shifting) appears to be more difficult for children, and in some cases adults (Dajani et al.,

2020; Dick, 2014). Shifting tasks that require greater cognitive flexibility include more

abstract/complex dimensional shifts or rules requiring a greater number of dimensions or

switches (Dajani et al., 2020; Dick, 2014). Further, the requirements during the shift are

also more complex and require using more cognitive abilities (Yasumura et al., 2015).

       In one study that examined set-shifting abilities in children using the WCST task,

children had activation in the right insula, a region important for switching between brain

networks to enable flexible behavior (Menon & Uddin, 2010), with increasing activation

with age (Taylor et al., 2012). Only one study in adults has used the Flexible Item

Selection Task (FIST), a complex version of a set-shifting task that requires subjects to

abstract a matching dimension and switch flexibly to a new matching dimension (Dajani

et al., 2020; Dick, 2014). Although the FIST has only been studied in adults, behavioral
15

evidence reveals age-related changes at least until 10 years of age, suggesting age-related

neural differences in set-shifting at least until 10 years (Dick, 2014).

Rule-switching

       Rule-switching is another cognitive flexibility task that requires higher order

processes particularly in rule switching that requires working memory to mentally

maintain the rules (Wendelken et al., 2012). The earliest study found adolescents had

similar brain activation as adults during rule-switching, among regions of the pre-

SMA/SMA (Crone, Donohue, et al., 2006). In a recent rule-switching study, the authors

utilized a task that required participants to switch flexibly from one task rule to another

(Wendelken et al., 2012). In children (8-13 years) and adults, brain regions of the L

dlPFC, L PPC, and pre-SMA regions were involved in rule-switching trials. The dlPFC

and SMA were similarly activated as in other cognitive flexibility studies. Further, there

were key developmental differences seen between children and adults in their brain

activations during the task. Adults had greater activation overall and among the pre-

SMA, PMC, and L PPC. Children also had regions more activated than adults among the

right inferior parietal lobe and the cingulate gyrus. Additionally, children engaged the L

superior temporal gyrus and the R middle temporal gyrus, in the switch trials compared

with repeating trials. However, this pattern of activation was not seen in adults,

suggesting rule switching may create greater cognitive demands and may be more

difficult for children compared to adults. Further, children had more initial dlPFC

activation driven by the previous trial rule, suggesting children are more likely to

maintain previous rules when it is no longer relevant and may have difficulties with

switching to a new rule set (Wendelken et al., 2012).
16

Performance-based switching

        Performance-based switching studies have revealed many behavioral and

neurodevelopmental differences between children and adults (Crone et al., 2008; Hauser

et al., 2015; van den Bos et al., 2012). The type of tasks used to test performance-based

switching involve an adaptation of the WCST, where participants had to spatially switch

between three possible response rules (Crone et al., 2008) and a probabilistic task

(Hauser et al., 2015; van den Bos et al., 2012), where participants had to switch their

stimulus choice based on probabilistic feedback (e.g., choosing stimulus A receives

positive feedback 80% of the time whereas choosing B results in only 20% positive

feedback). The insula appears to be an important contributor to performance-switching as

indicated by activation during the WCST adapted task and in adolescents, where greater

activation occurred in the anterior insula for reward prediction errors prior to switching

(Crone et al., 2008). This is also in line with findings in a meta-analysis of developmental

executive functions, where the right anterior insular cortex was shown to have age-related

involvement among inhibition, switching, and working memory (Houdé et al., 2010).

Other regions were important during performance-switching including the DLPFC,

mPFC, ACC, striatum, vmPFC, amygdala, L PCC, L Putamen, R precentral gyrus, L

SFG, and L IPL. The mPFC was commonly observed across the three developmental

studies, consistent with reports of the role of the PFC in cognitive flexibility (Rougier et

al., 2005).

Overview of neurodevelopmental findings

        The neurodevelopmental studies of cognitive flexibility provide initial insight into

the developmental processes surrounding switching/shifting, an ability important for life
17

outcomes. The regions commonly activated in children during switching/shifting across

task types include the dlPFC and the pre-SMA/SMA. The dlPFC is one region of the

frontoparietal network involved in switching in adults. The dlPFC is primarily thought to

be involved with working memory (Thomason et al., 2009), therefore children may be

using working memory to complete the tasks. However, in cognitive flexibility tasks with

higher loads of working memory, children typically perform less accurately than adults

(Thomason et al., 2009; Wendelken et al., 2012), suggesting the integration of working

memory and switching processes are not fully developed during childhood. The pre-

SMA/SMA was also discovered to be activated in children across multiple developmental

cognitive flexibility studies. Activation of the pre-SMA is commonly seen in set-shifting

tasks (Barber & Carter, 2005) in adults and broadly during executive functioning tasks

(Duncan & Owen, 2000). The Pre-SMA is overall thought to be involved in task-set

reconfiguration, suppression of previous responses, and error likelihood estimation

(Morton et al., 2009).

        The developmental neuroimaging studies reviewed also provided insight into the

regions that were not commonly activated in children during cognitive flexibility tasks.

For example, only one study found evidence of activation of the IFJ in both children and

adults (Morton et al., 2009). The IFJ was found to be implemented across various

cognitive flexibility tasks in adults (Dajani et al., 2020; C. Kim et al., 2011). The IFJ was

additionally found to coordinate switches among brain regions during cognitive

flexibility (Dajani et al., 2020). Specifically, the IFJ gets activated first, and leads to

activations in other regions involved in cognitive flexibility. Since this region was not
18

commonly activated in cognitive flexibility studies with children, it suggests the ability to

coordinate switching undergoes developmental changes.

        Another set of brain regions found to be commonly observed in adults is the M-

CIN (dACC, AI, insula). There were mixed findings surrounding the M-CIN among the

children studies with some instances with increased activation of the insula in children

(Ezekiel et al., 2013; Mogadam et al., 2018; Rodehacke et al., 2014) and conversely some

instances of decreased or no activation of the insula during switching (Crone, Donohue,

et al., 2006; Crone et al., 2008; Dibbets et al., 2006; Dirks et al., 2020; Nelson et al.,

2007; Taylor et al., 2012; Wendelken et al., 2012). The M-CIN supports coordination

among large-scale networks (M-FPN/DMN and L-FPN/CEN) and is thought to enable

flexible behavior (Lucina Q. Uddin et al., 2015). The mixed findings regarding the M-

CIN brain regions in children may be due to the various tasks (i.e., attention-shifting vs.

performance-switching) or varying age groups across the studies (i.e., from 3 years to 17

years). In most cases, children and adolescents were separately grouped, and generally

adolescents had more insula activation than children, suggesting an age-related increase

of activation of the insula (Taylor et al., 2012) associated with better behavioral

performance (Chen et al., 2016). The studies reviewed provide valuable insight into the

development of cognitive flexibility but there are still many questions to be answered,

including how the brain organization of these brain regions enable cognitive flexibility

and changes across development.
19

Brain network findings in cognitive flexibility

Brain networks enabling flexibility

       A growing body of literature has begun to explore the dynamic brain network

topology that enables cognitive flexibility and flexible behavior. Prior evidence reveals

adaptive behavior is supported by flexible network interactions among the L-FPN and M-

CIN (Braun et al., 2015; Cohen et al., 2014; Cole et al., 2013; Shine et al., 2016). The L-

FPN and M-CIN have the most flexible connections with other brain regions compared

with other brain networks (Cocuzza et al., 2020; Mattar et al., 2016). The medial

frontoparietal network (M-FPN or default network) appears to have the greatest

flexibility using static FC methods (Cole et al., 2013), where the FC is averaged across

the duration of the scan, but recent advanced dynamic FC methods that account for time-

varying FC across the duration of the scan, reveal the M-CIN has flexible connections

(Nomi et al., 2016). Specifically, the right dAIC of the M-CIN acts as a switcher hub,

facilitating flexible interactions between the M-FPN and L-FPN (Nomi et al., 2016;

Sridharan et al., 2008; L. Q. Uddin et al., 2011). The temporal flexibility of the M-CIN

has also been shown to predict individual differences in cognitive flexibility in adults

(Chen et al., 2016).

Functional/Dynamic connectivity cognitive flexibility

       Brain network associations, identified using FC or correlations between separate

brain regions (Eickhoff & Müller, 2015), have revealed network interactions in cognitive

flexibility not otherwise seen using brain activation methods. One study evaluated the FC

during a resting-state fMRI scan to task performance on a set-shifting task performed

outside of the scanner. Greater posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus (M-FPN)

connectivity with the ventromedial striatopallidum (basal ganglia) was correlated with

fewer total errors on the set-shifting task, suggesting the relationship between the M-FPN
20

and basal ganglia is important for cognitive flexibility (Vatansever et al., 2016).

However, another study found evidence for state-dependent FC relationships between the

L-FPN and M-FPN such that greater L-FPN and M-FPN connectivity during task-state is

associated with cognitive flexibility, whereas greater L-FPN and M-FPN connectivity

during resting-state relates to poorer performance (Douw et al., 2016). Together, these

studies reveal the importance of considering resting- or task-states when investigating

brain network relationships with cognitive flexibility and considering the role of the M-

FPN in cognitive flexibility.

       A recent fMRI study examined the neural correlates of cognitive flexibility in

adults using the FIST (Dajani et al., 2020; Dick, 2014). A variety of brain regions were

activated in flexibility versus control comparisons including the M-FPN and M-CIN,

such as the lIFJ extending into the left IFG, dlPFC, FEFs, AI, dACC/pre-SMA and IPL

(Dajani et al., 2020). The regions activated by the flexibility versus control comparisons

were also analyzed using FC network analyses and an extended unified structural equal

modeling approach to estimate ROI to ROI directed network connectivity (Dajani et al.,

2020). They found only the lIFJ was directly activated and other activations seen were

due to their functional connections with the lIFJ. The lIFJ therefore was hypothesized to

be the first region to activate in response to engagement with cognitive flexibility and

leads to engagement among other regions of the prefrontal, parietal and cerebellar

regions. Therefore, the lIFJ modulates cognitive flexibility due to its role in updating task

sets during switching (Chobok Kim et al., 2012). Dajani et al., (2020) additionally found

roles for the dlPFC in maintaining information such as during working memory tasks, the

AI and dACC of the M-CIN as being an important connection to the lIFJ during cognitive
21

flexibility, and the lAG of the M-FPN in task performance as reported in the literature

(Douw et al., 2016; Vatansever et al., 2016).

Development

       A network approach has also provided greater insight to the brain related changes

associated with cognitive flexibility across development. In a study using the DCCS, an

attention-shifting task, age-related differences were seen among the FC of the lPFC with

the anterior cingulate, inferior parietal cortex, and the ventral tegmental area, showing

stronger activation in adults compared with children (12 years mean age) (Ezekiel et al.,

2013). Children had greater FC between the frontal pole and insula. Together, these

findings suggest children may utilize a different cognitive strategy to implement

attention-shifting as shown by the connection among the frontal pole and insula, and may

develop FC among the lPFC later (Ezekiel et al., 2013).

       Brain network flexibility is also shown to change across development, and reflects

changes in cognitive functioning. In children (8-15 years), brain signal variability

increases with age and corresponds to a reduction in behavioral variability and greater

accuracy (McIntosh et al., 2008). Across the lifespan, brain signal variability decreases

across most regions of the brain, but these decreases are offset by increases in select brain

regions including the anterior insula of the M-CIN (Nomi, Bolt, et al., 2017). There are

also age-related changes in dwell time and frequency of certain brain states, and increases

in the number of brain state transitions during task-elicited states across development

(Hutchison & Morton, 2015), suggesting the brain becomes more flexible with age (9

years to 32 years). Additionally, in separate study with a sample of 7-16 year old

children, there were age related increases in temporal variability of FC among the M-
22

CIN, L-FPN, and M-FPN (Marusak et al., 2017). The same study also found highly

variable connections between the L-FPN and M-FPN, indicating these regions integrate

early in life (Marusak et al., 2017). Additional work found the functional organization of

the whole brain in children (9-12 years) during resting-state is similar to adults, and

variability in network organization among the L-FPN and M-CIN can be observed in late

childhood (Le et al., 2020). This finding aligns with behavioral evidence suggesting

children reach similar behavioral performance as adults on set-shifting tasks around 10

years of age (Dick, 2014). The dynamic coordination within the brain and primarily

among large-scale neurocognitive networks support the development of cognitive

flexibility.

Co-activation Pattern Analysis (CAP)

        Although the studies reviewed provide insight into the dynamic network changes

seen across development and their relationship with cognitive performance, the methods

used rely on either ‘static’ methods (i.e., FC), where the time series is averaged across the

fMRI scan (Biswal et al., 1995), or time-varying methods (i.e., dynamic FC), where the

moment to moment changes in FC is captured (Chang & Glover, 2010). FC methods in

particular do not capture time-varying representations of the brain. Further, dFC relies on

the ‘sliding window’ approach (Chang & Glover, 2010) to capture FC changes across a

fixed window length (Preti et al., 2017). FC and dFC methods, although valuable, rely on

many assumptions and arbitrarily collapse data into time and space. Novel dynamic

methods such as co-activation pattern (CAP) analysis (Liu et al., 2013) are increasingly

utilized (Kupis et al., 2020, 2021) because they capture time-varying brain state

alterations not otherwise observed using static methods and in some instances reveal
23

more brain and behavior relationships compared with static methods (Lurie et al., 2020).

For the first time, this study aims to examine the neural correlates of cognitive flexibility

across development using a novel brain dynamic method and link brain function during a

cognitive flexibility task with behavior.

Specific aims and hypotheses

Aim 1: To understand the neural bases of cognitive flexibility in neurotypical children

and adults during task elicited brain states.

       Aim1a. To examine brain activation responses in children compared with adults

during a flexible item selection task that requires flexible switching between stimulus

dimensions (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001).

       Aim1b. To examine dynamic brain state metrics (dwell time, frequency, and

transitions) in children compared with adults during a flexible item selection task that

requires flexible switching between stimulus dimensions (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001).

Hypotheses: Children will have reduced activation in regions previously associated with

flexibility in adults (IFJ, dACC, Cerebellum, AI), but not in regions of the dlPFC, L PPC,

and pre-SMA. The IFJ, dACC, cerebellum, and AI are previously shown to be activated

in adults during cognitive flexibility tasks (Dajani et al., 2020; Dajani & Uddin, 2015;

Chobok Kim et al., 2012). Conversely, the dlPFC, L PPC, and pre-SMA have been

shown to be as strongly activated in children when compared to adults in cognitive

flexibility tasks (Crone, Donohue, et al., 2006; Crone et al., 2008; Morton et al., 2009;

Wendelken et al., 2012).

       Children will additionally exhibit longer dwell times and less frequently occurring

brain states associated with brain dynamics during the cognitive flexibility task, and
24

have fewer transitions during flexibility trials. Previous research shows children have

longer dwell times (Ryali et al., 2016) and less frequent occurrences of certain brain

states during rest fMRI (Kupis et al., under review). Additionally, children are

hypothesized to have fewer transitions in brain network configurations compared with

adults due to growing evidence demonstrating brain state transitions increase across

development and during task states (Hutchison & Morton, 2015).

Aim 2: To investigate developmental differences in behavioral measures of flexibility

(BRIEF) (Dajani et al., 2020; Gioia G. A., Isquith P. K., Guy S. C., Kenworthy L.., 2000;

Roth R. M., Isquith P. K., Gioia G. A.., 2005) in children and adults and behavioral

associations (reaction time, accuracy, BRIEF) with dynamic brain state metrics during

the cognitive flexibility task.

Hypotheses: Children will have higher (poorer) scores on the BRIEF shift scales

compared with adults. Children will also have less brain activation in the IFJ, dACC,

and AI associated with poorer (lower) scores on the BRIEF shift scales. Prior studies

found the IFJ to be an important hub for cognitive flexibility tasks in adults (Dajani et al.,

2020; Chobok Kim et al., 2012), indicating this region may undergo the greatest

developmental changes to support switching mechanisms. Additionally, the dACC and

AI of the M-CIN is hypothesized to be less activated and associated with behavioral

performance and the BRIEF due to a growing body of literature suggesting this area has

the most age-related changes during development during cognitive flexibility tasks

(Crone et al., 2008; Hauser et al., 2015; Rubia et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2012).

Additionally, there is prior evidence showing the flexibility within the M-CIN predicts

individual differences in cognitive flexibility in adults (Chen et al., 2016). Therefore, it is
25

hypothesized that lower activation in the dACC and AI will be associated with poorer

cognitive flexibility behavior in children.

       Secondly, using CAP analysis, children will have reduced brain dynamics as

measured by dwell time, frequency of occurrence, and transitions during the cognitive

flexibility task, associated with poorer behavioral measures of flexibility compared with

adults. Specifically, longer dwell times, less frequently occurring states, and fewer

transitions is predicted to be associated with poorer performance on the cognitive task

and lower BRIEF scores. Many prior studies have observed increased brain flexibility

with age corresponding with greater task performance (Burzynska et al., 2015; Hutchison

& Morton, 2015; McIntosh et al., 2008).
CHAPTER 2: METHODS

        Participants included 32 adults (19-46 years) and 25 typically developing (TD)

children (7-12 years) recruited from the University of Miami and the wider Miami

community (Table 1). Exclusionary criteria included 1) less than 2 usable task runs and

2) incidental findings. Subjects additionally underwent a visual Quality Control

inspection and were excluded if they had one or more visually identifiable artifacts

including but not limited to: excessive motion, ringing, blurring, ghosting, wrapping,

signal loss, and head coverage. All participants were right-handed as determined by self

or parental report, with no history of psychological disorders.

Table 1

                      Children (N = 25)        Adults (N = 32)           p-value

 Age (year)           10.20 ± 1.50 (7 - 12)    24.78 ± 6.46 (19 - 46)    < .001

 Sex                  16M 9F                   17M 15F                   .409

 Ethnicity            16 (Not                  15 (Not                   .053
                      Hispanic/Latino) 8       Hispanic/Latino) 11
                      (Hispanic/Latino) 1      (Hispanic/Latino) 6
                      (NR)                     (NR)

 Race                 15 (White) 0 (Black      16 (White) 0 (Black or    .002
                      or African American)     African American) 0
                      0 (Asian) 6 (More        (Asian) 0 (More than
                      than one race) 3         one race) 4 (Other) 12
                      (Other) 1 (NR)           (NR)

 Mean FD (mm)         .21 ± .11 (.07 - .47)    .09 ± .03 (.05 - .15)     < .001

 BRIEF Shift T        44.84 ± 6.97 (36 -       45.19 ± 9.87 (39 - 77)    .882
 score                63)
        Mean ± sd (minimum - maximum)

                                              26
27

       Note: M, male; F, female; Mean FD, mean framewise displacement; NR, not
reported; BRIEF, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function.

       Participant preparation for MRI

       Children participants viewed a 5-minute videotape of a child volunteer

undergoing a fMRI scan to familiarize them of the procedure before their scan date. All

participants underwent a training procedure the day of their scheduled scan. Participants

were trained on the FIST/cFIST by an experimenter on a computer desktop and practiced

a computer-based task and fMRI adapted task before undergoing a mock MRI training.

During the mock MRI, participants completed the same fMRI task. Scanner training was

followed by functional and structural brain imaging along with a task refresher before the

first task run in the scanner. For more details of the experimental procedure see Dajani et

al., 2020.

Behavioral measures

       Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)

       For children, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-2 (BRIEF-2)

was completed by parents (Gioia G. A., Isquith P. K., Guy S. C., Kenworthy L.., 2000).

The BRIEF-2 is a 86-item parent report questionnaire used to assess executive function

and organizational skills in children. The adults were administered the BRIEF-A, the

adult version of the BRIEF. The BRIEF-A is a 75-item self-report measure that assesses

executive function and organizational skills in adults (Roth et al., 2005). Both the BRIEF-

2 and BRIEF-A include a shift subscale and T scores were examined as shifting skills are

a measure of cognitive flexibility (Dajani & Uddin, 2015). T scores at or above 70 are

considered clinically elevated.
You can also read