Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

Page created by Angel Sutton
 
CONTINUE READING
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review
Neuroethics
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-021-09474-8

 ORIGINAL PAPER

Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical
Systematic Review
Anke Snoek       · Dorothee Horstkötter

Received: 8 April 2021 / Accepted: 15 August 2021
© The Author(s) 2021

Abstract Parenting books and early childhood                 encounter. In the last decade, parenting advice based
policy documents increasingly refer to neuroscience          on neuroscientific evidence has become so popular,
to support their parenting advice. This trend, called        that some speak of neuroparenting [1]. Neuroparent-
‘neuroparenting’ has been subject to a growing body          ing is a parenting style where neuroscientific insights
of sociological and ethical critical examination. The        are used to improve parenting, and thereby to foster
aim of this paper is to review this critical literature on   child development. The idea behind it is twofold: 1)
neuroparenting. We identify three main arguments:            neuroscientific evidence can provide essential insights
that there is a gap between neuroscientific findings         in how parental choices can positively or negatively
and neuroparenting advice, that there is an implicit         influence child brain development, 2) Knowledge
normativity in the translation from neuroscience to          about children’s brain development allows parents to
practice, and that neuroparenting is a form of neolib-       adjust their parenting to their child’s developmental
eral self-management. We will critically discuss these       stage [1, 2]. Examples of neuroparenting range from
arguments and make suggestions for ethically respon-         the advice to stimulate your newborn’s brain devel-
sible forms of neuroparenting that can foster child          opment by reading or breastfeeding [3] to parenting
development but avoid pitfalls.                              books on the ‘teenage brain’ to support parents in
                                                             managing adolescents’ lack of self-control [4].
Keywords Neuroparenting · Parenting ·                           This emerging trend of neuroparenting fits into a
Neoliberalism · Early childhood policies · Ethical           broader development of the neuroscientification of
review · Child development                                   the discourse on child and youth development [5]. It
                                                             can be observed in both popular parenting books as
                                                             in policy documents on how to support child develop-
Introduction                                                 ment [6].
                                                                The rise of neuroparenting has also triggered an
Many parents seek advice, to determine how to best           engaged and critical social and ethical discussion
raise their children or deal with challenges they            regarding its implications on child development, on
                                                             parent–child relationships, and on identity. To date,
                                                             a stalemate seems to have emerged between those
A. Snoek (*) · D. Horstkötter                                who aim to invoke neuroscientific research for parent-
Department of Health, Ethics and Society (HES),              ing and parenting support and those who are critical
Maastricht University, Metamedica, Postbus 616,
                                                             about this endeavor. This stalemate leads to a situation
6200 MD Maastricht, Netherlands
e-mail: A.snoek@maastrichtuniversity.nl                      where neuroscientific findings get either over-claimed

                                                                                                     Vol.:(0123456789)
                                                                                                          13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

in their practical usability or abandoned altogether as     Field Analysis: Age Groups, Aims, Dissemination
a source of insight. In order to overcome this stale-       and Methods
mate, the current article will review the sociological
and ethical literature on neuroparenting. The aim is to     We found that the critical sociological and ethical lit-
identify and evaluate the reasons that have been given      erature uses diverse methodologies, focussed on the
for and against the practice of neuroparenting.             different ways in which neuroparenting advice is dis-
                                                            seminated, and focused on different age-group with
                                                            different intervention aims. This analysis resulted in
Method                                                      the following field map (Fig. 2).
                                                                Neuroparenting advice – and hence the critical
Systematic reviews are comparatively new to ethics          literature – focuses on three age groups: 1) the ‘first
studies, but provide a particularly powerful way to         three years’, measured from conception, 2) young
systematically identify, analyze and synthesize nor-        children in general, and 3) adolescents [4, 9–11]. For
mative argumentation [7, 8]. We conducted a search          these age groups, different overlapping neuroparent-
of relevant keywords on 2­ nd February 2020 in Google       ing aims were identified. Regarding children’s early
scholar, Pubmed, Psycinfo, Philpapers, and Jstor. We        years, the main focus is on improving social-emo-
started with the rather broad term ‘neuroparenting’,        tional development, mediated by adequate bonding
and continued the search using ‘brain-based parent-         and love, and on optimizing cognitive development
ing’ as an alternative, because several search engines      and IQ. Neuroparenting early in life is also based on
(PsychInfo, Philpapers, and Jstor) yielded only one         the hope that psychological problems or even crimi-
result or none at all. In addition, we evoked snowball-     nal behavior later in life can be prevented enabling
ing methods and searched the references of selected         children to grow into productive citizens [2]. For ado-
article for further hints. Articles were scanned on title   lescents we found the focus was on both ways to con-
and abstract, and if in doubt, content.                     trol unruly, criminal, or violent behavior [10, 11] and
   Given that our main point of interest is the debate      also on enabling adolescents to reach their full poten-
on neuroparenting, we excluded primary sources:             tial, cognitively, socially and creatively [4].
empirical neuroscientific findings; parenting books             Neuroparenting was found to be disseminated
and their reviews. We also excluded literature on neu-      through various routes, most notably public policies,
roeducation and neuroenhancement because these              media, and the promotion of direct-to-consumer-
typically address professional educators, not parents       products like toys or parenting books. With regard to
and because they focus on cognitive development             the dissemination of neuroparenting directly to con-
rather than child development more broadly. Also            sumers, the studies focus on parenting books [4, 12],
non-English articles were excluded (Fig. 1).                flash cards [13], or educational toys for children [14].
   We identified 37 articles that critically discuss the        Methodologically, most critical sociological and
phenomenon of neuroparenting. A qualitative analy-          ethical studies focused on the content-analysis of
sis of the articles was conducted using Nvivo10, a          policy documents, media articles, or parenting mate-
software package designed to analyse qualitative            rials [6, 15, 16]. Some studies used a more sociologi-
data, but that is also used for literature reviews. We      cal analysis, exploring parenting norms [12, 14, 17].
uploaded pdfs of the articles and linked a label with a     Others used a qualitative empirical design that give
description (node) to the arguments we encountered.         insight into the experiences of parents [4, 13, 18–21],
We then grouped and synthesized the different nodes.        adolescents [9], or policy makers [15, 22]. Two stud-
That way it became apparent how certain ethical             ies had as primary target to review neuroscientific lit-
themes and categories reemerge across different prac-       erature on which the neuroparenting advice are based
tices of neuroparenting (Table 1).                          [23, 24].

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

                               Identification of studies via databases and registers

                                                                 Duplicate records removed before screening:
                  Records identified using keyword
                                                                    1. n = 6 (5 referred to separate book chapters of
                  ‘neuroparenting’ from:
                                                                         Macvarish Monography. We only included the
                     1. Google scholar (n = 111)
                                                                         monography)
                     2. Pubmed (n = 176)
                                                                    2. n = 32
                     3. Psycinfo (n = 1)
                                                                    3. n = 1
                     4. Philpapers (n = 1)
 Identification

                                                                    4. n = 1
                     5. Jstor ( n = 0)
                                                                    5. n = 0
                  Records identified using keyword
                                                                 Records identified using keyword ‘brain parenting’ from:
                  ‘brain parenting’ from:
                                                                    6. Google scholar (n = 228.000 key word too broad)
                      6. Google scholar (n =
                                                                    7. Pubmed (n = 1032 key word too broad)
                           228.000)
                                                                    8. Psycinfo (n = 108)
                      7. Pubmed (n = 1032)
                                                                    9. Philpapers (n = 996 key word too broad)
                      8. Psycinfo (n = 111)
                                                                    10. Jstor ( n = 3340 key word too broad)
                      9. Philpapers (n = 996)
                                                                    If the first 50 pages revealed no useful hit, the keyword was
                      10. Jstor ( n = 3340)
                                                                    considered too broad, and the rest of the pages were not
                                                                    screened
                  Record identified through
                  scanning literature of selected
                                                                 Record identified through scanning literature of selected articles:
                  articles:
                      11. n = 33                                    11. n = 0

                  Records screened                               Records excluded**
                     1. n = 105                                     Reason: non English (n = 7)
                     2. n = 144                                     Reason: Book review ( n = 5)
                     8. n = 3                                       Reason: Parenting books (n = 7)
                     11. n = 33                                     Reason: Only minor reference to neuroparenting (n = 114)
                                                                    Reason: medical literature on neural conditions (n = 109)
    Screening

                  Reports sought for retrieval                   Reports not retrieved
                  n = 43                                         (n = 0)

                  Reports assessed for eligibility                Reports excluded:
                  n = 43                                             Reason: neuroeducation (n = 2)
                                                                     Reason: neuroenhancement (n = 2)
                                                                     Reason: focused on treatment of neural conditions (n = 2)

                  Studies included in review
  Included

                      1. n = 15
                      2. n = 0
                      8. n = 3
                      11. n = 19
                  Total: n = 37

Fig. 1  Search strategy literature review

                                                                                                                          13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

Table 1  Overview of included studies, describing title, country, methodology and which critical arguments are most prominent
      Study                                 Country            Methodology                         Arguments

1     Macvarish (2016) Neuroparent-         UK                 Sociological analysis, document     Gap science practice, hidden
        ing: The expert invasion of                             analysis                            normativity, neoliberal
        family life
2     Macvarish (2014) The ’first three     UK                 Review of neuroscientific litera-   Gap science practice
        years’ movement and the infant                          ture
        brain: A review of critiques
3     Bruer (1999) The myth of the first    US                 Review of neuroscientific litera-   Gap science practice
        three years: A new understand-                          ture
        ing of early brain development
        and lifelong learning
4     Maxwell & Racine (2012) Does          Canada             Review of neuroscientific litera-   Gap science practice
        the neuroscience research on                            ture
        early stress justify responsive
        childcare? examining interwo-
        ven epistemological and ethical
        challenges
5     Belsky & De Haan (2011) Parent-       General            Review of neuroscientific litera-   Gap science practice
        ing and children’s brain develop-                       ture
        ment: The end of the beginning
6     Wall (2004) Is Your Child’s Brain     Canada             Sociological analysis               Neoliberal
        Potential Maximized ?: Mother-
        ing in an Age of New Brain
        Research
7     Wall (2010) Mothers’ experiences      Canada             Empirical                           Neoliberal
        with intensive parenting and
        brain development discourse
8     Jacobs & Hens (2018) Love,            Netherlands        Empirical                           Neoliberal
        neuro-parenting and autism:
        from individual to collective
        responsibility towards parents
        and children
9     Mackenzie & Roberts (2017)            UK                 Empirical                           Neoliberal
        Adopting Neuroscience: Parent-
        ing and Affective Indeterminacy
10 Broer, Pickersgill & Cunningham-         Scotland           Empirical                           Gap science practice
        Burley, (2020) Neurobiological
        limits and the somatic sig-
        nificance of love: Caregivers’
        engagements with neuroscience
        in Scottish parenting pro-
        grammes
11 Leysen (2019) Upbringing and             Flemisch Belgium   Philosophy, sociological analysis   Neoliberal
        Neuroscience. Embodied Theory
        as a Theoretical Bridge Between
        Cognitive Neuroscience and the
        Experience of Being a Parent
Policies
12 Wastell (2012) Blinded by                UK                 document analysis                   Gap science practice
        neuroscience: Social policy, the
        family and the infant brain

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

Table 1  (continued)
     Study                                  Country           Methodology                     Arguments

13   Beddoe (2016) Questioning              New Zealand       document analysis               Neoliberal
      the uncritical acceptance of
      neuroscience in child and family
      policy and practice: A review of
      challenges to the current doxa
14   Edwards, Gillies, and Horsley          UK                Empirical/ document analysis    Neoliberal, Gap science practice
      (2015) ‘Brain science and early
      years policy: hopeful ethos or
      ‘cruel optimism’?’
15   Broer & Pickersgill (2015) Target-     UK                document analysis               Gap science practice
      ing brains, producing responsi-
      bilities: The use of neuroscience
      within British social policy
16   Garrett (2019) Wired: Early            UK                Critical studies                Neoliberal
      Intervention and the ‘Neuromo-
      lecular Gaze’
17   Shonkoff (2011) Science does not       US                Document analysis / empirical   Gap science practice
      speak for itself: Translating child
      development research for the
      public and its policymakers
18   Wall (2018) ‘Love builds brains’:      Canada            Document analysis               Hidden normativity
      Representations of attachment
      and children’s brain develop-
      ment in parenting education
      material
19   Wilson (2002) Brain Science,           UK, US, New       Sociological analysis           Hidden normativity, Gap science
      Early Intervention and ‘At             Zealand, South                                    practice
      Risk’ Families: Implications for       Africa
      Parents, Professionals and Social
      Policy
20   Macvarish (2014) Babies’ brains        UK                Document analysis               Gap science practice
      and parenting policy: The insen-
      sitive mother
21   Lee, Lowe, and Macvarish (2014)        UK                Document analysis               Gap science practice
      The Uses and Abuses of Biol-
      ogy: Neuroscience, Parenting
      and Family Policy in Britain. A
      ‘Key Findings’ Report, Univer-
      sity of Kent
22   Macvarish (2015) Neurosci-             UK                Document analysis               Gap science practice, neoliberal
      ence and family policy: What
      becomes of the parent?
23   Macvarish (2015) Biologising par-      UK                Document analysis               Gap science practice, neoliberal
      enting: neuroscience discourse,
      English social and public health
      policy and understandings of
      the child
24   Lowe & Macvarish (2015) Grow-          UK                Document analysis               Gap science practice, hidden
      ing better brains? Pregnancy and                                                         normativity
      neuroscience discourses in Eng-
      lish social and welfare policies

                                                                                                                     13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

Table 1  (continued)
     Study                                 Country       Methodology                   Arguments

25    Leysen (2020). Neuro-stuffed         Belgium       Document analysis             Neoliberal
       parenthood? Discursive con-
       structions of good parenthood
       in relation to neuroDiscourse in
       Flemish social policy documents
       addressing parents: a case study
Media
26 O’Connor & Joffe (2013) Media           UK            Media analysis                Gap science practice
       representations of early human
       development: Protecting, feed-
       ing and loving the developing
       brain
27 O’Connor & Joffe (2015) How             UK            Partly empirical              Gap science practice
       the Public Engages With Brain
       Optimization: The Media-mind
       Relationship
28 Thompson & Nelson (2001).               US            Media analysis                Gap science practice
       Developmental science and the
       media: early brain development
Directed to consumers
29 Nadesan (2002) Engineering the          US            Sociological analysis         Neoliberal
       Entrepreneurial Infant: Brain
       Science, Infant Development
       Toys, and Governmentality
30 Thornton (2011) Neuroscience,           US            Sociological analysis         Neoliberal
       affect, and the entrepreneuriali-
       zation of motherhood
31 Chen (2019) Beyond black and            Taiwan        Empirical                     Neoliberal
       white: heibaika, neuroparenting,
       and lay neuroscience
Teenage brain
32 Van de Werff (2017) Being a good        Netherlands   Partly empirical              Gap science practice
       external frontal lobe: Parenting
       teenage brains
33 Van de Werff (2018) Practic‑            Netherlands   Partly empirical              Gap science practice
       ing the plastic brain. Popular
       neuroscience and the good life.
       Maastricht University. (strong
       overlap with the above article)
34 Elman (2014) Crazy by Design.           US            Analysis of parenting books   Hidden Normativity
       Neuroparenting and Crisis in the
       Decade of the Brain
35 Elman (2015) Policing at the            US            Sociological analysis         Hidden Normativity
       Synapse: Ferguson, Race, and
       the Disability Politics of the
       Teen Brain. (Strong overlap with
       above article)
36 Choudhury, McKinney, & Merten           UK            Empirical                     Neoliberal
       (2012) Rebelling against the
       brain: Public engagement with
       the “neurological adolescent.”

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

Table 1  (continued)
      Study                                Country              Methodology                      Arguments
37    Bessant (2008) Hard wired for        AU/UK/US             Sociological analysis            Gap science practice, hidden
       risk: Neurological science, “the                                                           normativity
       adolescent brain” and develop-
       mental theory

Content analysis                                                       neuroparenting literature. In this review we added a
                                                                       fourth layer: 4) Our critical examination of the criti-
Looking at the content of the critical literature, three               cal literature. In the following section we will mostly
lines of criticism emerge. First, a gap is identified                  focus on the third layer, the critical literature itself.
between available neuroscientific evidence and result-                 However, to understand and illustrate the arguments,
ing neuroparenting advice. Second, when translating                    we will also outline part of the neuroscientific litera-
scientific findings into parenting advice and practice,                ture and primary neuroparenting literature.
critics point out that translators’ implicit norms color
this translation. The third line of criticism regards                  Gap Between the Neuroscientific Evidence and
the way in which the advice is informed by and con-                    Neuroparenting Advice
tributes to harmful neoliberal ideas of the goal of
parenting.                                                             Neuroparenting advice typically presents itself as
   When examining the content of the critical litera-                  directly following from neuroscientific evidence. Dif-
ture we found it useful to distinguish between three                   ferent scholars in the critical literature have pointed
different layers. 1) The neuroscientific literature on                 out that the translation of neuroscience to policies and
which the neuroparenting advice is based. 2) The pri-                  parenting advice is not as straightforward as is often
mary neuroparenting literature, i.e. parenting books,                  assumed. ‘Evidence for policy making does not sim-
policy documents or media articles. 3) The critical                    ply repose in journals “ready to be harvested”’ [25].
sociological and ethical literature that evaluates the                 Science does not speak for itself, Shonkoff and Bales

Fig. 2  Field map on critical sociological and ethical literature on neuroparenting

                                                                                                                     13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

(2011) warn. Translating scientific findings to prac-      visual input in one eye from birth to three months old.
tices is a skill and the following four examples show      Afterwards, these kittens stayed blind permanently in
what this means.                                           the deprived eye. In contrast, eye-closure in adult cats
                                                           had no permanent effect [26]. While this shows that
The Scientific Myth of the ‘First Three Years              visual input in the first three months of life is neces-
Movement’                                                  sary for the development of normal vision in cats,
                                                           Bruer argues that the implications of this for par-
One of the earliest criticisms of neuroparenting found     enting humans during early infancy is unclear [23].
in our study is Bruer’s discussion of the “first three     Finally he argues that much of the advice was not
years” movement. Bruer, a former president of a            based on revolutionary new brain insights, but rather
foundation that supported research in cognitive devel-     on psychological theories such as attachment theory.
opment, child health, and brain development, noticed          More generally Bruer questions the notion of vul-
in the mid 90 s an increase in US media reports stat-      nerability and critical periods based on neuroscience.
ing that new brain science was about to revolutionize      Neuroscientific studies also present evidence that
child care and parenting. Bruer identified three spear     human development is a process of life-long learn-
point of these revolutionizing insights: i) there are      ing, based on the plasticity of the brain [23]. Based
critical periods – windows of opportunity – for brain      on these observations, Bruer concluded, in 1999, that
development that should not be missed; ii) during          the first three years movement, so prominent in policy
these critical periods, the brain needs the right stimu-   and media, is based on scientific myths. In an update
lation to develop well, if that does not happen, perma-    memo from 2011, he argued this still rings true: ‘The
nent damages can occur, iii) the first three years of a    evidentiary base for claims about early brain devel-
child’s life is a period of rapid synaptic growth, hence   opment does not seem to be expanding, the interpre-
this is an important critical period whereupon inter-      tations are not improving, and the same examples,
ventions should focus in order that the right stimula-     phrases, and images constantly recur.’ (page 11) [1].
tion occurs. He dubbed advice focused on these three          In a similar vein, Macvarish [1] showed that neu-
spearpoints ‘the first three years movement’ [23]. He      roscience gets invoked to support two rather opposite
noted that this interpretation of the neuroscience was     messages. When parenting advice is given, focus is
also informing early childhood policies.                   put on the vulnerability of the developing brain and
    Despite forceful claims to be based on novel scien-    the danger of inflicting irreversible harm. However,
tific findings, Bruer argued that the scientific under-    when arguing for the adoption of early interventions,
pinning of apparent critical periods very early in life    a focus on the plasticity of the brain and the reversi-
stemmed from 1) either preliminary neuroscientific         bility of early damage gets promoted. It is argued that
findings not yet understood in behavioral terms; 2)        the neuroscience is being invoked in an instrumental
animal studies without any obvious implications for        way, supporting whichever political agenda is pur-
humans or 3) pre-existing psychological theories on        sued, rather than setting or shaping the agenda itself.
attachment [23]. First, he points out that the neurosci-
entific research appealed to described brain structures    The mere rhetorical force of brain scan images
or mechanisms without detailing how these neural           for early intervention policies
changes influence behavior or development. Instead
the links to behavior tended to have the status of a       In 2002 neuroscientist Perry, who conducted research
hypothesis. In contrast those, such as policy makers,      on neglect, published a harrowing image of a CT-
who were interpreting the neuroscience in order to         scan of the brain of a severely neglected child (cf Pic-
develop parenting advice, jumped to unjustified con-       ture 1) [27].
clusions. Secondly, he pointed out that research cited        This image became prominent in UK policy docu-
to support the claim that there are critical periods of    ments advocating early intervention, even illustrating
brain development was done on animals and argued           the cover image of these reports [28, 29], However,
that its application to the human case was far from        the critical literature questions the representativeness
clear. For example, much cited in this movement is         of the image, suggesting it is used not because of its
a study that used kittens that had been deprived of        scientific validity but rather because of its rhetorical

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

                                                                    their children must learn to adapt. In this sense, co-
                                                                    sleeping, feeding on demand and baby slings charac-
                                                                    terize responsive parenting, whereas sleep-training,
                                                                    care scheduling and (forward-facing) prams indicate
                                                                    unresponsive parenting. Many parenting books pre-
                                                                    sent neuroscientific evidence to argue that responsive
                                                                    parenting is the best parenting style to reduce psycho-
                                                                    logical problems like anxiety in children [32].
                                                                       A review by neuroscientists Lupien and colleagues
                                                                    [33] is frequently cited in support. They reviewed
                                                                    studies on the influence of stress on the brain and
                                                                    argue that because humans are born relatively help-
                                                                    less they are very dependent on caregivers, and hence
Picture 1  Perry’s image of a CT scan of a healthy and              babies have a stress response system that is highly
extremely neglected 3 year old child (Perry 2002, p.93)             attuned to environmental cues [33]. In order to reduce
                                                                    the activation of infant’s stress system they recom-
force [25, 30, 31]. It is noted that no information                 mend responsive parenting [33].
regarding the case history of the child in question is                 Maxwell and Racine [24] question the scientific
provided. Without this information we cannot rule out               evidence reviewed by Lupien and colleagues. They
the possibility that the child might have had a mas-                point out that these studies typically measure stress
sive birth trauma, or some congenital condition that                by elevated cortisol levels. However the link between
caused the neglect and as a consequence the observed                higher cortisol levels at certain points in time and
aberrations. The brain scan image made famous by                    adverse behavioural outcomes is not well established
policy makers is much less instructive than often                   and much less evidence exists for the alleged irrevers-
claimed [25].                                                       ibility of the effects of high cortisol levels. Moreover,
   Perry’s original study [2] notes that in severely                the effect of responsive parenting on stress and stress
neglected children only 11 out of 17 brain scans                    reduction has not been studied at all. Therefore, they
(64.7%) were deviant. In other words more than a                    argue the claim that neuroscience shows that respon-
third were not. For children with a chaotic upbring-                sive parenting is essential for child wellbeing, is
ing, but no severe neglect, only 3 out of the 26 brain              unjustifiable [24].
scans (11.5%) were deviant [27]. So, while some chil-                  Macvarish [1] provides a different line of criti-
dren who experience severe neglect or adverse living                cism, showing that ‘responsive parenting’ is a revival
conditions show visible brain aberrations, many do                  of attachment parenting that became popular during
not. Perry himself publicly objected that his work was              the sixties. She notes that, while it was originally pro-
oversimplified and misinterpreted. His findings only                moted as one parenting style among equals, by sug-
related to extreme neglect and not broken homes,                    gesting that it has neuroscientific support, responsive
as some politicians suggested [15]. For example the                 parenting is presented as being best for brain devel-
neglected child whose brain scan is depicted was                    opment, implicitly disqualifying parents who parent
locked in a basement for several years [25].                        differently.

The Lack of Neuroscience Behind ‘Responsive                         The Limited Neuroscience Behind Neuroparenting
Parenting’                                                          Advice for Enriched Environments

‘Responsive parenting’ is another focus of the criti-               Nadesan’s [14] and Thornton’s [12] critical exami-
cal literature. This is a parenting style in which par-             nation focuses on the neuroparenting claim that
ents are continuously attuned to, and promptly react                enriched environments are crucial for proper brain
to any cues of children’s distress. Unresponsive par-               development and the resulting industry of supposed
enting, by contrast, is characterized by the parent                 brain stimulating toys. They argue that very little
setting scheduled sleep- and feeding times, to which                research has been done on which brain regions these

                                                                                                                 13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

toys stimulate nor on the long term cognitive effects      mechanisms, and hypothesizes what this can mean
they afford [12, 14].                                      for behavior. Policy makers and the general public
    For example, in 1998 the governor of Georgia,          too quickly equate the mechanisms with behavior. 3)
US, recommended to buy Mozart cd’s for every               In this translation from mechanisms to behavior, one
newborn, claiming that neuroscience had shown that         can easily come to different, quite opposite conclu-
listening to this music stimulates babies’ cognitive       sions. For example, the neuroscientific evidence can
development [13, 16]. However, the only study that         both support theories of vulnerability as of plastic-
demonstrated that listening to Mozart’s sonata could       ity. 4) Neuroscientific research is often static, or only
improve brain performance was conducted among              describes short time effects, yet the conclusions are
a small group of college students and the effect was       applied on development and long term outcomes. 5)
measured on a short-term, i.e. ten minutes after lis-      Neuroscience is sometimes used to give existing theo-
tening to the music [34]. No studies had been done         ries more weight, instead of generating new insights.
on babies, none had involved repeated brain meas-          In this way, neuroscience seemed to be used more in
urement and no long-term positive effects of music         rhetorical rather than scientific ways.
on brain development had ever been measured. This              Some critical scholars think it is too early for
example demonstrates how initial findings get extrap-      neuroscience to inform early child policies, that the
olated into claims on long-term brain development          current neuroscientific results are too preliminary to
[35].                                                      usefully inform child rearing advice. ‘As it turns out,
    A second focus of the critical literature are dubi-    the study of parenting and brain development is not
ous neuroparenting toys such as Disney’s series of         even yet in its infancy; it would be more appropriate
‘Baby Einstein’ animations. These animated DVDs            to conclude that it is still in the embryonic stage, if
for babies from one month old expose them to shapes,       not that which precedes conception.’ (page 410) [35].
colors, animals, music, art and even science. Disney       Bruer states: ‘We do not have a revolutionary, brain-
claimed that neuroscience has shown that this sup-         based action agenda for child development’, express-
ports cognitive development [12] saying in their           ing the fear that the wrongful application of neu-
1997 press release: ‘According to cognitive research,      roscience will give it a bad name, preventing future
dedicated neurons in the brain’s auditory cortex are       research to inform policies [23]. Many scholars argue
formed by repeated exposure to phonemes, the unique        that what is currently presented as neuroscience in
sounds of language. Studies show that if these neu-        media and policy documents is often a caricature of
rons are not used, they may die’. (cited in: [36]). How-   neuroscience: neuromyths, neurogossip, neurobabble,
ever, research found that infants aged 8–16 months         brain porn, neuroscience fiction, a mythological ver-
who watched the video’s scored lower on language           sion of the infant brain [13, 15, 25, 30]. In our discus-
development than their peers who didn’t watch ani-         sion, we will present some suggestion on how the gap
mations [37]. While this negative link was contested,      can be bridged.
a positive link between language development and the
mentioned animations could never be established [38]       Hidden Normativity in the Translation From
and Disney had to refund parents for providing mis-        Neuroscience to Practices
leading information [12].
                                                           The apparent gap between allegedly neuro-informed
                                                           policies and neuroparenting advice and actual scien-
Conclusion                                                 tific findings, leads to a second line of argument in
                                                           critical studies on neuroparenting. In the translational
In examining the criticisms focused on the translation     process from science to policy and parenting advice it
of the science, we identified five distinct pitfalls: 1)   is argued that preexisting normative judgments seep
Neuroscience is done within a specific, sometimes          in [4, 16]. Van de Werff [4] calls this phenomenon
exceptional context, but gets uncritically translated to   ‘value work’. How we attribute meaning to scientific
other groups and situations. For example, research on      findings is not an objective process, but also steered
severe neglect gets translated into regular parenting      by values. While this is partly inevitable because sci-
situations. 2) Neuroscientific research often describes    ence is always a social practice impacted by cultural

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

values, this does not justify biased or even discrimi-              are impoverishing or even neglecting their children
natory interpretation of findings [39]. However, vari-              [15]. Rather than fostering equal chances and break-
ous authors point out that the neuroscientific data                 ing social determinism, by invoking pre-existing
is interpreted in terms of pre-existing ideas of good               prejudices under the disguise of neuroscience, social
parenting [1, 4, 10, 40]. We present here a series of               differences are intensified, leading to further stigma
examples of the interpretation of neuroscience to                   and possible additional adverse effects for parents and
enforce existing power hierarchies with regard to                   children with a comparatively low social economic
class, ethnicity and gender in parenting. It is argued              status [15]. ‘Working-class parents, who lack the cul-
that these normative judgments tend to go unregis-                  tural capital of their middle-class counterparts, are
tered because people view neuroscientific evidence as               implicitly targeted as lacking the skills to adequately
objective and unbiased.                                             stimulate and prepare their infants.’ (page 423) [14].

Class and Socio‑Economic Status                                     Ethnicity and Cultural Variation in Child Rearing

At first glance, neuroparenting early intervention ini-             Elman [10] makes a similar argument with regard to
tiatives hold a positive message. If all children get the           ethnicity. She studied ethnicity bias in the presenta-
right brain stimulation and nurturing from birth, they              tion of neuroscientific results on the so-called ‘teen-
will no longer be held back by their social class. Neu-             age brain’ and the claim that generally adolescents
roscience can help to give everyone an equal chance,                exhibit more impulsive and more antisocial behavior
regardless of their social-economic background [41].                than both children and adults. Current neuroscientific
Before, we showed how research on severe neglect                    studies try to offer explanations for this erratic behav-
lead to pleads for early intervention policies that                 ior, such as imbalances in the development of dif-
focus on enriched environments and responsive par-                  ferent brain regions and delayed development of the
ents. However, it remains unclear what this research                prefrontal cortex responsible for self-regulation and
on severe neglect means for typical households,                     inhibition [4, 9–11].
where there is no neglect, let alone severe neglect. So                While the neuroscientific findings actually hold
far no scientific research has been done about what                 for all adolescents as an age-group, Elman outlines
the threshold is for an environment that is too poor to             how the same data leads to different judgements
safeguard proper brain development, and what counts                 depending on one’s ethnic group. When Caucasian
as an enriched environment [1, 23].                                 adolescents exhibit annoying, or antisocial behavior,
   Despite this lack of knowledge, popular media,                   the tendency is to whitewash it as being age-typical
policy reports, and parental educational material                   because adolescents’ brains are out of balance. How-
seems to equalize an enriched environment with a                    ever, when it comes to similar behavior in adolescents
white, middle-class environment [14–16, 42]. ‘Com-                  of color, neuroscience is not invoked to excuse it as
plex, enriched environments for humans end up hav-                  age-congruent, but rather to argue that these young-
ing many of the features of upper-middle class, urban,              sters’ brains were wired for violence from a very
and suburban life’ ([43], page 10). For example lis-                early age. Youth of color get excluded from childhood
tening to classical music, watching educational televi-             innocence, and therefore are more often institutional-
sion like Sesame street, or playing with certain edu-               ized [10].
cational toys like Lego building blocks are presented                  Another example is cultural variation in child rear-
as constituting an enriched environment. In contrast,               ing. The research on severe neglect shows the impor-
watching Sponge Bob or listening to rap music are                   tance of bonding, however, this concept also gets
hardly ever presented as being part of an enriched                  translated into a white, Western conception of ideal
environment [14]. Supposed neuroscience findings                    family life, wherein a nurturing, constantly avail-
are being used to reinforce existing values and cul-                able mother is essential [15]. This bonding, however,
tural norms, in the absence of scientific evidence on               could likewise occur between a baby and another sta-
what counts as enrichment. Equating enrichment with                 ble caregiver like a father, elder siblings, or grandpar-
white middle-class features suggests that people who                ents. Building family policies on an Eurocentric ideal
do not value typical middle or upper-class activities               of family life and suggesting that this is backed-up by

                                                                                                                 13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

objective neuroscience, risks pathologizing and sanc-       is only one scene in the entire series where a father
tioning culturally different but equally appropriate        is comforting a crying baby. O’Connor and Joffe [16]
practices of child-rearing [15, 42].                        analyzed 505 media articles on early development in
                                                            the UK, which focused on brain development. They
Gender                                                      conclude: ‘Mothers were generally positioned as the
                                                            target of parenting directives, with articles often using
The critical literature also points out that implicit       the word ‘mothers’ where the gender-neutral ‘parents’
normative ideas about gender color the interpretation       would have also been appropriate’ (page 304). Critics
of neuroscientific results. Macvarish [1] argues that       point out that neurodevelopmental research is often
neuromothering rather than neuroparenting better            used to reinforce traditional gender roles.
describes much of the advice. Two characteristics of
neuroparenting contain pitfalls for reinforcing tradi-
tional gender roles: the focus on very early interven-      Summary
tion (often prenatal) and the focus on the importance
of love.                                                    In this strand of the critical literature we can see how
   The strong focus on early intervention as prenatal       the translation of neuroscientific findings to practices
puts much pressure on mothers. A Unicef brochure            and parenting advice is not a clear-cut top down infu-
‘Building a happy baby’, argues that parents can            sion of science, but happens in interaction with pre-
stimulate neural development by stroking their bump,        existing values and norms. Although the reference to
playing music to the fetus, or reading a story to him       neuroscientific literature suggests that an objective
or her [3]. While this could technically also be done       basis had been established to distinguish between
by the father, it is suggested that this focus on inter-    good and bad, normal and abnormal parenting [44],
ventions starting in uterus are more likely to be felt as   the advices mostly echo existing ideas about class,
a responsibility for mothers, and most of the images        ethnicity, and gender. Biologising these differences in
in the brochure contain women [1].                          class, ethnicity and gender risks that already vulner-
   Jacobs and Hens [19] remark that the new, neuro-         able groups might be further stigmatized instead of
scientifically imbued discourse on the importance of        helped, increasing existing inequalities.
parental love, can largely be equated with the nine-
teenth century discourse on maternal love. In this dis-     Neuroparenting as a Form of Neoliberal
course maternal love and nurture were presented as a        Self‑Management
kind of ‘natural’ state of motherhood. Thornton [12]
argues that neuroparenting books encourage mothers          The final form of criticism of neuroparenting we iden-
to manage their emotions in order to ensure the emo-        tified is that it is part of a neoliberal tradition in which
tional well-being of their children. Mothers should         individuals are increasingly held responsible for their
internalize a loving, nurturing attitude, for example,      own success and that of their children. As ‘neolib-
smile and look at their baby when feeding. The mes-         eral’ can mean various things, we cite some defini-
sages this discourse sends is an obligation to enjoy        tions from the critical literature on neoliberalism: a
mothering, to not just act happily but sincerely be and     tradition in which ‘individual self-management, self-
feel happy when nurturing your child. In these par-         enhancement, and personal responsibility’ are key
enting books, fathers and their need to manage their        points [45]; ‘body/ self-maintenance have become the
emotions do not even get mentioned [12].                    new duties of the neoliberal citizen where, by looking
   It is also argued that this focus on mothers rather      after oneself one avoids being a financial liability to
than parents is present in many educational materi-         the state’ [44]; ‘entrepreneurial models of self-con-
als on neuroparenting, and media representations of         duct’ [12]. These definitions point towards a culture
early development. Wall [42] analyzed a series of vid-      of capitalistic self-improvement.
eos of a Canadian parental education campaign called            Neuroparenting is analysed as a form of gov-
‘Healthy Baby Healthy Brain’. She noticed that of the       ernmentality, in which people internalize certain
scenes depicting a parent interacting with their baby,      norms to become productive citizens [1, 6, 12, 14,
43 were of mothers and only seven of fathers. There         45]. It is suggested that the neoliberal norms behind

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

neuroparenting advice includes claims that children’s               duty of neoliberal citizens to avoid becoming a finan-
brain are highly malleable, that it is parents’ responsi-           cial burden to the state [15, 44, 46].
bility to properly form their children’s brains, and that
parents are blameworthy when things go wrong [1].                   An Unjustifiable Amount of Responsibility Attributed
This has been criticized on three accounts.                         to Parents

Tension Between Individual Interests of Child                       Neuroparenting advice is also criticized for its ideas
Wellbeing and Societal Economic Interests                           on malleability; that life can be orchestrated and that
                                                                    children can become whatever they want to if only
Neuroparenting holds a promise of bettering chil-                   their brains are stimulated early and intensively [47].
dren’s lives by providing them better cognitive and                 The UNICEF brochure entitled ‘Building a happy
social emotional development. However, several                      baby’ [3] (emphasis by the authors) expresses this
scholars have pointed towards an apparent difference                idea rather clearly, suggesting that raising a child is
between what parents think ‘better lives for children’              comparable to building a house. This triggers the
means, and what governments have in mind [14, 19].                  concern that whenever children do not perform as
While parents aim to see their children becoming                    intended, parents can be held personally responsible.
smart, social and happy, for governments ensuring                      O’Connor and Joffe [20] found in their analysis
social-emotional and cognitive development of chil-                 of media articles on early development that various
dren rather seems to be a means to raise more produc-               phenomena, like psychiatric disorders, obesity, alco-
tive citizens, and reduce antisocial behaviour [6, 12,              holism, and even sexual orientation are presented as
14, 19]. The main goal of early interventions hence                 direct consequences of ‘prenatal events impacting
might not be family wellbeing, but prevention of chil-              on the fetal brain’ (p.5) [16]. The Sun, for example
dren becoming a burden to society [14, 44, 45].                     released the following header: ‘Pregnant women
   For example UK early intervention policies often                 can impair their unborn tot’s IQ by eating liquorice,
refer to the economist Heckman who explicitly linked                researchers have warned.’ (7 October 2009). The crit-
child development to societal costs [44]. By training               ical literature outlines that warnings like these can put
parents to take care of their children’s brains, better             strong pressure on (prospective) parents, particularly
citizens can be formed. The titles of relevant policy               mothers, to ensure that they do not disrupt fetal brain
reports reflect this attitude. For example: ‘Good Par-              development [16]. As a consequence, there is an ever-
ents, Great Kids, Better Citizens’ [2], or ‘Early Inter-            stronger focus on the choices of individual parents
vention: Smart Investment’ [28]. Early brain interven-              and less acknowledgment of the wider social factors
tion will save the taxpayer money [22].                             that also influence development.
   It is argued that through a process of medicaliza-                  The critical literature points out that, based on the
tion, expert invasion, surveillance, and a shift from               neuroparenting trend, government programs indeed
societal to personal responsibility, governmental                   increasingly invest in individual parent-training,
norms of productivity get internalized by parents.                  while disinvesting in social support networks, like
Neuroparenting advice tends to medicalize normal                    good and affordable preschools or daycare [1, 45].
development, such as bonding with one’s baby, or the                This is especially hard on parents who are living in
changes during adolescence: ‘Neuroparenting formed                  relative poverty. The hopeful ethos neuroscience
one of rehabilitative citizenships’ lifelong treatment              seemed to entail – that all children, with the right
regimes for chronic youth’. ([10] p. 135) This rheto-               brain stimulation could overcome social adversities
ric of medicalization justifies formal surveillance                 – runs the risk to turn into a cruel understanding of
and intervention in young people’s lives [14]. Sev-                 success according to which people who do not suc-
eral authors in the critical literature conclude that the           ceed should take the blame for their failure [15].
main purpose of neuroparenting would be to train                       For comparatively well-settled middle-class par-
parents in entrepreneurial self-governance to deliver               ents a focus on malleability in the context of a com-
better citizens [12, 14, 15, 44]. Taking care of one’s              petitive society can also have detrimental effects.
own and one’s children’s brains becomes the new                     The critical literature outlines that neuroparenting is
                                                                    particularly popular among high- and middle-class

                                                                                                                 13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

parents. They have the resources to buy the ‘right’       Influence on Intimate Relationships Between Parents
toys, books, and other brain stimulating tools [1, 13].   and Children
However, these run the danger of changing responsive
parenting within a normal range, into behavior that       The neoliberal view implicit in neuroparenting advice
better would be called hyper-parenting, intensive par‑    has also been criticized as changing the role of par-
enting or even paranoid parenting [48]. These types       ents and influencing the intimate relationship between
of parenting are very time-consuming, and parents         parents and children. This supposedly happens in sev-
and children can experience this as stressful, while it   eral ways: 1) The process of bonding and other inti-
is unclear whether this extra stimulation is beneficial   mate rituals between parents and children gets instru-
[13, 48].                                                 mentalized; 2) Parents are stimulated to adopt the role
   For example Wall [18] described how Canadian           of managers and view their children as the passive
mothers are training their kindergarten-aged chil-        recipients of parents’ training program and; 3) The
dren in primary school subjects. They do so not only      relationship between parents and children is mediated
to ensure that their children have a better start once    by expert advice, while parental intuitive knowledge
they go to school, but also because of the continuous     is portrayed as insufficient.
media attention on how to boost one’s child’s brain           The critical literature outlines that for most par-
development and an increasing pressure among              ents bonding with and stimulating their child comes
middle-class parents to engage in these efforts. As a     naturally. Parents cuddle their newborn baby because
consequence, parents are constantly afraid that their     it feels nice, they play games with their children and
offspring might trail behind, believing other parents     make them laugh because that is fun and because they
train their children more. Parents themselves, how-       love them, not because neuroscience has appointed
ever, describe this kind of hyper-parenting as stress-    these behaviors as conducive for brain development.
ful, demanding, and occurring at the cost of their own    Parents perform these gestures not because they want
wellbeing [18]. In the end, it might undermine rather     to shape their children’s brains, but because they find
than increase well-being.                                 the gestures intrinsically rewarding. Putting these
   Chen [13] described the popularity of flash-           regular daily activities into the context of the need
cards on a private maternity ward in Taiwan. These        of bonding for the sake of healthy brain development
heibaika cards contain black and white silhouettes        runs the risk of instrumentalizing the loving relation-
of animals and everyday objects and they claim to         ship parents build with their young children. Current
stimulate brainpower of infants below three months.       intimate rituals of family life might be replaced by
Chen interviewed parents of newborns who stayed at        the new instrumentalized rituals of neuroparenting,
the ward. One parent reported keeping her newborn         which might aversively affect parent–child relation-
awake for up to one hour to train her with the cards.     ships [1].
However, there is no scientific evidence that these           Thornton describes how neuroparenting books are
cards stimulate newborn’s brain development, nor          ‘dedicated to codifying and specifying the minutiae
that this has any long-term positive effect. Instead,     of practices, expressions, and feelings that constitute
the popularity of these cards can be linked to an         effective maternal love’ (page 412) [12]. Parenting
‘ever-increasing anxiety among Taiwanese parents          is presented as a technical process that can be opti-
about competition and excellence in the globalising       mized, and where a wrong choice can have disastrous
world’ (p.4) [13]. Instead of empowering people, this     effects. Instructions on how to make eye contact with
anxiety makes parents vulnerable and invest money         one’s child is a suitable example. When parents make
and energy in initiatives that might have no positive     too little eye contact with their children, this can impair
but potentially negative effects, such as the described   language development, bonding and harm children’s
sleep deprivation in newborns. Neuroparenting             social, and emotional health. However, when parents
advice that was meant to help deprived children have      make too much eye contact, they risk overstimulating
a better start in life is taken up by high- and middle-   their child [49]. These instructions put great pressure on
class parents as a form of overdrive parenting that       parents, who must constantly ‘calibrate the appropriate
is rather geared at exceeding the norm than at safe-      quantity and timing of eye contact, precisely navigating
guarding normal development [12, 14, 18, 48].             the twin dangers of too little and too much’ (page 412)

13
Neuroparenting: the Myths and the Benefits. An Ethical Systematic Review

[12]. While eye contact might be optimised from a neu-              literature, we noticed a series of shortcomings. In
roscience point of view, parent–child relationships are             order to further the debate on the worth and the lim-
likely to suffer.                                                   its of neuroscience informed parenting, it is important
    Jacobs and Hens [19] warn that in the current dis-              to also identify these shortcomings and show where,
course parental love is presented as a duty, as a neces-            when, and why neuroscience findings on parent-
sity in the child’s development, such as clothing and               ing are not necessarily doomed to lead to a series of
feeding. This, however, might not only lead to an                   problems but could also result in benefits and support
instrumentalisation of love, but, philosophically speak-            child and family well-being. Our impression is that
ing, also leads to an absurd situation, because ‘love’ as           the critical literature solely focuses on potential pit-
such cannot be requested as one cannot will oneself to              falls and drawbacks but fails to acknowledge the con-
love [19]. This duty to love can paradoxically disrupt              structive potential and possible benefits entailed.
normal bonding by stimulating a mechanic and instru-                    We will discuss four shortcomings of the criti-
mentalized form of parental love, and result in children            cal literature itself and provide suggestions on how
lacking in affect [1, 42].                                          to balance potential harms and pitfalls with possible
    Two metaphors seem to depict this new, instrumen-               benefits and advantages of neuroscience-informed
talised role of parents rather clearly. Leysen [17] speaks          approaches to parenting practices. Given the great
of parents becoming ‘parenters’: ‘a figure performing               diversity of neuro-practices, we first question whether
learned parenting tasks, directed to act in a specific way          the general term of ‘neuroparenting’ is even useful.
towards the specific goal of optimal brain development’             We then discuss the proper boundaries of neuropar-
(p.252). Nadesan [14] describes how parents are made                enting practices. Third, we argue for the importance
into ‘managers’ and their children become ‘entrepre-                of a strong evidence base of the neuroparenting
neurial subjects’. Parents risk to become more focused              advice. Finally, we look at the needs and experiences
on their children’s brains than on their children as per-           of parents and adolescents themselves investigating
sons and children risk being seen as engineering fail-              the alleged harm of such practices.
ures rather than valuable in themselves [14].
    In this view of parenting, children are presented as            The Generalizability of Results: the Diversity of
merely passive recipients of parenting, with no pref-               Neuroparenting
erences, capacities or talents of their own [18]. The
parental duty to shape children’s brains might result in            Macvarish [1] was one of the first to coin the term
ever more top-down relationships between parents and                ‘neuroparenting’ as an overarching concept that
children. Instead of fostering familial interaction where           describes how childrearing practices are increasingly
parents and children learn from each other [4].                     informed by neuroscientific findings. In this review,
    At the same time, parents themselves are consid-                we followed this terminology. The advantage is that
ered in constant need of expert knowledge. In order to              it becomes apparent how a variety of local practices
become able to shape children’s brains properly, parents            are part of a larger, social movement with potentially
need to comply to expert advice and educate them-                   shared risks and drawbacks. However, the concept
selves with the latest neurobiological insights. Macvar-            itself entails the pitfall that the diversity of practices
ish called this the expert invasion of family life [1, 31].         get generalized under the same umbrella. As a conse-
Existing parental knowledge and intuition becomes dis-              quence, the diversity of implications of various prac-
carded [17] and parents become mere amateurs whose                  tices tends to get overlooked. The critical literature
knowledge about child development and whose cogni-                  then risks throwing the baby out with the bathwater,
tive capacities are insufficient to adequately take care of         overlooking the potential, benefits and advantages of
their children.                                                     some neuroscientific findings. The potentially posi-
                                                                    tive impact on parents, educators and the wider social
                                                                    environment is also ignored.
Critique of the Critical Literature                                    For example, neuroparenting practices vary widely
                                                                    in different countries. In the US, neuroparenting lit-
The critical studies of neuroparenting perform impor-               erature on adolescence emphasises the explanation of
tant pioneering work. However, when reviewing this                  the deviant functioning of adolescent brains and aims

                                                                                                                  13
A. Snoek, D. Horstkötter

to provide a recipe against violent behaviours, like         contact or breastfeeding as being ‘mere neuroparent-
school shootings. [10]. In the Netherlands, by con-          ing’, depends itself on a reductionist understanding of
trast, Van de Werff [4] showed that the neuroscientist       the worth of these practices.
Crone’s prominent parenting books on adolescence                Another example comes from Wall’s qualitative
contain an explicitly positive perspective on adoles-        study on parents’ experiences with intensive parent-
cent brains, as a unique phase of creativity, positive       ing and the ‘brain development discourse’ [18]. In
risk taking and social connectivity. This unique phase       her conclusions, Wall argued that the brain develop‑
is presented as offering adolescents benefits on the         ment discourse led to an escalation of parenting into
job market [4, 50]. These differences in the evaluation      hyper-parenting. However, in the quotes of respond-
of the value and challenges of adolescent brains for         ents, there is hardly any explicit reference to the brain
young people’s behavior are striking. They also cast         development discourse or to neuroscience. Respond-
doubt on the concept of ‘neuroparenting’ as singular         ents mainly talked about intensive parenting and
and uniform. Instead, it seems more appropriate to           their wish that their children will do well in school,
specify the practice a critical debate has in mind.          but they did not link this to neuroscientific findings
   Similarly, it is striking that most critical reports on   or the brain discourse. Wall seems to equate intensive
early interventions target UK policy documents [6,           parenting with neuroparenting, thereby overlooking
15, 25, 30], while in other countries, corresponding         the possibility that intensive parenting stems from a
debates hardly seem to exist. Have other countries           broader tradition of maximizing a child’s chances in a
found more sound and agreeable ways to use neu-              competitive world.
roscience to inform their early childhood policies?             Practices should always be interpreted within
Leysen found many differences between Flemish and            their context, paying attention to cultural, familial,
Anglo Saxon neuroscientific parental educational             and social meanings. In the same way as scientific
material. The Flemish tend to focus less on the first        approaches should not limit their understanding of
three years movement, present less evidence from             children’s development to brain development, ethical
severe neglect, and had less deterministic views [47].       critiques of such approaches should not limit them-
Again it is not clear that criticism based on the prac-      selves to this focus.
tices in one country generalize to practices in other
countries.
                                                             The Importance of the Evidence Base of
                                                             Neuroparenting Practices
Defining Neuroparenting
                                                             As we outlined earlier, some critical scholars identi-
Some critical authors define certain parenting prac-
                                                             fied a gap between neuroscientific evidence and the
tices as neuro-parenting practices, when it seems to
                                                             neuroparenting advice based on it (see for example:
us that the impact of the prefix ‘neuro’ is doubtful.
                                                             [23, 24, 35]). However, other scholars explicitly state
Obviously, the brain is involved in almost all human
                                                             that the scientific validity underlying neuroparenting
processes, and most practices impact the brain. How-
                                                             advice is outside the scope of their critique. Elman
ever, labelling all kinds of practices that influence
                                                             [10] and Wall [45] respectively, argue as follows:
brain development as neuro-interventions can be
rather confusing and undermine the quality of the               It is not the task of this chapter to evaluate the
critical debate.                                                scientific validity of this body of knowledge, but
   Macvarish [1] or example, terms the advice on the            rather to analyse its cultural work. This chap-
importance of breastfeeding and skin-to-skin con-               ter offers an analysis of the cultural stakes and
tact between mothers and newborn babies as forms                knowledge-power of brain-based thinking about
of neuroparenting. Indeed, these practices are often            adolescence as it pervaded popular culture of
offered together with neuroscientific evidence that             the 1990s and beyond. (page 133) [10]
they improve bonding and brain development [1].                 My aim here is not to establish the truth or
But these practices could also be interpreted as part           falsity of the scientific claims being made,
of a countermovement against the medicalization of              but rather to suggest that, like other scien-
childbirth. In that sense, dismissing advice on skin            tific claims, they are not beyond question and

13
You can also read