Stress Inoculation-Induced Indications of Resilience in Monkeys

Page created by Angela Carr
 
CONTINUE READING
Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 20, No. 4, August 2007, pp. 423–433 (
                                                                       C 2007)

Stress Inoculation-Induced Indications
of Resilience in Monkeys
                              David M. Lyons and Karen J. Parker
                              Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University Medical Center,
                              Stanford, CA

                     The negative consequences of stress are well-recognized in mental health research. Exposure to early life
                     stressors, for example, increases the risk for the development of mood, anger, anxiety, and substance abuse
                     disorders. Interestingly, however, early life stressors have also been linked to the subsequent development
                     of resilience. Variously described as inoculating, immunizing, steeling, toughening, or thriving, the
                     hypothesis that early life stressors provide a challenge that, when overcome, induces adaptations that
                     enhance emotional processing, cognitive control, curiosity, and neuroendocrine regulation is examined
                     in this review of squirrel monkey research.

    Early exposure to severe forms of stress is a risk factor                                Garmezy, 1991; Rutter, 1993; Yates, Egeland, & Sroufe,
for the development of subsequent psychopathology (Agid                                      2003). In this review, we summarize our studies designed
et al., 1999; Davidson, Stein, Shalev, & Yehuda, 2004; Foa,                                  to test the hypothesis that early life stressors may foster the
Stein, & McFarlane, 2006; Heim, Plotsky, & Nemeroff,                                         development of resilience as modeled in squirrel monkeys.
2004). Far less researched, but of equal importance, are                                        Monkeys raised in groups comprised of 3–4 mother–
indications that early life stressors may also foster resilience.                            infant pairs were initially randomized to a stress inocula-
Adults cope better with spousal loss, illness, and major                                     tion protocol or a no-stress control condition at 17 weeks of
accidents if they have previously experienced and coped                                      age. The stress inoculation protocol consisted of 10 weekly
with stressors in childhood (Forest, 1991; Khoshaba &                                        1-hour social separations (Parker, Buckmaster, Schatzberg,
Maddi, 1999). Work-related stressors likewise have fewer                                     & Lyons, 2004) designed to provide repeated opportunities
depressive effects in adults previously exposed to work-                                     for emotional activation and cognitive processing while not
related stressors in adolescence (Mortimer & Staff, 2004).                                   overwhelming the capacity for coping with adversity. Dur-
These findings suggest that stressful events that are not                                     ing each 1-hour separation session, each monkey was indi-
overwhelming, but challenging enough to elicit emotional                                     vidually placed in a cage with various toy-like objects, and
activation and cognitive processing may make subsequent                                      could see, hear, and smell, but not touch other unfamiliar
coping efforts more efficient, and therefore easier and more                                  monkeys. Separations evoked repetitive distress peep-calls,
likely to be used later in life (Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005;                                   locomotor agitation, and acute elevations in plasma cortisol

Supported by the Nancy Pritzker Network for the Study of Depression, and Public Health Service grants DA16902, MH77884, MH66573, and MH47573. The authors declare no
competing interests. All procedures were approved by Stanford University’s Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care and were carried out in accordance with the NIH Guide
for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: David M. Lyons, Psychiatry Neuroscience, Stanford University Medical Center, 1201 Welch Road, MSLS P104–Mail
Code 5485, Stanford, CA 94305-5485. E-mail: dmlyons@stanford.edu.

C 2007 International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/jts.20265

                                                                                     423
424                                                                Lyons and Parker

levels with baseline measures restored soon after each so-
cial reunion with the natal group (Coe, Glass, Wiener, &
Levine, 1983; Hennessy, 1986; Stanton & Levine, 1985).
Separations were scheduled at weekly intervals to allow
ample time for recovery at an age when squirrel monkeys
are typically developing physical and psychosocial inde-
pendence (Boinski & Fragaszy, 1989; Rosenblum, 1968).
The transition from filial, mother-directed behavior (e.g.,
clinging and nursing) to exploratory, other-directed be-
havior (e.g., object exploration and social play) presents
important learning opportunities that are thought to pro-
mote the development of psychosocial functions (Lyons,
1993; Mason, 1971).

EMOTIONAL AND PSYCHOSOCIAL
INDICATIONS OF RESILIENCE
At 9 months of age, each monkey was tested along with
its mother for 30 minutes on 5 consecutive days in a novel
environment that contained an assortment of foods and ob-
                                                                                Figure 1. Emotional and psychosocial indications of re-
jects (Parker et al., 2004). Similar test conditions have been                  silience are enhanced by stress inoculation. Measures of (A)
used to demonstrate that prior exposure to overwhelming                         time spent clinging to mother and (B) frequency of object ex-
postnatal stressors increases indications of anxiety-related                    ploration during repeated novel environment test sessions are
                                                                                presented for stress inoculated (n = 11) and noninoculated
psychopathology in marmoset monkeys and macaques
                                                                                (n = 9) monkeys (M + SEM ). A significant postnatal stress
(Andrews & Rosenblum, 1993; Dettling, Feldon, & Pryce,                          inoculation treatment-by-test session interaction ( p < .05)
2002; Hinde & Spencer-Booth, 1971). Here the novel en-                          was discerned for both measures (adapted from data presented
vironment stress test was used to assess stress inoculation-                    in Parker et al., 2004).
induced indications of resilience.
    The behavior of stress inoculated and noninoculated
monkeys was initially similar, but differences emerged over                     calls and more time spent near peers (Levine & Mody,
repeated test sessions in the novel environment (Figure 1).                     2003; Lyons, Martel, Levine, Risch, & Schatzberg, 1999).
Gradually, stress-inoculated compared to noninoculated
monkeys appeared to become less anxious as inferred by                          Cognitive Indications of Resilience
decreased maternal clinging and increased object explo-
ration. As described elsewhere in greater detail (Parker                        At 1.5 years of age, the stress inoculated and noninoculated
et al., 2004), these results indicate that stress inoculated                    monkeys were administered a test to examine prefrontal-
monkeys more readily self-regulate arousal and engage in                        dependent cognitive control of behavior described else-
more exploration than noninoculated monkeys. This con-                          where in detail (Parker, Buckmaster, Justus, Schatzberg, &
clusion is consistent with earlier findings from different                       Lyons, 2005). Briefly, a clear Plexiglas box with one open
cohorts of stress-inoculated monkeys that responded to the                      side was baited with a favorite food treat. When the box
removal of mothers at weaning with fewer distress peep-                         opening was oriented straight toward the monkey, food

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Stress Inoculation in Monkeys                                                                425

retrieval was achieved by reaching straight into the box.
When the box was rotated 90 degrees so the opening was
oriented toward either side, inhibition of straight-reaching
and a detour-reach around either side was required to
retrieve food from the clear baited box. Reaches aimed
straight toward the center of the box when the opening
was oriented toward either side were scored as response
inhibition errors.
    Inhibitory control of the reaching response in marmoset
monkeys and rhesus macaques is impaired by lesions of pre-
frontal cortex (Diamond, 1990; Dias, Robbins, & Roberts,
1996; Wallis, Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 2001), but not                    Figure 2. Prefrontal-dependent cognitive response inhibi-
lesions of the hippocampus (Diamond, Zola-Morgan, &                       tion is enhanced by stress inoculation. Stress inoculated mon-
Squire, 1989). Response inhibition assessed on this test is               keys (n = 11) completed more successful test trials with the
                                                                          box opening oriented toward either side compared to non-
also impaired in a marmoset monkey model of parental ne-                  inoculated monkeys (n = 9) examined over successive test
glect (Pryce, Dettling, Spengler, Spaete, & Feldon, 2004),                days (mean + SEM). Maximum score is 6 successful tri-
and in squirrel monkeys treated with cortisol according to                als per day. A significant postnatal stress inoculation treat-
a protocol that simulates a chronic stress response (Lyons,               ment main effect ( p < .05) was discerned over repeated test
                                                                          days. From “Prospective Investigation of Stress Inoculation
Lopez, Yang, & Schatzberg, 2000). Here this test of cog-                  in Young Monkeys,” by K. J. Parker, C. L. Buckmaster,
nitive control was used to assess stress inoculation-induced              A. F. Schatzberg, and D. M. Lyons, 2004, Archives of General
indications of resilience. As indicated below, prefrontal-                Psychiatry, 61, 933–941. Copyright 2004 by the American
dependent cognitive control likely plays a role in emotion                Medical Association. Adapted with permission.
regulation and resilience.
    Initially, each monkey was administered 10 trials per
day for 7 consecutive days with the box opening always                        These results are in keeping with evidence that postna-
oriented straight toward the monkey subject. Food re-                     tal stress inoculation stimulates the development of larger
trieval was achieved by reaching straight into the center                 prefrontal cortical volumes without affecting hippocam-
of the box. These 70 trials served to reinforce as prepotent              pal volumes determined in squirrel monkeys by high-
the straight-reaching response. Thereafter, each monkey                   resolution magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Lyons, Afar-
was administered 10 trials per day for 14 consecutive days                ian, Schatzberg, Sawyer-Glover, & Moseley, 2002; Lyons,
with the orientation of the box opening systematically var-               Yang, Sawyer-Glover, Moseley, & Schatzberg, 2001). Pre-
ied to assess inhibitory control of the straight-reaching                 frontal corticolimbic circuits play a key role in cognitive
response. The stress inoculated and noninoculated mon-                    control of behavior in humans and monkeys (Garavan,
keys performed equally well on all trials except for those                Ross, & Stein, 1999; Konishi, Nakajima, Uchida, Sekihara,
that required inhibitory control of the straight-reaching                 & Miyashita, 1998; Lyons et al., 2000; Miller & Cohen,
response. Performance on the side-facing trials improved                  2001; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis,
gradually over repeated test days (Figure 2), but at peak                 2004). Increased prefrontal activation, for example, cor-
levels of performance all stress-inoculated monkeys suc-                  responds with diminished amygdala activation, and with
cessfully completed all response inhibition trials whereas                increased cognitive control of emotion in humans stud-
fewer than half of the noninoculated monkeys achieved                     ied by functional MRI (Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Taylor,
similar levels of success.                                                Eisenberger, Saxbe, Lehman, & Lieberman, 2006; Urry

                        Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
426                                                                Lyons and Parker

et al., 2006). In ongoing research we plan to assess whether
stress inoculation enhances prefrontal inhibition of amyg-
dala activity in monkeys.

CURIOSITY AND EXPLORATION-BASED
INDICATIONS OF RESILIENCE
At 2.5 years of age, the stress-inoculated and noninocu-
lated monkeys were administered a test to examine cu-
riosity and the exploratory behavior it elicits. This was
accomplished by monitoring exploration of novel and fa-
miliar objects presented in a stress-free condition (Parker
et al., 2007). Briefly, a familiar and a novel object were
each secured to the back wall of a test compartment. The
compartment was affixed to the monkey’s home cage 5
minutes before the start of each test, after which time a
sliding door was removed and the monkey had free access
to the test compartment. Test sessions lasted 30 minutes
in duration, and each monkey was tested on 5 consecutive
days. Similar test situations have been used to demon-
strate that prior exposure to overwhelming postnatal stres-
sors reduces exploration of novelty in macaque monkeys
(Hinde & Spencer-Booth, 1971; Roder, Timmermans, &
Vossen, 1989; Sackett, 1972). Here exploration of novelty
was used to assess stress inoculation-induced indications
of resilience. As described below, information-seeking and                      Figure 3. Curiosity and exploratory behavior are enhanced
sensory types of curiosity play a role in the maintenance of                    by stress inoculation. Measures of (A) latency to enter the
life-long resilience.                                                           test compartment, (B) frequency of entries into the compart-
                                                                                ment, and (C) duration of time spent inside the compartment
    All stress inoculated and all but one of the noninoculated                  across repeated test sessions are presented for stress inoculated
monkeys approached the test compartment and looked                              (n = 11) and noninoculated (n = 9) monkeys (M + SEM ).
inside. However, more stress inoculated than noninocu-                          A significant postnatal stress inoculation treatment main ef-
lated monkeys entered the compartment, and stress inoc-                         fect ( p < .05) was discerned across repeated test sessions
                                                                                for all three measures of behavior. From “Early Life Stress
ulated monkeys entered faster, with greater frequency, and                      and Novelty Seeking Behavior in Adolescent Monkeys,” by
spent more time inside the test compartment compared to                         K. J. Parker, K. L. Rainwater, C. L. Buckmaster, A. F.
noninoculated monkeys (Figure 3). In addition, more                             Schatzberg, S. E. Lindley and D. M. Lyons, 2007, Psychoneu-
stress-inoculated than noninoculated monkeys explored                           roendocrinology, in press. Copyright 2007 by Elsevier Ltd.
                                                                                Adapted with permission.
the objects inside the compartment, and stress-inoculated
monkeys preferred to explore the novel compared to the                          cantly more object exploration compared to noninoculated
familiar object (Parker et al., 2007).                                          monkeys (Figure 1). Object exploration in the earlier study
    In the earlier novel environment stress test conducted at                   was assessed, however, in inescapable conditions that in-
9 months of age, inoculated monkeys also showed signifi-                         volved involuntary separation of monkeys from the home

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Stress Inoculation in Monkeys                                                                427

cage. Both stress inoculated and noninoculated monkeys
in the earlier study presented with significantly higher cor-
tisol levels immediately after the novel environment stress
test compared to baseline levels measured in undisturbed
home cage conditions (Parker et al., 2004). Moreover, be-
cause posttest levels of cortisol were significantly lower in
stress inoculated compared to noninoculated monkeys, dif-
ferences in objection exploration in the earlier study were
confounded with differences in stress-related anxiety.
    In the study conducted at 2.5 years of age, exploratory
behavior was assessed in the test compartment attached to
each monkey’s home cage. To further reduce the poten-
tially stressful nature of the test situation, each monkey
was extensively acclimated to the procedures and test com-                Figure 4. Conceptual model of how stress fosters the devel-
partment prior to the start of the study. Stress hormones                 opment of resilience.
measured immediately after Test Sessions 1 and 5 con-
firmed that posttest cortisol levels were not significantly
different from baseline levels in undisturbed home cage
conditions at 2.5 years of age. Moreover, neither base-                   pared to noninoculated monkeys. Information-seeking and
line nor posttest cortisol levels correlated with measures                sensory types of curiosity motivate individuals to seek
of exploration in the stress-free conditions at 2.5 years of              new opportunities for action (Reio, Petrosko, Wiswell, &
age. Individual differences previously observed in object                 Thongsukmag, 2006). Engagement in new situations that
exploration at 9 months of age were, however, correlated                  entail challenging, but not overwhelming stress may further
with the rank order of individual differences in test com-                induce neuroadaptations that enhance cognitive control
partment entries, r s = .41, p < .05, and total time spent                and emotional self-regulation. According to this concep-
in the test compartment, r s = .51, p < .05, at 2.5 years of              tual working model (Figure 4), curiosity thereby plays a role
age. Individual differences previously observed in cognitive              in the life-long maintenance of stress inoculation-induced
control of behavior at 1.5 years of age were also correlated              resilience.
with test compartment entries, r s = .48, p < .05, and total
time spent in the test compartment, r s = .42, p < .05, at
2.5 years of age.                                                         IS STRESS INOCULATION-INDUCED
    Taken together, these data suggest that postnatal stress              RESILIENCE MATERNALLY MEDIATED?
inoculation affects interrelated aspects of cognitive con-
trol, emotion regulation, and curiosity consistently ex-                  Stress inoculation-induced resilience in monkeys resembles
pressed over time and across situations. Exploration of nov-              the effects of postnatal handling in studies of rats. Neona-
elty reflects more than diminished anxiety because stress-                 tal rats removed from the nest and briefly separated from
inoculated monkeys are more curious than noninoculated                    nest-related cues respond with behavioral and physiological
monkeys when tested in the absence of adrenocortical or                   indications of arousal (Hofer, 1996; Lyons & Banks, 1982).
behavioral indications of anxiety. Exploration of novelty                 When studied as adults, these rats then display increased ex-
and curiosity does not, in turn, reflect impulsivity because               ploration, diminished emotionality, improved learning and
stress-inoculated monkeys more readily exercise cognitive                 memory, and diminished activation of the hypothalamic–
control and successfully inhibit impulsive behavior com-                  pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis by various stressors relative to

                        Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
428                                                                Lyons and Parker

rats that are raised in undisturbed conditions (Fernandez-                      hypothesis thus predicts that offspring removed along with
Teruel et al., 2002; Levine, 2000; Meaney et al., 1996).                        their mother will not develop resilience due to diminished
   In rats, these outcomes seem to reflect the effects of                        levels of maternal care. In contrast, the stress-inoculation
maternal behavior directed toward pups that are briefly                          hypothesis predicts that resilience will develop in monkeys
separated and returned to the nest (Denenberg, 1999;                            exposed to either intermittent separation condition because
Smotherman & Bell, 1980). Brief intermittent separations                        both conditions elicit acute anxiety and transiently activate
increase maternal licking and grooming not only immedi-                         the HPA axis.
ately after reunion, but also throughout postnatal develop-                         Unlike brief separations in rats, neither separation con-
ment (Lee & Williams, 1974). Importantly, the neuroen-                          dition in monkeys caused a permanent increase in lev-
docrine indications of resilience observed in rats exposed to                   els of maternal behavior (Parker et al., 2006). Moreover,
brief intermittent postnatal separations can be replicated in                   group differences in maternal behavior did not correspond
unmanipulated offspring that naturally receive high levels                      with the development of neuroendocrine indications of
of maternal care (Liu et al., 1997). That increased maternal                    resilience. Juvenile monkeys exposed with their mother to
care is sufficient to produce resilience in rats is corroborated                 repeated separations received less maternal care in the home
by evidence that the amount of maternal stimulation re-                         cage throughout development compared to juveniles sepa-
ceived by pups during development is negatively correlated                      rated alone, or juveniles from undisturbed groups. Yet both
with later stress-induced activation of the HPA axis (Liu                       separation conditions induced in juvenile monkeys subse-
et al., 1997; Rosenberg, Denenberg, & Zarrow, 1970).                            quent neuroendocrine indications of resilience compared
   To determine whether the development of resilience in                        to juveniles not exposed to either separation condition.
monkeys exposed to intermittent separations is maternally                       Specifically, juveniles separated alone or separated along
mediated, we recently examined maternal behavior and                            with their mother exhibited diminished cortisol responses
subsequent neuroendocrine measures of resilience in 30                          to a subsequent novel environment stress test compared to
monkeys randomized to three postnatal treatment condi-                          monkeys that were not previously exposed to either of the
tions (Parker, Buckmaster, Sundlass, Schatzberg, & Lyons,                       two separation conditions (Parker et al., 2006).
2006). In one condition, each juvenile monkey was sepa-
rated from its mother and the natal group for 10 weekly
sessions that each lasted 1-hour in duration. In the sec-
ond condition, each juvenile monkey and its mother were                         LONG-LASTING NEUROENDOCRINE EFFECTS
removed together from the group for 10 weekly 1-hour ses-                       OF STRESS INOCULATION
sions. Both of these two conditions elicit distress peep-calls,
locomotor agitation, and transiently activate the HPA axis                      The findings described above suggest that the development
with baseline measures restored soon after reunion with the                     of resilience corresponds more closely to prior stress expo-
natal group (Coe et al., 1983; Jordan, Hennessy, Gonzalez,                      sure than to differences in the quantity or quality of mater-
& Levine, 1985). In the third condition, nonseparated                           nal care. A follow-up experiment confirmed this conclusion
monkeys remained in their natal groups.                                         in a different cohort of squirrel monkeys exposed from 10–
   The maternal mediation hypothesis predicts that juve-                        22 weeks of age to a high-demand foraging condition or a
nile monkeys exposed alone to repeated separations will                         low-demand foraging control. During the 12-week high-
receive consistently more maternal care than age-matched                        demand condition, squirrel monkey mothers stopped car-
undisturbed monkeys. Conversely, the repeated removal of                        rying their juvenile offspring at earlier ages and spent more
both mothers and offspring will diminish maternal behav-                        time foraging for food (Lyons, Kim, Schatzberg, & Levine,
ior as stressed mothers direct their attention toward rein-                     1998). Plasma cortisol levels were intermittently higher
tegration with the natal group. The maternal mediation                          in high-demand compared to low-demand offspring, but

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Stress Inoculation in Monkeys                                                                429

foraging demands did not affect postnatal growth, nursing                  of HPA axis activation because endogenous cortisol cannot
behavior, or amounts of solid-food consumed.                               be distinguished from exogenous hydrocortisone (Lyons
    By selectively reducing certain aspects of maternal                    et al., 2004).
care, squirrel monkey mothers seem to protect their                            A significant postnatal foraging condition-by-
offspring from the full impact of the stressful forag-                     hydrocortisone pretreatment interaction was discerned
ing task (Lyons et al., 1998). A similar process appears                   in postrestraint measures of ACTH with baseline levels
to occur in studies of bonnet macaques. Whereas un-                        controlled as a statistical covariate. Subsequent analysis
predictable variable-demand foraging conditions impair                     of simple main effects confirmed that monkeys from
emotional (Rosenblum & Andrews, 1994) and neurobio-                        the high-demand condition responded with diminished
logical (Coplan et al., 2006) aspects of development, con-                 activation of the HPA axis determined by smaller increases
sistently predictable high-demand foraging does not ap-                    in postrestraint ACTH levels compared to monkeys
pear to adversely affect development in bonnet macaques                    from the low-demand condition after pretreatment with
(Rosenblum & Paully, 1984).                                                saline (Figure 5). As expected, hydrocortisone suppressed
    Based on these findings, we studied squirrel monkeys                    postrestraint stress levels of ACTH, but enduring postnatal
raised in consistently high- versus low-demand foraging                    effects were not discerned when restraint was preceded
conditions for subsequent differences in neuroendocrine                    by hydrocortisone to test for differences in sensitivity to
indications of resilience (Parker et al., 2006). The maternal              glucocorticoid feedback.
mediation hypothesis predicts that diminished levels of ma-                    In rodents, neuroendocrine indications of resilience are
ternal care induced by high-demand foraging will increase                  induced by maternal stimulation and mediated, in part, by
subsequent HPA axis responses to stress in monkeys pre-                    enhanced sensitivity to glucocorticoid feedback (Liu et al.,
viously raised in the high- versus low-demand condition.                   1997; Meaney et al., 1996). In monkeys, we failed to find
Conversely, if early life stressors foster the development of              evidence for maternal mediation or enhanced sensitivity to
resilience, then HPA axis responses to subsequent stressors                glucocorticoid feedback. Our finding that increased mater-
in adulthood should be diminished in the monkeys raised                    nal stimulation is not required to foster resilience does not,
in high- versus low-demand foraging conditions.                            however, diminish the role of parenting in primate devel-
    In midlife adulthood at ∼8 years of age, restraint stress              opment. In our studies (Lyons et al., 1998), mother–infant
tests were administered to squirrel monkeys raised from                    attachment relationships were established well before on-
10–22 weeks of age in high- versus low-demand conditions.                  set of the postnatal protocols and all monkeys received
Restraint is a well-studied psychological stressor in ani-                 more than the typical levels of maternal care experienced
mal biomedical research (Glavin, Pare, Sandbak, Bakke, &                   in naturalistic conditions (Boinski & Fragaszy, 1989). Psy-
Murison, 1994). An intramuscular injection of saline was                   chosocial support derived from attachment relationships
given 60 minutes before initiation of the first 30-minute                   promotes coping skills in humans (Cohen & Wills, 1985)
restraint stress. Seven days later, an intramuscular injec-                and nonhuman primates (Harlow & Suomi, 1974; Mason
tion of exogenous cortisol (i.e., 2.5 mg/kg hydrocortisone                 & Capitanio, 1988), and a minimal threshold of mater-
sodium succinate) was given 60 minutes before the second                   nal care is likely required for the development of stress
and otherwise identical restraint stress test. This dose of                inoculation-induced resilience.
hydrocortisone was used to assess glucocorticoid feedback,
and is known to suppress stress-induced increases in squir-                SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
rel monkey adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH; Lyons,
Yang, Eliez, Reiss, & Schatzberg, 2004). Plasma levels of                  The studies reviewed above suggest that repeated ex-
ACTH were determined 0, 30, and 60 minutes after com-                      posure to early life stressors that are challenging, but
pletion of each restraint stress test, and analyzed as an index            not overwhelming foster the development of interrelated

                         Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
430                                                                Lyons and Parker

                                                                                molecular explanations of stress resistance (Maier, Amat,
                                                                                Baratta, Paul, & Watkins, 2006; Meaney & Szyf, 2005).
                                                                                Differences in prefrontal neuroanatomy limit prospects for
                                                                                rodent research (Ongur & Price, 2000; Preuss, 1995),
                                                                                and access to human brain tissue occurs less often than
                                                                                in other fields of medicine where tissue biopsies are per-
                                                                                formed. In the rare case where human brain tissue is made
                                                                                available for biomedical research, the absence of prior psy-
                                                                                chosocial measures precludes direct comparisons between
                                                                                molecular, cellular, psychological, and social indications of
                                                                                resilience. In this regard, nonhuman primates offer attrac-
                                                                                tive opportunities to provide cross-disciplinary explana-
                                                                                tions for the development of stress-inoculation induced
                                                                                resilience. A better understanding of stress inoculation
                                                                                through repeated exposure to stressors is important because
                                                                                of concerns that reexperiencing stressors may impede re-
                                                                                covery instead of fostering the development of resilience
                                                                                (Bisson, Jenkins, Alexander, & Bannister, 1997; Foa,
                                                                                Zoellner, Feeny, Hembree, & Alvarez-Conrad, 2002;
Figure 5. Neuroendocrine indications of resilience are en-                      Hembree et al., 2003; Mayou, Ehlers, & Hobbs, 2000).
hanced by stress inoculation. Postrestraint stress levels of
ACTH at successive 30-minute intervals after pretreatment
with (A) saline or (C) hydrocortisone, and the corresponding
time-integrated levels (B and D), are presented for 6–7 adult
male monkeys from each postnatal foraging demand condi-                         REFERENCES
tion (mean + SEM). Significant postnatal foraging demand
treatment main effects ( p < .05) were discerned for postre-                    Agid, O., Shapira, B., Zislin, J., Ritsner, M., Hanin, B., Murad, H.,
straint measures of ACTH after pretreatment with saline, but                       et al. (1999). Environment and vulnerability to major psychiatric
not hydrocortisone. From “Maternal Mediation, Stress In-                           illness: A case control study of early parental loss in major depres-
oculation, and the Development of Neuroendocrine Stress                            sion, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry,
Resistance in Primates,” by K. J. Parker, C. L. Buckmaster,                        4, 163–172.
K. Sundlass, A. F. Schatzberg, and D. M. Lyons, 2006, Pro-                      Andrews, M. W., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1993). Assessment of at-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103,                            tachment in differentially reared infant monkeys (Macaca radi-
3000–3005. Copyright 2006 by the National Academy of                              ata): Response to separation and a novel environment. Journal of
Sciences USA. Adapted with permission.                                            Comparative Psychology, 107, 84–90.

                                                                                Bisson, J. I., Jenkins, P. L., Alexander, J., & Bannister, C. (1997).
                                                                                   Randomised controlled trial of psychological debriefing for vic-
                                                                                   tims of acute burn trauma. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171,
                                                                                   78–81.
aspects of emotion regulation, cognitive control, curios-
ity, and diminished HPA axis activation induced by ex-                          Boinski, S., & Fragaszy, D. M. (1989). The ontogeny of foraging in
                                                                                   squirrel monkeys, Saimiri oerstedi. Animal Behaviour, 37, 415–
posure to stress. Based on these findings, primate research
                                                                                   428.
is now well positioned to bridge the gap between psy-
                                                                                Coe, C. L., Glass, J. C., Wiener, S. G., & Levine, S. (1983). Behav-
chosocial studies of human resilience reviewed elsewhere
                                                                                  ioral, but not physiological, adaptation to repeated separation
(Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Masten, 2001; Yates et                                 in mother and infant primates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 8,
al., 2003) and rodent research focused on cellular and                            401–409.

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Stress Inoculation in Monkeys                                                               431

Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the              Forest, K. B. (1991). The interplay of childhood stress and adult
  buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.                      life events on women’s symptoms of depression. Dissertation
                                                                                  Abstracts International, 51, 3237.
Coplan, J. D., Smith, E. L., Altemus, M., Mathew, S. J., Perera, T.,
  Kral, J. G., et al. (2006). Maternal–infant response to variable             Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, E. A. (1999). Right hemispheric
  foraging demand in nonhuman primates: Effects of timing of                      dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional
  stressor on cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin-releasing factor and               MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  circulating glucocorticoid concentrations. Annals of the New                    USA, 96, 8301–8306.
  York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 525–533.
                                                                               Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children’s adaptation to negative
Davidson, J. R., Stein, D. J., Shalev, A. Y., & Yehuda, R. (2004). Post-          life events and stressed environments. Pediatric Annals, 20, 463–
  traumatic stress disorder: Acquisition, recognition, course, and                466.
  treatment. Journal Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience,
                                                                               Glavin, G. B., Pare, W. P., Sandbak, T., Bakke, H. K., & Murison,
  16, 135–147.
                                                                                  R. (1994). Restraint stress in biomedical research: An update.
Denenberg, V. H. (1999). Commentary: Is maternal stimulation                      Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 18, 223–249.
  the mediator of the handling effect in infancy? Developmental
                                                                               Harlow, H. F., & Suomi, S. J. (1974). Induced depression in mon-
  Psychobiology, 34, 1–3.
                                                                                 keys. Behavioral Biology, 12, 273–296.
Dettling, A. C., Feldon, J., & Pryce, C. R. (2002). Repeated parental
                                                                               Heim, C., Plotsky, P. M., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2004). Importance
   deprivation in the infant common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus,
                                                                                  of studying the contributions of early adverse experience to neu-
   primates) and analysis of its effects on early development. Bio-
                                                                                  robiological findings in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology,
   logical Psychiatry, 52, 1037–1046.
                                                                                  29, 641–648.
Diamond, A. (1990). Developmental time course in human infants
                                                                               Hembree, E. A., Foa, E. B., Dorfan, N. M., Street, G. P., Kowalski,
   and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of, inhibitory control
                                                                                 J., & Tu, X. (2003). Do patients drop out prematurely from
   of reaching. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608,
                                                                                 exposure therapy for PTSD? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16,
   637–676.
                                                                                 555–562.
Diamond, A., Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Success-
                                                                               Hennessy, M. B. (1986). Multiple, brief maternal separations in the
   ful performance by monkeys with lesions of the hippocampal
                                                                                 squirrel monkey: Changes in hormonal and behavioral respon-
   formation on AB and object retrieval, two tasks that mark devel-
                                                                                 siveness. Physiology and Behavior, 36, 245–250.
   opmental changes in human infants. Behavioral Neuroscience,
   103, 526–537.                                                               Hinde, R. A., & Spencer-Booth, Y. (1971). Effects of brief sep-
                                                                                  aration from mother on rhesus monkeys. Science, 173, 111–
Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Primate ana-
                                                                                  118.
   logue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Effects of excitotoxic
   lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. Behavioral Neu-           Hofer, M. A. (1996). Multiple regulators of ultrasonic vocalization
   roscience, 110, 872–886.                                                      in the infant rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21, 203–217.
Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: A                Jordan, T. C., Hennessy, M. B., Gonzalez, C. A., & Levine, S.
   framework for understanding healthy development in the face of                 (1985). Social and environmental factors influencing mother–
   risk. Annual Reviews of Public Health, 26, 399–419.                            infant separation–reunion in squirrel monkeys. Physiology and
                                                                                  Behavior, 34, 489–493.
Fernandez-Teruel, A., Gimenez-Llort, L., Escorihuela, R. M., Gil,
   L., Aguilar, R., Steimer, T., et al. (2002). Early-life handling            Khoshaba, D. M., & Maddi, S. R. (1999). Early experiences in
   stimulation and environmental enrichment. Are some of their                   hardiness development. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice
   effects mediated by similar neural mechanisms? Pharmacology,                  and Research, 51, 106–116.
   Biochemistry and Behavior, 73, 233–245.
                                                                               Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Sekihara, K., & Miyashita,
Foa, E. B., Stein, D. J., & McFarlane, A. C. (2006). Symptomatology              Y. (1998). No-go dominant brain activity in human infe-
   and psychopathology of mental health problems after disaster.                 rior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic reso-
   Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(Suppl. 2), 15–25.                          nance imaging. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 1209–
                                                                                 1213.
Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L. A., Feeny, N. C., Hembree, E. A., & Alvarez-
   Conrad, J. (2002). Does imaginal exposure exacerbate PTSD                   Lee, M. H., & Williams, D. I. (1974). Changes in licking behaviour
   symptoms? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70,                   of rat mother following handling of young. Animal Behaviour,
   1022–1028.                                                                     22, 679–681.

                             Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
432                                                                Lyons and Parker

Levine, S. (2000). Influence of psychological variables on the activity          Mason, W. A., & Capitanio, J. P. (1988). Formation and expression
   of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. European Journal of                of filial attachment in rhesus monkeys raised with living and
   Pharmacology, 405, 149–160.                                                    inanimate mother substitutes. Developmental Psychobiology, 21,
                                                                                  401–430.
Levine, S., & Mody, T. (2003). The long-term psychobiological
   consequences of intermittent postnatal separation in the squirrel            Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic. Resilience processes in de-
   monkey. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 83–89.                     velopment. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238.
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D.,                   Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A., & Hobbs, M. (2000). Psychological de-
   Freedman, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glu-                   briefing for road traffic accident victims. Three-year follow-up of
   cocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal re-                  a randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176,
   sponses to stress. Science, 277, 1659–1662.                                    589–593.
Lyons, D. M. (1993). Conflict as a constructive force in social life.            Meaney, M. J., Diorio, J., Francis, D., Widdowson, J., LaPlante, P.,
   In W. A. Mason & S. P. Mendoza (Eds.), Primate social conflict                  Caldji, C., et al. (1996). Early environmental regulation of fore-
   (pp. 387–408). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.                 brain glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: Implications for
                                                                                  adrenocortical responses to stress. Developmental Neuroscience,
Lyons, D. M., Afarian, H., Schatzberg, A. F., Sawyer-Glover, A.,
                                                                                  18, 49–72.
   & Moseley, M. E. (2002). Experience-dependent asymmetric
   variation in primate prefrontal morphology. Behavioural Brain                Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2005). Environmental programming of
   Research, 136, 51–59.                                                          stress responses through DNA methylation: Life at the interface
                                                                                  between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues
Lyons, D. M., & Banks, E. M. (1982). Ultrasounds in neonatal
                                                                                  in Clinical Neuroscience, 7, 103–123.
   rats: Novel, predator and conspecific odor cues. Developmental
   Psychobiology, 15, 455–460.                                                  Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of
                                                                                   prefrontal cortex function. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 24,
Lyons, D. M., Kim, S., Schatzberg, A. F., & Levine, S. (1998).
                                                                                   167–202.
   Postnatal foraging demands alter adrenocortical activity and psy-
   chosocial development. Developmental Psychobiology, 32, 285–                 Mortimer, J. T., & Staff, J. (2004). Early work as a source of de-
   291.                                                                           velopmental discontinuity during the transition to adulthood.
                                                                                  Development and Psychopathology, 16, 1047–1070.
Lyons, D. M., Lopez, J. M., Yang, C., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2000).
   Stress-level cortisol treatment impairs inhibitory control of be-            Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of
   havior in monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 7816–7821.                     emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249.
Lyons, D. M., Martel, F. L., Levine, S., Risch, N. J., & Schatzberg,            Ongur, D., & Price, J. L. (2000). The organization of networks
   A. F. (1999). Postnatal experiences and genetic effects on squirrel            within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys
   monkey social affinities and emotional distress. Hormones and                   and humans. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 206–219.
   Behavior, 36, 266–275.
                                                                                Parker, K. J., Buckmaster, C. L., Justus, K. R., Schatzberg, A. F., &
Lyons, D. M., Yang, C., Eliez, S., Reiss, A. L., & Schatzberg, A. F.               Lyons, D. M. (2005). Mild early life stress enhances prefrontal-
   (2004). Cognitive correlates of white matter growth and stress                  dependent response inhibition in monkeys. Biological Psychiatry,
   hormones in female squirrel monkey adults. Journal of Neuro-                    57, 848–855.
   science, 24, 3655–3662.
                                                                                Parker, K. J., Buckmaster, C. L., Schatzberg, A. F., & Lyons, D. M.
Lyons, D. M., Yang, C., Sawyer-Glover, A. M., Moseley, M. E., &                    (2004). Prospective investigation of stress inoculation in young
   Schatzberg, A. F. (2001). Early life stress and inherited variation             monkeys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 933–941.
   in monkey hippocampal volumes. Archives of General Psychiatry,
                                                                                Parker, K. J., Buckmaster, C. L., Sundlass, K., Schatzberg, A. F., &
   58, 1145–1151.
                                                                                   Lyons, D. M. (2006). Maternal mediation, stress inoculation, and
Maier, S. F., Amat, J., Baratta, M. V., Paul, E., & Watkins, L. R.                 the development of neuroendocrine stress resistance in primates.
  (2006). Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and                    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103,
  resilience. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 8, 397–406.                      3000–3005.
Mason, W. A. (1971). Motivational factors in psychosocial develop-              Parker, K. J., Rainwater, K. L., Buckmaster, C. L., Schatzberg, A.
  ment. In W. J. Arnold & M. M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska symposium                    F., Lindley, S. E., & Lyons, D. M. (2007). Early life stress and
  on motivation (pp. 35–67). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska                   novelty seeking behavior in adolescent monkeys. Psychoneuroen-
  Press.                                                                           docrinology, in press.

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Stress Inoculation in Monkeys                                                                433

Preuss, T. M. (1995). Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The Rose–              Rutter, M. (1993). Resilience: Some conceptual considerations. Jour-
   Woolsey–Akert program reconsidered. Journal of Cognitive Neu-                nal of Adolescent Health, 14, 626–631, 690–626.
   roscience, 7, 1–24.
                                                                             Sackett, G. P. (1972). Exploratory behavior of rhesus monkeys as a
Pryce, C. R., Dettling, A., Spengler, M., Spaete, C., & Feldon, J.              function of rearing conditions and sex. Developmental Psychol-
   (2004). Evidence for altered monoamine activity and emotional                ogy, 6, 260–270.
   and cognitive disturbance in marmoset monkeys exposed to early
   life stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032,            Smotherman, W. P., & Bell, R. W. (1980). Maternal mediation
   245–249.                                                                    of early experiences. In R. W. Bell & W. P. Smotherman (Eds.),
                                                                               Maternal influences and early behavior (pp. 201–210). New York:
Reio, T. G., Jr., Petrosko, J. M., Wiswell, A. K., & Thongsukmag,              Spectrum Publications.
   J. (2006). The measurement and conceptualization of curiosity.
   Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 117–135.                              Stanton, M. E., & Levine, S. (1985). Brief separation elevates cortisol
                                                                                in mother and infant squirrel monkeys. Physiology and Behavior,
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis,               34, 1007–1008.
   S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive
   control. Science, 306, 443–447.                                           Taylor, S. E., Eisenberger, N. I., Saxbe, D., Lehman, B. J., &
                                                                                Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Neural responses to emotional stimuli
Roder, E. L., Timmermans, P. J., & Vossen, J. M. (1989). Effects                are associated with childhood family stress. Biological Psychiatry,
  of rearing and exposure condition upon the acquisition of pho-                60, 296–301.
  bic behaviour in cynomolgus monkeys. Behaviour Research and
  Therapy, 27, 221–231.                                                      Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H.,
                                                                                Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and
Rosenberg, K. M., Denenberg, V. H., & Zarrow, M. X. (1970).
                                                                                ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during reg-
   Mice (Mus musculus) reared with rat aunts: The role of rat-mouse
                                                                                ulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cor-
   contact in mediating behavioural and physiological changes in
                                                                                tisol secretion among older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
   the mouse. Animal Behaviour, 18, 138–143.
                                                                                4415–4425.
Rosenblum, L. A. (1968). Mother–infant relations and early behav-
                                                                             Wallis, J. D., Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (2001).
   ioral development in the squirrel monkey. In L. A. Rosenblum
                                                                               Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and lateral pre-
   & R. W. Cooper (Eds.), The squirrel monkey (pp. 207–233).
                                                                               frontal cortex of the marmoset to performance on a detour
   Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
                                                                               reaching task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1797–
Rosenblum, L. A., & Andrews, M. W. (1994). Influences of environ-               1808.
   mental demand on maternal behavior and infant development.
                                                                             Yates, T. M., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, A. (2003). Rethinking re-
   Acta Paediatrica, 397(Suppl.), 57–63.
                                                                                silience: A developmental process perspective. In S. S. Luthar
Rosenblum, L. A., & Paully, G. S. (1984). The effects of varying                (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability. Adaptation in the context
   environmental demands on maternal and infant behavior. Child                 of childhood adversities (pp. 243–266). New York: Cambridge
   Development, 55, 305–314.                                                    University Press.

                           Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
You can also read