Impact of Free/Subsidized Secondary School Education on the Likelihood of Teenage Motherhood

Page created by Hugh Williams
 
CONTINUE READING
Demography (2021) 58(4):1401–1421                                                        Published online: 5 July 2021
                DOI 10.1215/00703370-9357498 © 2021 The Author
                This is an open access arti­cle dis­trib­uted under the terms of a Creative Commons license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).

                Impact of Free/Subsidized Secondary School Education
                on the Likelihood of Teenage Motherhood
                Steve Muchiri

                ABSTRACT Several countries in sub-Saharan Africa, includ­ing Kenya, have intro­duced
                free/sub­si­dized sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion. This paper exam­ines the role of these free/sub­
                si­dized edu­ca­tion pol­i­cies on teen­age moth­er­hood. To iden­tify the causal effect, I
                exploit the tim­ing of a national reform in Kenya that elim­in­ ated/sub­si­dized sec­ond­ary
                school fees using a dif­fer­ence-in-dif­fer­ence esti­ma­tion design. Using the 2014 Kenya
                Demographic and Health Survey (DHS), I esti­mate that the like­li­hood of teen­age moth­
                er­hood decreased by approx­im     ­ a­tely 5 per­cent­age points after the pol­icy’s implemen-
                tation. This study reit­er­ates that the teen­age period is cru­cial in terms of devel­op­ing
                human cap­i­tal through for­mal school­ing. In most devel­op­ing countries, par­ents often
                deter­mine and fund human cap­i­tal, which makes house­hold wealth/income a crit­i­cal
                fac­tor in human cap­i­tal accu­mu­la­tion and its inter­gen­er­a­tional pro­cess. I also high­light
                pos­i­tive exter­nal­i­ties from edu­ca­tional-cen­tered pol­i­cies, such as long-term eco­nomic
                growth, pov­erty reduc­tion, and reduc­tion of social wel­fare depen­dency.

                KEYWORDS Teenage moth­ er­
                                         hood • Sub-Saharan Africa • Education • Poverty •
                Educational pol­i­cies

                Introduction
                In 2013, the United Nations Population Fund (UNFPA 2013b; for­merly the United
                Nations Fund for Population Activities) noted that teen­age preg­nancy1 remains a
                global chal­lenge that requires urgent resolve. Demonstrating the grav­ity of teenage
                preg­nancy, the UNFPA (2015) reported that approx­i­ma­tely 16 mil­lion girls aged 15–
                19 become preg­nant world­wide. Estimates for 2010 show that about 95% of ado­les­
                cent births world­wide occur in devel­op­ing countries, with about 36 mil­lion women
                aged 20–24 reporting hav­ing given birth before their 18th birth­day and about 5.6
                mil­lion reporting a birth before their 15th birth­day (UNFPA 2013a, 2013b). In 2013,
                the highest inci­dence of teen­age preg­nan­cies occurred in sub-Saharan Africa. In this
                region, teen­age births accounted for more than one-half of all­births, an esti­mated 101

                1
                  Teenage preg­nancy is defined as preg­nan­cies among girls aged 10–19. Developed countries often use
                age to define teen­age preg­nancy. Although devel­op­ing countries also use age to define teen­age preg­nancy,
                they con­sider teen­age preg­nancy to be an issue only when it involves unmar­ried girls (Cherry et al. 2009).
                In this paper, I use the term teen­age moth­er­hood, defined as teen­age child­bear­ing regard­less of mar­i­tal sta­
                tus. Additionally, I pre­fer the term moth­er­hood as opposed to preg­nancy because the data do not pro­vide
                infor­ma­tion on abor­tions, those cur­rently preg­nant, or mis­car­riages.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1402                                                                                  S. Muchiri

                 births per 1,000 women, which was almost dou­ble the global aver­age (Odimegwu
                 and Mkwananzi 2016). Additionally, among the 15 countries world­wide where more
                 than 30% of 20- to 24-year-olds gave birth before age 18, 14 are in the sub-Saharan
                 region, includ­ing Niger, Uganda, and Malawi. Although Kenya is not among this
                 group, it is in the top 10 in terms of num­bers of women aged 20–24 who gave birth
                 by their 18th birth­day (see Loaiza and Liang 2013).
                     Ramifications of teen­age child­bear­ing are enor­mous, one of the fore­most being
                 its high risk of adverse health out­comes for the mother and the child (Kennedy et al.
                 2011; Magadi 2006). For instance, whereas these births account for approx­i­ma­tely
                 11% of births world­wide, they con­trib­ute approx­im    ­ a­tely 25% of all­the preg­nancy- or
                 child­birth-related mor­tal­ity, with the mor­tal­ity rate of 10- to 14-year-olds esti­mated
                 to be five­fold that of adult women (Kennedy et al. 2011). Among 15- to 19-year-olds,
                 child­birth com­pli­ca­tions are the lead­ing cause of death (Godding 2008). Infants face
                 sim­i­lar adverse out­comes, such as pre­term deliv­ery, low birth weight, greater than a
                 50% increased risk of dying within the first month of life, and higher rates of peri­na­
                 tal mor­bid­ity com­pared with those born to adult women (Kennedy et al. 2011; Patton
                 et al. 2009; Shah and Åhman 2012).
                     Most teen­age moth­ers drop out of school and may not return; they are vul­ner­a­
                 ble to a vari­ety of unfa­vor­able out­comes, such as high rates of unem­ploy­ment, low
                 labor mar­ket earn­ings, lower pros­pects of mar­riage, and high wel­fare depen­dency
                 and pov­erty rates (Godding 2008; Hao and Cherlin 2004; Micklewright and Stewart
                 1999). Consequently, these teen­age moth­ers are unpre­pared for the psy­cho­log­i­cal,
                 emo­tional, and finan­cial respon­si­bil­i­ties and chal­lenges of being a par­ent (Hoffman
                 and Maynard 2008), and they are more likely to per­pet­u­ate these unfa­vor­able out­
                 comes to mul­ti­ple gen­er­a­tions. The phe­nom­e­non is not lim­ited to devel­op­ing coun-
                 tries: devel­oped countries face sim­i­lar chal­lenges, with stud­ies show­ing that chil­dren
                 born to teen­age moth­ers are likely to have lower lev­els of edu­ca­tion and are later
                 likely to become teen­age par­ents them­selves (Bonell et al. 2006; Jaffee et al. 2001;
                 Moffitt and Team 2002; Pevalin 2003). However, the devel­oped countries often have
                 pro­grams in place to ease teen­age moth­ers’ finan­cial bur­dens or to pro­vide them with
                 ave­nues for self-reli­ance. Because many gov­ern­ments in devel­op­ing countries are
                 already bud­get-constrained, they may not be a­ ble to afford such pro­grams.
                     In devel­op­ing countries, the like­li­hood of teen­age moth­er­hood is influ­enced by
                 myr­iad issues, such as edu­ca­tion level, cul­ture (eth­nic­ity and reli­gion), socio­eco­
                 nomic sta­tus, and demo­graphic or sex­ual behav­ior fac­tors (e.g., early mar­riage, age
                 at sex­ual debut) (Pradhan et al. 2015). The United Nations Population Fund (UNFPA
                 2015) reported that girls in the poorest regions of the world are four times more likely
                 to give birth in ado­les­cence than girls in richer regions.
                     These obser­va­tions are par­tic­u­larly rel­e­vant in sub-Saharan Africa, a region with
                 the highest teen­age preg­nancy rates and a high pro­por­tion of girls with poor socio­
                 eco­nomic sta­tus (Odejimi and Young 2014). Therefore, iden­ti­fy­ing pro­tec­tive fac­tors
                 against teen­age moth­er­hood, espe­cially in sub-Saharan Africa, could inform pol­icy-
                 mak­ing and help abate the adverse health and eco­nomic out­comes of teen­age moth­er­
                 hood. Human cap­i­tal improve­ment is crit­i­cal, and reduc­ing bar­ri­ers to school­ing (e.g.,
                 cost), espe­cially for girls whose school enroll­ment rate drops dur­ing the teen­age years
                 (Lloyd et al. 2000), is espe­cially impor­tant (Duflo 2004; Duflo et al. 2015; Keats
                 2014; Odejimi and Young 2014; Osili and Long 2008).

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                                              1403

                    The con­cen­tra­tion of ado­les­cent girls aged 10–17 is projected to increase world­
                wide, with the most sub­stan­tial change expected to occur in sub-Saharan Africa,2
                where ado­les­cent preg­nancy is most com­mon, the use of con­tra­cep­tives is the low­
                est, and income lev­els are among the low­est in the world. In gen­eral, ages 6–18 are
                offi­cially pri­mary and sec­ond­ary school ages. Unfortunately, many girls are out of
                school, espe­cially in sub-Saharan Africa and other Asian regions, such as South and
                West Asia, where almost one-third of ado­les­cents of sec­ond­ary school age are out of
                school (Loaiza and Liang 2013).
                    Addressing ado­les­cents’ preg­nan­cies and vulnerabilities par­al­lel one of the United
                Nations Millennium Development Goals: to increase the level of for­mal edu­ca­tion for
                all­school-age chil­dren by 2015 and to pro­vide both boys and girls with equal access to
                edu­ca­tion at all­ lev­els.3 Addressing the ini­tia­tive of edu­ca­tion for all­, sev­eral countries
                in sub-Saharan Africa have taken steps to elim­i­nate or have already elim­i­nated pri­mary
                school fees in gov­ern­ment-aided (pub­lic) schools: Malawi (in 1994); Uganda (in 1997);
                Tanzania (in 2000); and Cameroon, Burundi, Ghana, Rwanda, and Kenya (in 2003)
                (Grogan 2008). Other countries—such as Uganda (in 2007), Rwanda (in 2007), and
                Tanzania (in 2016)—have expanded these efforts to the sec­ond­ary school level.
                    For many countries, an imme­di­ate out­come of this pol­icy has been a sig­nif­i­cant
                increase in school enroll­ment, per­haps an indi­ca­tion of its high impact or a grave state
                of many house­holds that were pre­vi­ously unable to edu­cate their chil­dren. As a point
                of ref­er­ence, the aver­age net enroll­ment ratio for pri­mary school edu­ca­tion in Africa
                increased from 56% in 1999 to 73% in 2007. Although the aver­age net enroll­ment
                ratio for sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion is lower, it showed a 9 per­cent­age point improve­ment
                (from 18% to 27%) in the same period (Ohba 2011). The rel­a­tively small response at
                the sec­ond­ary school level could be attrib­uted to two fac­tors. First, in Kenya, par­ents
                are cus­tom­ar­ily respon­si­ble for guid­ing and selecting a child’s pri­mary and some­times
                sec­ond­ary school level. Parents’ incen­tives, how­ever, may not be fully aligned with the
                chil­dren’s long-term poten­tial earn­ings (Baland and Robinson 2000), and par­ents may
                opt to enter the child into the labor mar­ket. Second, Jensen (2010) noted that mis­in­for­
                ma­tion on the finan­cial returns to fur­ther school­ing may lead indi­vid­u­als (in this case,
                par­ents) to underinvest in school­ing. This point is par­tic­u­larly impor­tant in the con­text
                of Kenya, where some com­mu­ni­ties may hold a neg­a­tive per­cep­tion of for­mal edu­ca­
                tion, espe­cially in areas with high unem­ploy­ment rates among high school grad­u­ates.
                    Despite evi­dence of increas­ing school enroll­ment resulting from free pri­mary or
                sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion, the full impact of free edu­ca­tion is sub­ject to research, and results
                from some research are still contested. For exam­ple, some stud­ies have found that free
                pri­mary edu­ca­tion (FPE) increased pub­lic pri­mary school enroll­ment and did not com­
                pro­mise the qual­ity of edu­ca­tion (Lucas and Mbiti 2012a). Some stud­ies have found
                that although FPE improved both boys’ and girls’ pri­mary school com­ple­tion rates, it
                had a more sub­stan­tial impact on boys, thereby increas­ing the gen­der gap in grad­u­a­tion
                (Lucas and Mbiti 2012b). Other stud­ies have not found evi­dence of a net change in
                pub­lic school enroll­ment (Bold et al. 2011), per­haps because of a sub­stan­tial increase in

                2
                  It is projected that by 2030, approx­i­ma­tely 1 in every 4 ado­les­cent girls will reside in sub-Saharan Africa.
                Countries with the highest projected increases are Nigeria (9.2 mil­lion), United Republic of Tanzania (3.7
                mil­lion), Democratic Republic of the Congo (3.3 mil­lion), Uganda (2.5 mil­lion), and Kenya (2.3 mil­lion).
                3
                  See https:​­/​­/www​­.un​­.org​­/sustainabledevelopment​­/education​­/.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1404                                                                                                    S. Muchiri

                 pri­vate school enroll­ment, par­tic­ul­ arly in urban areas (Dixon and Tooley 2012). Aiming
                 to add to this lit­er­a­ture, I exam­ine the impact of a nation­wide elim­in­ a­tion or sub­si­diz­ing
                 of pub­lic sec­ond­ary school fees on the like­li­hood of teen­age moth­er­hood.
                     I take advan­tage of the national free sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion (FSE) pro­gram in Kenya
                 and use a dif­fer­ence-in-dif­fer­ence frame­work to ana­lyze the ques­tion. Although the
                 mech­a­nism through which the FSE pro­gram acted is not straight­for­ward, I find evi­
                 dence of a reduced like­li­hood of teen­age moth­er­hood. Although these results are
                 irrec­on­cil­able with Filmer and Schady’s (2014) find­ings for Cambodia, for exam­ple,
                 they com­ple­ment those of Baird et al. (2010) and Ferré (2009) show­ing that improv­
                 ing sec­ond­ary school atten­dance in East Africa had a sig­nif­i­cant impact on teen­age
                 moth­er­hood.

                 Education System and Free Secondary Schooling
                 Before 1985, the Kenyan edu­ca­tion sys­tem required seven years of pri­mary edu­
                 ca­tion, four years of sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion, two years of high school edu­ca­tion, and
                 a min­i­mum of three years in the uni­ver­sity. To cre­ate a more prac­ti­cal cur­ric­ul­um
                 (voca­tion­ally ori­ented cur­ric­u­lum), the Kenyan gov­ern­ment overhauled the edu­ca­tion
                 sys­tem in 1985 to require eight years of pri­mary edu­ca­tion, four years of sec­ond­ary
                 edu­ca­tion, and a min­i­mum of four years in the uni­ver­sity (Amutabi 2003).4 Students
                 are required to take the Kenya Certificate of Primary Education (KCPE) exam to com­
                 plete pri­mary edu­ca­tion and the Kenya Certificate of Secondary Education (KCSE)
                 exam to com­plete sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion. Both exams are nation­ally stan­dard­ized and
                 are used for high school and uni­ver­sity admis­sions, respec­tively. Because stu­dents are
                 required to reg­is­ter for these exams in advance, school enroll­ment is essen­tial for test
                 tak­ing. Historically, how­ever, many schools denied stu­dents with out­stand­ing school
                 fees the oppor­tu­nity to reg­is­ter for these exams, put­ting addi­tional finan­cial pres­sure
                 on poor house­holds to pay the out­stand­ing fees or for­feit the stu­dents’ oppor­tu­nity to
                 take the national exam.5

                 Attendance Patterns and the Reform

                 Kenya has his­tor­i­cally had fewer high schools than pri­mary schools. When it gained
                 its inde­pen­dence in 1963, Kenya had approx­im­ a­tely 40 times more pri­mary schools
                 than sec­ond­ary schools (Ohba 2011). This gap, how­ever, shrunk to almost 4 times
                 by 2011 (Glennerster et al. 2011) mostly due to com­mu­nity-built sec­ond­ary schools
                 (Ohba 2011). Because the estab­lish­ment of these com­mu­nity-built schools depended

                 4
                   In Kenya, sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion is a sin­gle stage in the edu­ca­tion sys­tem, fol­low­ing pri­mary school edu­
                 ca­tion but pre­ced­ing the ter­tiary level. In other countries, edu­ca­tion may be divided into lower/junior and
                 upper/senior lev­els. This study exam­ines a sin­gle stage of sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion designed for stu­dents to
                 begin pri­mary school at age 6 or 7, although some stu­dents delay their entry into for­mal school­ing or with­
                 draw and reenroll when they are older (Kramon and Posner 2016).
                 5
                   Registration for the exams, taken in Octo­ber and Novem­ber, occurs in March. Those fail­ing to reg­is­ter in
                 time are often denied the oppor­tu­nity to take the exams and must wait a year for a sec­ond chance.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                                1405

                on a com­mu­nity’s eco­nomic devel­op­ment and strength, some areas were unable to
                estab­lish their own.
                    In addi­tion, although the num­ber of sec­ond­ary schools in Kenya has increased, sig­
                nif­i­cant bot­tle­necks in the edu­ca­tion sys­tem remain. For instance, dis­tance to the near-
                est sec­ond­ary school is still an imped­i­ment, more so in some rural areas than in oth­ers.
                In some areas, less than 50% of indi­vid­u­als are within walk­ing dis­tance of a sec­ond­ary
                school (Glennerster et al. 2011). According to Alderman and King (1998), walk­ing dis­
                tance to school has a more sig­nif­i­cant influ­ence on females’ school­ing because of safety
                con­cerns. Other imped­i­ments to Kenya’s edu­ca­tion sys­tem include low tran­si­tion rates
                to sec­ond­ary school (Ohba 2009) and high drop­out rates in pri­mary schools (Somerset
                2007). The cost of edu­ca­tion remains an espe­cially sig­nif­i­cant bar­rier for many house­
                holds (Glennerster et al. 2011; Holla and Kremer 2009; Keats 2014).
                    Kenya intro­duced a free sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion (FSE) pro­gram in Feb­ru­ary 2008. In
                his FSE intro­duc­tion speech, President Kibaki clearly artic­u­lated that “. . . ​the main
                objec­tive of pro­vid­ing free sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion is to ensure that chil­dren from poor
                house­holds acquire qual­ity edu­ca­tion that enables them access [to] oppor­tu­ni­ties for
                self-advance­ment and become pro­duc­tive mem­bers of soci­ety” (Ohba 2009:5). The
                FSE pro­gram cov­ered 10,265 KES (approx­im         ­ a­tely US$100) per pub­lic sec­ond­ary stu­
                dent per year in basic tuition expenses.6 Glennerster et al. (2011) esti­mated that house­
                hold spend­ing on sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion amounts to one-half of their expen­di­tures, and
                they spend approx­i­ma­tely 8 times more on edu­ca­tion for sec­ond­ary school stu­dents than
                for pri­mary school stu­dents: an aver­age of 25,000 vs. 3,000 KES per year. The gov­ern­
                ment grant cov­ered only approx­i­ma­tely one-half of the cost per stu­dent, mak­ing house­
                holds respon­si­ble for the costs of school uni­forms, boarding fees, and infra­struc­ture and
                therefore leav­ing sig­nif­i­cant finan­cial bur­den on many fam­il­ies for whom the cost of
                school fees remains a sig­nif­i­cant bar­rier to school­ing. Funds from the grant were dis­trib­
                uted from the cen­tral gov­ern­ment to indi­vid­ual schools based on the num­ber of stu­dents.
                    Although the FSE pro­gram was national, regional var­i­a­tion in pro­gram inten­sity
                is likely, per­haps stem­ming from het­ero­ge­ne­ity in pre-pro­gram tran­si­tion rates from
                pri­mary to sec­ond­ary school across the coun­try. Regions with low pre-FSE pro­gram
                tran­si­tion rates to sec­ond­ary school could have expe­ri­enced larger increases in sec­
                ond­ary school enroll­ment. For instance, North Eastern prov­ince may have expe­ri­enced
                higher pro­gram inten­sity than Nairobi and thus may have expe­ri­enced a larger decline
                in teen­age moth­er­hood. However, the data I use do not allow me to dis­en­tan­gle poten­
                tial var­i­ance in inten­sity lev­els by region, and the results I pres­ent are a coun­try aver­age.

                Possible Link Between Education and Pregnancy
                I first offer a sim­ple mech­a­nism on how a house­hold’s wealth can impact school­ing
                and teen­age moth­er­hood. Suppose a teen­age pop­u­la­tion is split between those who
                reside in poor house­holds, P, and those that reside in wealthy house­holds, W. Assume
                that teen­ager i is endowed with abil­ity αi and lives in a house­hold with assets Ait at
                time t. I assume that αi varies such that stu­dents from wealthy house­holds are more

                6
                    This pro­gram aid was not avail­­able to stu­dents attend­ing pri­vate schools.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1406                                                                                  S. Muchiri

                 likely to per­form well on the KCPE exam because they can pur­chase text­books or
                 pay for tutors or because they are less likely to miss meals. However, credit con­
                 straint does not affect the abil­ity to reach the required KCPE cut­off mark to prog­
                 ress to sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion: in the absence of credit con­straint, stu­dents from poor
                 and wealthy households would exhibit sim­il­ ar tran­si­tion rates to sec­ond­ary school. A
                 teen­ager’s abil­ity and the house­hold’s acces­si­ble assets deter­mine school­ing invest­
                 ment. An indi­vid­ual’s achiev­able school­ing level is S P *(α i , Ait ) for poor house­holds
                 and SW *(α i , Ait ) for wealthy house­holds, where SW * (αi, Ait) > S P* (αi, Ait). Wealthy
                 house­holds invest in higher lev­els of school­ing for their teen­ag­ers; teen­ag­ers in poor
                 house­holds remain trapped at low lev­els of school­ing and con­se­quently main­tain a
                 poor stan­dard of liv­ing and, in this case, I will show that teen­ag­ers in poor house­holds
                 face a greater like­li­hood of teen­age moth­er­hood.
                     A thresh­old level of assets, Â, exists such that SW*(αi, Ait) = SP*(αi, Ait.). House-
                 holds with an ini­tial asset endow­ment lower than  are bud­get constrained and can­not
                 invest in higher school­ing lev­els. Policies such as the FSE in Kenya could address
                 this need by ele­vat­ing poor house­holds beyond the thresh­old asset level. The FSE
                 would also likely affect tran­si­tion rates to sec­ond­ary school for teen­ag­ers in wealthy
                 house­holds, but it is more likely to have a sig­nif­i­cant impact for teen­ag­ers in poor
                 house­holds who achieved the required exam cut­off marks and would, in the absence
                 of the FSE, oth­er­wise not attend sec­ond­ary school because of credit con­straint.
                     In this con­cep­tual mech­a­nism, the removal of finan­cial bar­ri­ers poten­tially pro­vi­
                 des access to sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion, espe­cially for the poor, at an age when most girls
                 would ben­e­fit from for­mal edu­ca­tion. Secondary school enroll­ment pro­vi­des girls
                 with oppor­tu­ni­ties for growth beyond sim­ple domes­tic life, espe­cially in sub-Saharan
                 Africa, where early mar­riage and child­bear­ing are cus­tom­ary (UNFPA 2013b). Sec-
                 ondary school could pro­vide them with oppor­tu­ni­ties that may com­pete with cus­tom­
                 ary norms and lim­it­a­tions, such as early mar­riage and early child­bear­ing (Caldwell
                 et al. 1983; Westoff 1992). A Kenya Demographic and Health Survey conducted in
                 1998 found that women with at least a sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion marry later than those
                 with lower than secondary school education (22 vs. 17 years of age) and have fewer
                 chil­dren (National Council for Population and Development et al. 1999). In addi­tion,
                 stud­ies have shown that edu­ca­tion may change indi­vid­u­als’ pref­er­ences toward fewer
                 chil­dren with a bet­ter qual­ity of life (e.g., Grossman 2006). Other stud­ies have shown
                 that a super­vised envi­ron­ment, such as that pro­vided by schools, con­trib­utes to lower
                 preg­nancy rates. For instance, in a study in South Africa, Rosenberg et al. (2015)
                 found lower preg­nancy rates dur­ing the school term than dur­ing sum­mer hol­i­days.
                     Educated women are more likely to use con­tra­cep­tive meth­ods (Ferré 2009), and
                 sec­ond­ary school edu­ca­tion pro­vi­des an addi­tional level of edu­ca­tion. Contraceptives
                 are discussed in clas­ses at the teacher’s dis­cre­tion, in biol­ogy clas­ses, and in the cur­
                 ric­u­lum on the biol­ogy of AIDS and HIV trans­mis­sion and pre­ven­tion (Duflo et al.
                 2015). Contraceptive knowl­edge and access could lower the like­li­hood of both school
                 drop­out and early mar­riages that could have been an out­come of preg­nancy (Ferré
                 2009; LeVine et al. 1996).
                     Another pos­si­ble link between edu­ca­tion and preg­nancy is the increased sta­tus that
                 a sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion would con­fer. As Caldwell and Caldwell (1987) noted, edu­
                 cated women in sub-Saharan Africa, regard­less of mar­i­tal sta­tus, are more respected
                 and enjoy a higher sta­tus in soci­ety. This higher stand­ing based on edu­ca­tion diverges

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                           1407

                from ear­lier, tra­di­tional cul­tural trends in which mar­riage and child­bear­ing were
                highly regarded. Consequently, edu­ca­tion may be per­ceived as a chan­nel through
                which girls can ele­vate their sta­tus in soci­ety (Caldwell et al. 1983).
                    Education also influ­ences the tim­ing of repro­duc­tive events, such as delayed sex­
                ual ini­ti­a­tion. Pregnancy in sub-Saharan Africa is a fre­quent expla­na­tion for school
                drop­out, and although preg­nant girls are allowed to return to school after birth in
                some countries, the frac­tion actu­ally returning is often low (Lloyd and Mensch 2008).
                If hav­ing a child pre­cludes a girl from con­tinu­ing with school­ing, girls may delay sex­
                ual activ­it­ies to ensure they com­plete their school­ing.
                    Regardless of the mech­a­nism through which school­ing influ­ences preg­nancy, reduced
                rates of teen­age child­bear­ing could have long-term ben­efi ­ ts for the child, mother, and
                soci­ety at large.

                Estimation

                Isolating the Impact of the Reform

                Examining the impact of edu­ca­tion on teen­age moth­er­hood requires acknowl­edg­ment
                of cau­sal­ity issues—that is, deter­min­ing whether teen­age moth­er­hood reduces girls’
                for­mal edu­ca­tion attain­ment or that their low edu­ca­tion lev­els lead to teen­age mother­
                hood. I address this endogeneity issue by tak­ing advan­tage of a quasi-exper­i­ment
                set­ting (the pol­icy change). Although the pro­vi­sion of this free/sub­si­dized sec­ond­ary
                school edu­ca­tion has a soci­e­tal-level impact, it is more likely to have a direct and
                imme­di­ate effect on teen­age moth­er­hood. Isolating this effect pres­ents meth­od­o­log­i­
                cal chal­lenges. For exam­ple, period effects may be con­tem­po­ra­ne­ous with the intro­
                duc­tion of the FSE pro­gram. In this case, a com­par­is­ on group to the tar­get pop­u­la­tion
                is nec­es­sary, but then there may be per­sis­tent soci­e­tal fac­tors that could pro­duce dif­
                fer­ences between the tar­get group and a com­par­is­ on group.
                    To address these issues, I eval­u­ate first births for women aged 15–18 from two
                cohort groups: (1) the treated group, those impacted directly by the FSE (born in
                1990–1993); and (2) the con­       trol group, those not impacted by the FSE (born
                in 1985–1988). Both cohorts were sur­veyed in 2014, when indi­vid­u­als in the treated
                group were aged 21–24 (i.e., they were aged 15–18 at pol­icy implementation) and
                those in the con­trol group were aged 26–29 (i.e., they were aged 20–23 at pol­icy
                implementation). Figure 1 dis­plays a Lexis dia­gram of the anal­y­sis and helps cre­ate
                cross-sec­tions of the birth cohort in 2008 (when the pol­icy was implemented) and
                2014 (when the sur­vey was conducted). Given my focus here on teen­age moth­er­hood
                and the 2014 sur­vey’s ques­tion on respon­dent’s age at first birth, I con­sider only those
                women who were at least age 20 in 2014.
                    Another poten­tial issue is that some stu­dents will not have achieved a suf­fi­cient
                score on the KCPE exam to per­mit their tran­si­tion to sec­ond­ary school regard­less of
                the abil­ity to finance it. I expect this to affect the con­trol group and the treated group
                sim­i­larly; there­fore, although all­sec­ond­ary school stu­dents were eli­gi­ble for treat­
                ment after the FSE pol­icy, only those who suc­cess­fully com­pleted pri­mary school
                poten­tially received the treat­ment. Therefore, the meth­od­o­log­i­cal approach pro­vi­
                des esti­ma­tes akin to the aver­age treat­ment effect on the treated. The FSE expo­sure

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1408                                                                                            S. Muchiri

                 Fig. 1 Lexis diagram of birth cohorts and cohort age. The treated group is the 1990–1993 birth cohort, and
                 the control group is the 1985–1988 birth cohort. Age in 2008 (the FSE policy year) is plotted on the left
                 axis, and age in 2014 (when individuals were observed/interviewed) is indicated on the right axis.

                 period dif­fers among respon­dents. For exam­ple, women born in 1993 were exposed
                 lon­ger to the pol­icy than those born in 1990 (i.e., they were in their first year of sec­
                 ond­ary school at pol­icy implementation, whereas the lat­ter were in their final years of
                 sec­ond­ary school). I col­lapse the birth cohorts into two groups and con­sider the aver­
                 age expo­sure, enabling me to com­pare trends in first births between the two cohorts.
                    Human cap­i­tal invest­ment (i.e., for­mal edu­ca­tion) often takes place dur­ing the
                 teen­age years. However, as noted ear­lier, the high cost of edu­ca­tion in devel­op­ing
                 countries reg­u­larly hin­ders this pro­cess. In poor house­holds, many teen­ag­ers may
                 be forced to quit school, and girls’ edu­ca­tional oppor­tu­ni­ties may be sac­ri­ficed in
                 favor of pro­vid­ing such oppor­tu­ni­ties for boys. Therefore, chil­dren in poor house­
                 holds are less likely to attend school, and girls are often dis­ad­van­taged com­pared
                 with boys. I use Demographic and Health Survey (DHS) data on house­hold wealth
                 to cat­e­go­rize women in the sam­ple into those who are most likely to be impacted by
                 FSE (those from poor house­holds) and those who are less likely to be impacted by
                 FSE (those from wealthy house­holds). Using this infor­ma­tion and a dif­fer­ence-in-

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                                          1409

                dif­fer­ence method, I com­pare trends (1) between treated and con­trol groups and (2)
                between poor and wealthy house­holds. Ideally, this method con­trols for unob­served
                fac­tors between the cohorts, which could affect the dif­fer­ences between the treated
                and the con­trol cohorts. I attri­bute these dif­fer­ence-in-dif­fer­ence esti­ma­tes to the FSE
                pro­gram after con­trol­ling for observed indi­vid­ual and house­hold char­ac­ter­is­tics.7

                Data

                I use data from the 2014 Kenya Demographic and Health Survey (DHS) (Kenya
                National Bureau of Statistics [Nairobi] and ICF 2015). DHSs are national house­hold
                sam­ple sur­veys with ques­tions on health, socio­eco­nomic, and anthro­po­mor­phic indi­
                ca­tors. The sur­vey inter­views all­mem­bers of the selected house­hold, usu­ally men and
                women of repro­duc­tive age (15–49). The DHS uses a mul­ti­stage sam­pling design, first
                selecting pri­mary sam­pling units (clus­ters) and then ran­domly sam­pling listed house­
                holds within the ran­domly selected clus­ters (exclud­ing fam­i­lies liv­ing in insti­tu­tional
                facil­i­ties, such as boarding schools, hos­pi­tals, army bar­racks, or police camps).

                Descriptive Statistics and Underlying Patterns in the Data

                Summary sta­tis­tics of key var­ia­ bles are presented in Table 1. The table reveals some
                dif­fer­ences in sev­eral var­i­ables, such as age at first birth (although I con­sider only
                whether a birth occurred) and edu­ca­tion. Age at first birth is higher for the youn­
                ger cohort, suggesting that the young cohort is more likely to delay first birth than
                the older cohort. For the youn­ger cohort, edu­ca­tion level is approx­i­ma­tely one year
                lower, per­haps because some in this cohort are still enrolled in school. Overall, most
                sam­pled women iden­ti­fied as Protestant (as opposed to Cath­o­lic, Mus­lim, or hav­ing
                no reli­gion), reflecting the high con­cen­tra­tion of Protestants in the coun­try. Reflect-
                ing the coun­try’s major eth­nic groups, most of the sam­pled women also iden­ti­fied as
                Kalenjin, Kamba, Kikuyu, Luhya, Luo, and Kisii (as opposed to Maasai or Somali).
                On aver­age, the two cohorts have rel­a­tively sim­i­lar char­ac­ter­is­tics.
                    Evidence that sec­ond­ary school admis­sions increased with the intro­duc­tion of the
                FSE pro­gram can be found in Brudevold-Newman (2016: fig­ure 2). Building on this
                evi­dence, I pres­ent a visual rep­re­sen­ta­tion of trends in teen­age birth rates in Figure
                2. Approximately 35% to 40% of the sam­pled women were teen­age moth­ers, but the
                trend decreased over time such that youn­ger cohorts have lower teen­age birth rates.
                Importantly, teen­age birth rates are, on aver­age, higher for women born before 1990
                than for women born later.
                    Figure 3 plots teen­age birth rates by rural/urban sta­tus, reli­gion, and prov­ince. The
                fig­ure shows that rural women were 10 per­cent­age points more likely to be teenage
                moth­ers than urban women. This gap, how­ever, was larger for the older cohort than

                7
                  I am not aware of any other national pro­gram that was enacted in 2008 that would have affected these
                groups dif­fer­ently. However, if there were pub­lic health ini­tia­tives passed in 2008 or later, they would
                likely affect groups sim­i­larly, and they would be accounted for by the dif­fer­ence-in-dif­fer­ence approach
                used here.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1410                                                                                                S. Muchiri

                 Table 1 Sample char­ac­ter­is­tics

                                                        1985–1988 Cohort                   1990–1993 Cohort

                                                      Proportion/                       Proportion/
                                                         Mean              SD              Mean              SD     Difference

                 Teenage Birth Rate                      0.31             0.46             0.34              0.47    0.03**
                 Age at First Birth                     18.26             2.25            19.49              3.17    1.23**
                 Education (years)                       8.46             3.88             7.84              4.53   −0.62**
                 Urban                                   0.42             0.49             0.43              0.50    0.02
                 Religion
                   Cath­o­lic                             .20             0.40               .19             0.40     –.01
                   Protestant                             .64             0.48               .66             0.47      .02
                   Mus­lim                                .14             0.35               .13             0.33     –.01
                   None                                   .02             0.13               .02             0.14      .00
                 Ethnicitya
                   Kalenjin                               .13             0.34               .15             0.36      .02*
                   Kamba                                  .15             0.36               .15             0.35     –.00
                   Kikuyu                                 .09             0.28               .10             0.29      .01
                   Kisii                                  .06             0.24               .06             0.24     –.00
                   Luhya                                  .13             0.34               .11             0.32     –.02*
                   Luo                                    .11             0.32               .11             0.31     –.00
                 Provinces
                   Nairobi                                .04             0.20               .04             0.21      .00
                   Central                                .08             0.27               .09             0.29      .01†
                   Coast                                  .13             0.34               .12             0.32     –.01†
                   Eastern                                .15             0.36               .17             0.37      .02†
                   Nyanza                                 .14             0.34               .13             0.34     –.01
                   Rift Valley                            .32             0.46               .31             0.46     –.00
                   Western                                .09             0.29               .08             0.27     –.01†
                   North Eastern                          .05             0.22               .05             0.22      .00
                 Number of Observations                         5,366                                5,500

                 a
                     Less than 5% of respon­dents iden­ti­fied as Maasai, Somali, Taita Taveta, or Turkana.
                 †
                     p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01

                 for the youn­ger cohort. The fig­ure is encour­ag­ing: it shows a decline in the over­
                 all rate of teen­age moth­er­hood and helps to deci­pher which groups are driv­ing the
                 results. Women who iden­tify as Mus­lim were more likely to be teen­age moth­ers than
                 were those iden­ti­fy­ing as Cath­o­lic or Protestant. This dif­fer­ence is larger in the pre-
                 FSE period (1985–1988), with Mus­lim women approx­i­ma­tely 10 per­cent­age points
                 more likely to be teenage moth­ers. Still, this gap declined remark­ably for the youn­ger
                 cohort. At the tail end of the period cov­ered, Mus­lim women were less likely to be
                 teenage moth­ers than were Protestant women.
                     Trends in teen­age moth­er­hood between prov­inces could be related to cul­tural
                 dif­fer­ences between eth­nic groups, which often share sim­i­lar migra­tion pat­terns.
                 Although eth­nic groups are not as con­cen­trated within par­tic­u­lar prov­inces as they
                 used to be, a high degree of this homo­ge­ne­ity remains, espe­cially in the North East-
                 ern, Nyanza, and Central prov­inces. Figure 3 shows that, on aver­age, Nyanza had the
                 highest rate of teen­age moth­er­hood (between 42% and 52%), and these high lev­els
                 persisted over time; by con­trast, Central and Nairobi prov­inces had the low­est rates

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                                    1411

                Fig. 2 Teenage birth rates by year of birth. Data are from the 2014 Kenya Demographic and Health Survey.

                of teen­age moth­er­hood. The observed sig­nif­i­cant drop for the North Eastern prov­ince
                and a cor­re­spond­ing increase in Nairobi around 1989 may be the result of an influx
                of Somali peo­ple, who are con­cen­trated in the North Eastern prov­ince (bor­der­ing
                Somalia), into some Nairobi sub­urbs.
                    Figure 4 pres­ents trends by house­hold wealth sta­tus (low, mid­dle, and high). DHS
                data divide wealth index into five categories, with higher val­ues indi­cat­ing wealth­ier
                house­holds. I define house­holds with a wealth index of 0–2 as low income, house­holds
                with a wealth index of 3 as mid­dle income, and house­holds with a wealth index of 4 as
                high income. Figure 4 shows that women in high-income house­holds were less likely
                to be teen­age moth­ers than women from low-income house­holds. For the con­trol group
                (the 1985–1988 birth cohort), the trend for low-income house­holds is rel­a­tively sta­ble,
                but the trend for high-income house­holds declines around the 1987 birth cohort. I do not
                expect this dif­fer­ence to impact the results; if it does, then it will bias the results down­
                ward, mak­ing them under­stated. For the treated group (the 1990–1993 birth cohort),
                low-income and high-income house­holds seem to have a sim­i­lar trend. Trends for those
                in high-income house­holds are rel­a­tively sim­i­lar before (1985–1988) and after (1990–
                1993) the reform.
                    Overall, rates of teen­age moth­er­hood vary across char­ac­ter­is­tics, such as place of
                res­i­dence or reli­gion. What is com­mon, how­ever, is that teen­age girls from low-income
                house­holds or who live in rural areas are more likely to be teen­age moth­ers than are
                their urban or high-income coun­ter­parts, respec­tively.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1412                                                                                       S. Muchiri

         Fig. 3 Trend in teenage birth rates by urban/rural residence, religion, and province. Data are from the 2014 Kenya
         Demographic and Health Survey.

                 Empirical Strategy

                 Because the pol­icy reform has a direct impact on a spe­cific age cohort, I iso­late its
                 effect by com­par­ing birth rate changes of women affected by the pol­icy (i.e., youn­ger
                 cohort) with those of women not affected by it (i.e., older cohort) and by com­par­ing
                 low-income with high-income house­holds. Subscripts L, H, O, and Y denote women
                 from low-income house­holds, high-income house­holds, the older age cohort, and the
                 youn­ger age cohort, respec­tively. Equation (1) gives the change in first-birth rates,
                 Ratet, for the treated (the youn­ger cohort) between those from low-income house­
                 holds and those from high-income house­holds, and Eq. (2) gives the change in first-
                 birth rates, Ratec, for the con­trol (the older cohort) between those from low-income
                 house­holds and those from high-income house­holds.

                                                        ∆ Ratet = Rate(Y ,L) − Rate(Y ,H ).                       (1)
                                                        ∆ Ratec = Rate(O,L) − Rate(O,H ) .                        (2)

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                              1413

                Fig. 4 Teenage birth rates by household wealth index. Data are from the 2014 Kenya Demographic and
                Health Survey.

                The dif­fer­ence between the two then pro­vi­des the esti­mated impact of free/sub­si­dized
                sec­ond­ary school edu­ca­tion on teen­age moth­er­hood. This is the dif­fer­ence-in-dif­fer­ence
                (DD) approach.

                                                           DD = ∆ Ratet − ∆ Ratec .                           (3)

                   Table 2 pres­ents the esti­mated mean first-birth rates. Teens are defined as 15–18
                years old in panel A and as 15–19 years old in panel B. In each panel, the first col­umn
                con­tains mean first-birth rates for the older cohort (women not directly affected by
                the pol­icy), the sec­ond col­umn con­tains mean first-birth rates of the youn­ger cohort
                (women directly affected by the pol­icy), and the third con­tains the dif­fer­ence between
                the cohorts. Row 1 of each panel in the table con­sid­ers women from high-income
                house­holds only, and row 2 of each panel con­sid­ers women from low-income house­
                holds only. Women from high-income house­holds had a 0.7 per­cent­age point increase
                (15.5% vs. 16.2%) in first births com­pared with a 4.6 per­cent­age points decrease
                (43.7% vs. 39.1%) for women from low-income house­holds. The DD esti­ma­tes of
                these rates, shown in bold, cor­re­sponds to a (sta­tis­ti­cally sig­nif­i­cant) 5.3 per­cent­age
                point rel­a­tive decline in first-birth rates after the reform for women from low-income
                house­holds com­pared with women from high-income house­holds.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1414                                                                                                  S. Muchiri

                 Table 2 Difference-in-dif­fer­ence (DD) esti­ma­tes

                                                                              Pre-Reform             Post-Reform    Difference in
                                                                              Birth Cohort           Birth Cohort   Birth Cohort
                 Variable                                                          (1)                    (2)            (3)

                 A. Teens Defined as Ages 15–18

                      1. Individuals from high-income house­holds                 0.155                 0.162          0.007
                                                                                 (0.362)               (0.368)        (0.011)
                                                                                 [1,203]               [1,125]

                      2. Individuals from low-income house­holds                  0.437                 0.391        −0.046
                                                                                 (0.496)               (0.488)       (0.009)
                                                                                 [3,203]               [3,201]

                      3. Difference between income groups                         0.282                0.299
                                                                                 (0.009)              (0.009)
                        DD esti­mate                                                                  −0.053
                                                                                                      (0.000)
                 B. Teens Defined as Ages 15–19

                      1. Individuals from high-income house­holds                 0.160                 0.161          0.001
                                                                                 (0.366)               (0.368)        (0.012)
                                                                                  [959]                [1,001]

                      2. Individuals from low-income house­holds                  0.445                 0.430        −0.042
                                                                                 (0.497)               (0.491)       (0.010)
                                                                                 [2,467]               [2,781]

                      3. Difference between income groups                         0.285                0.242
                                                                                 (0.011)              (0.010)
                        DD esti­mate                                                                  −0.043
                                                                                                      (0.002)

                 Notes: Data are from the 2014 Kenya Demographic and Health Survey. Cells con­tain mean teen­age birth
                 rates for women in high-income house­holds (in row 1 in each panel) and low-income house­holds (in row
                 2 of each panel). Standard errors are shown in paren­the­ses, and sam­ple sizes are shown in square brack­
                 ets. Column 1 shows mean teen­age first-birth rates for birth cohorts not impacted by the free sec­ond­ary
                 school (FSE) pol­icy; these are women who were beyond sec­ond­ary school age when the pol­icy was passed.
                 Column 2 shows mean teen­age first-birth rates for birth cohorts affected by the pol­icy; these are women
                 who were youn­ger than 19 when the pol­icy was passed. The DD esti­mate is obtained by cal­cu­lat­ing the
                 dif­fer­ence between val­ues in col­umn 3.

                     In a sen­si­tiv­ity anal­y­sis, I con­duct a sim­il­ar exer­cise but define teens as 15 to
                 19-year-olds.8 The results, illus­trated in panel B of Table 2, show a sta­tis­ti­cally sig­
                 nif­i­cant 4.3 per­cent­age point decrease in first-birth rates for women from low-income
                 house­holds com­pared with women from high-income house­holds. Although this DD
                 esti­mate indi­cates a declin­ing trend and is sta­tis­ti­cally sig­nif­i­cant, it is smaller in mag­
                 ni­tude rel­a­tive to the esti­mate shown in panel A. Nonetheless, the evi­dence from the
                 DD esti­ma­tes thus far pro­vi­des pre­lim­i­nary evi­dence supporting the hypoth­e­sis that
                 teen­age moth­er­hood responded to the pol­icy reform.

                 8
                     Most stu­dents grad­u­ate high school at the age of 18, but it is not uncom­mon for some to do so at age 19.

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
Free Secondary Schooling and Teenage Motherhood                                                                 1415

                Table 3 Estimates on teen­age moth­er­hood (ages 15–18)

                                                                       Low vs. High Income               Low vs. Middle Income

                                                                         1                 2                3               4

                Post-FSE Cohort × Low-Income Family                 −0.052**           −0.053**
                                                                    (0.020)            (0.019)
                Post-FSE Cohort                                      0.006              0.005            −0.051**       −0.057**
                                                                    (0.015)            (0.015)           (0.019)        (0.018)
                Low-Income Family                                    0.282**            0.252**
                                                                    (0.014)            (0.017)
                Post-FSE Cohort × Low-Income Family                                                       0.005          0.008
                                                                                                         (0.022)        (0.022)
                Low-Income Family                                                                         0.159**        0.142**
                                                                                                         (0.016)        (0.017)
                N                                                      8,732             8,732             8,521          8,521
                Control Variables                                        N                 Y                 N              Y

                Notes: Women in the pre–free sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion (FSE) cohort were born in 1985–1988, and women in
                the post-FSE were born in 1990–1993. Low-income fam­i­lies are those with wealth index between 0–2,
                mid­dle-income fam­i­lies have a wealth index of 3 and higher-income fam­i­lies have a wealth index of 4.
                Control var­i­ables are years of for­mal edu­ca­tion, dummy var­i­able for rural/urban sta­tus, reli­gion, prov­ince
                sta­tus, and eth­nic­ity.
                **p < .01

                Regression Model

                I for­mal­ize the empir­i­cal strat­egy in a regres­sion frame­work to account for dif­fer­
                ences in indi­vid­ual and house­hold char­ac­ter­is­tics. Moreover, con­trol­ling for indi­vid­
                ual and house­hold char­ac­ter­is­tics pro­duces more effi­cient esti­ma­tes. I esti­mate a DD
                esti­ma­tor of the fol­low­ing form:

                                                                               (                     )
                               Yi,t = β0 + β1Cohortt + β 2 Lowi + β3 Cohortt × Lowi + β 4 X i,t + ε i,t ,                       (4)

                where Yi,t is an indi­ca­tor of whether a woman was a teen­age mother (equal to 1) or
                not (equal to 0). Lowi equals 1 for women from low-income house­holds and 0 oth­er­
                wise; Cohortt equals to 1 for women in the youn­ger cohort and 0 oth­er­wise. Xi,t is a
                vec­tor of indi­vid­ual and house­hold char­ac­ter­is­tics: edu­ca­tion (in years), prov­ince of
                res­id­ ence, rural/urban res­id­ ence, eth­nic­ity, and reli­gion. The pri­mary coef­fi­cient of
                inter­est, β3, cap­tures the pol­icy’s impact on teen­age moth­er­hood and is interpreted as
                the DD esti­ma­tor. εi,t is an idi­o­syn­cratic error term.

                Empirical Results
                Tables 3 and 4 pres­ent the results with­out indi­vid­u­al and house­hold con­trols (in col­
                umns 1 and 3) and with these indi­vid­ual and house­hold char­ac­ter­is­tics (in col­umns 2
                and 4). In Table 3, I define teens as aged 15–18; in Table 4, I relax this age restric­tion
                and con­sider teens to be aged 15–19, in line with the idea that grad­u­a­tion could be
                delayed, either because of delayed school entry or grade rep­et­i­tions. In addi­tion to

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
1416                                                                                                    S. Muchiri

                 Table 4 Estimates of teen­age moth­er­hood (ages 15–19)

                                                                        Low vs. High-Income            Low vs. Middle-Income

                                                                           1                2              3                 4

                 Post-FSE Cohort × Low-Income Family                  −0.043*           −0.044*
                                                                      (0.021)           (0.021)
                 Low-Income Family                                     0.286**           0.266**
                                                                      (0.015)           (0.019)
                 Post-FSE Cohort × Low-Income Family                                                   0.016             0.019
                                                                                                      (0.025)           (0.025)
                 Low-Income Family                                                                     0.155**           0.142**
                                                                                                      (0.018)           (0.019)
                 Post-FSE Cohort                                       0.001            −0.001        −0.057**          −0.065**
                                                                      (0.017)           (0.017)       (0.021)           (0.020)
                 N                                                      7,208            7,208          7,010             7,010
                 Control Variables                                        N                Y              N                 Y

                 Notes: Women in the pre–free sec­ond­ary edu­ca­tion (FSE) cohort were born in 1985–1988, and women
                 in the post–FSE cohort were born in 1990–1993. The low-income fam­i­lies are those with wealth indi­ces
                 between 0–2, mid­dle-income fam­i­lies have a wealth index of 3, and higher-income fam­i­lies have a wealth
                 index of 4. Control var­i­ables are years of for­mal edu­ca­tion, a dummy var­i­able for rural/urban sta­tus, reli­
                 gion, prov­ince sta­tus, and eth­nic­ity.
                 *p < .05; **p < .01

                 com­par­ing women from low-income house­holds with those from high-income house­
                 holds, I con­duct a sen­si­tiv­ity anal­y­sis com­par­ing women from low-income house­
                 holds (col­umns 1 and 2) with those from mid­dle-income house­holds (col­umns 3 and
                 4) for sen­si­tiv­ity anal­y­sis.
                     In col­umn 1 of Table 3, the coef­fi­cient on Cohort × Low, which pro­vi­des the pol­
                 icy’s impact on teen­age moth­er­hood, is −0.052 and is sta­tis­ti­cally dif­fer­ent from
                 zero. This fig­ure changes mar­gin­ally to −0.053 when I include addi­tional con­trols
                 in col­umn 2, but it remains neg­a­tive and sta­tis­ti­cally sig­nif­i­cant at the 1% level of
                 sig­nif­i­cance. These results sug­gest that the FSE pol­icy led to a 5.3 per­cent­age point
                 reduc­tion in teen­age moth­er­hood. If the aver­age rate of teen­age moth­er­hood for poor
                 women before the pol­icy is approx­im   ­ a­tely 48%, as shown in Figure 4, then a 5.3 per­
                 cent­age point decrease is equiv­a­lent to approx­im   ­ a­tely a 12% drop in teen­age moth­
                 er­hood. I con­sider this to be a con­se­quen­tial impact because, as discussed ear­lier,
                 reduced teen­age moth­er­hood could trans­late into higher rates of sec­ond­ary school
                 com­ple­tion, lower rates of child mar­riage, reduced inci­dence of com­pli­ca­tions from
                 child­birth, and reduced inter­gen­er­a­tional trans­mis­sion of pov­erty. In addi­tion, this
                 decline in teen­age moth­er­hood pro­vi­des jus­ti­fic­ a­tion for a targeted edu­ca­tional pol­
                 icy like FSE, espe­cially given the projected increase of teen­ag­ers in devel­op­ing
                 countries.
                     The results shown in Table 3 may be under­es­ti­mates if 19-year-olds are regarded
                 as non-school-aged. As men­tioned ear­lier, most stu­dents grad­u­ate high school at age
                 18, but grad­u­at­ing at age 19 is not uncom­mon. I test the sen­si­tiv­ity of the esti­ma­tes
                 to the def­i­ni­tion of teens by con­sid­er­ing those aged 19 or youn­ger. The results are
                 presented in Table 4. Teenage moth­er­hood declined by approx­i­ma­tely 4.3 per­cent­age

Downloaded from http://read.dukeupress.edu/demography/article-pdf/58/4/1401/934193/1401muchiri.pdf
by guest
You can also read