ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience

Page created by Maurice Sullivan
 
CONTINUE READING
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
ARTICLE

Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional
Experience
DANIELA DE ANGELI       , RYAN M. KELLY, AND EAMONN O’NEILL

     Abstract Museums are emotionally driven sites. People visit museums to feel and their emotions
     influence how the museum and its artefacts are perceived. Thus, evaluating emotional states are
     increasingly important for museums. However, evaluating visitors’ experiences is increasingly challenging,
     especially with the introduction of new and emerging technology. Moreover, people’s behaviour is not
     strictly objective and rational. While emotional states are subjective and hard to verbalize or observe, emoji
     are often used to express emotions on mobile and smartphone messaging applications. In this paper we
     investigate whether emoji can capture emotional states elicited by museum experiences, supporting
     traditional methods such as interviews. While other non-verbal self-report methods have been used to
     evaluate emotions, this is the first tool of this kind designed specifically to measure emotions elicited by
     museum experiences. We designed a set of 9 emoji illustrating a variety of emotional states beyond happy-
     or-not. Then, we confirmed that participants understood our emoji’s intended concept using a word
     association task. Finally, we used our 9 emoji to evaluate an interactive museum experience. We also run
     interviews and we investigated the correspondence between participants’ comments and the emoji they
     chose. Through this study we gained a better understanding of how the emoji can be deployed to capture a
     range of visitors’ emotional experiences. Our findings suggest that emoji can capture which emotional
     states participants felt beyond the happy-or-not dichotomy, but that they should be complemented with
     traditional methods such as interviews to understand why specific emotions were felt.

INTRODUCTION                                                    objects, emoji illustrating facial expressions are
                                                                particularly popular in everyday life to express
     In the past couple of decades, emoji have                  emotions (Barbieri, Ronzano, & Saggion, 2016;
been growing in types and number (G€      ulsßen,               G€ulsßen, 2016).
2016), becoming part of everyday language and                        Face emoji have been increasingly deployed
                ulsßen, 2016; Oxford Dictionar-
everyday life (G€                                               to capture users’ satisfaction, both online and
ies, 2015). Emoji are text-based pictographic                   offline. For example, emoji have been used to
characters illustrating facial expressions and                  evaluate consumers’ emotions regarding the
abstract concepts such as emotions (Rodrigues                   design of new products or brands such as food
et al., 2017). According to Rodrigues et al.                    and drinks (Desmet, Overbeeke, & Tax, 2001;
(2017), emoji are used to help us to visually                   Jaeger et al., 2017). An increasingly common
express our emotions, especially in social net-                 example of this practice is the ‘smiley terminal’
works (Kelly & Watts, 2015; Vidal, Ares, &                      or ‘satisfaction kiosk’ often seen in airports.
Jaeger, 2016). While there are multiple types of                Emoji have recently been introduced in muse-
emoji, ranging for example from animals to                      ums too to capture visitor satisfaction, but they

Daniela De Angeli (dada21@bath.ac.uk) is a postdoctoral researcher at the Centre for the Analysis of Motion,
Entertainment Research and Applications (CAMERA) at the University of Bath. Ryan M. Kelly (ryan.kelly@unimelb.
edu.au) is a Research Fellow in the School of Computing and Information Systems at the University of Melbourne.
Eamonn O’Neill (maseon@bath.ac.uk) is Professor of Human-Computer Interaction at the University of Bath.

© 2020 The Authors. Curator: The Museum Journal published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.                                      1
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution
and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
Figure 1. Visitor satisfaction kiosks from a museum in Spain (left and center) and Germany (right). Photographs
taken by Daniela De Angeli in 2018.

are far less common (Figure 1). These satisfac-             have limitations when it comes to capturing
tion kiosks usually display 3 to 5 emoji ranging            emotions (Foster, 2008; “Visitor Evaluation
from happy to unhappy, so are limited to                    Guidelines” 2015) (see A Visitor-Centred
expressing the dichotomy of happy-or-not.                   Museum). Emotions are not easy to verbalize
While this may be enough to judge whether a                 or to observe (Desmet, Overbeeke, & Tax,
user is satisfied with the service in an airport            2001; Hein, 1998; Mehrabian, 1995). More-
bathroom or in a shop, museum visitor experi-               over, the aforementioned methods tend to be
ences are more complex as they are very personal            time consuming both for the museum and the
and influenced by a number of factors, including            visitors. Image-based methods illustrating, for
emotions (Brent Ritchie et al., 2011; Norris &              example, facial expressions are often more reli-
Tisdale, 2013; de Rojas & Camarero, 2008).                  able in capturing emotional states. However,
      As visitors are more and more interested in           these methods also have limitations (see Evalu-
‘feeling’ rather than ‘learning’ (Munro, 2014), it          ation of Visitors’ Experiences). For example,
is becoming critical for museums to understand              they may present too many emotions and
which emotions their public feel, where, and                require visitors too much time and effort to
why. Once museums have a better understand-                 use. Emoji may offer an approach that is
ing of visitors’ emotional experiences, they can            quicker but still effective but there is very little
plan for narratives that are more engaging,                 formal research to support whether or not
meaningful, and ultimately satisfying (Galani               emoji can effectively capture visitors’ emotions.
et al., 2011; Hansen, Kortbek, & Grønbæk,                        This paper explores the potential of a range
2012; Simon, 2016). For example, memorials                  of emoji specifically designed for evaluating visi-
and history museums may wish to elicit empa-                tors’ emotional experience beyond simply
thy with historical events and with victims of              happy-or-not. We present an initial investiga-
atrocities (Savenije & de Bruijn, 2017). Local              tion of an emoji-based method tailored specifi-
museums may want to connect with their com-                 cally to museums, to evaluate whether museum
munities at a deeper emotional level (Munro,                visitors are satisfied (i.e. happy) or not with their
2014).                                                      experience. The method must be rapid and intu-
      While understanding visitors’ emotional               itive, so it should include a limited number of
experience is such a key factor in improving visi-          emoji. Thus, we designed a set of 9 emoji icons
tors’ satisfaction, traditional methods such as             illustrating key emotions elicited during a
questionnaires, interviews and observations can             museum visit (see Design Process for Emoji).

2                               Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
Here, we use the term emoji to identify icons          specific item. Here, when we talk about emo-
that are pictorial (i.e. not textual) and are not      tions, we refer to a range of emotional experi-
necessarily digital. Since emoji may be misinter-      ences including boredom, anger and inspired.
preted (Caicedo & Van Beuzekom, 2006), we                   Researchers also agree that emotions are
set out to validate both the method and the            multidimensional. While these dimensions can
emoji themselves. First, we evaluated whether          sometimes vary across fields and researchers,
the emoji we designed presented the intended           there seems to be agreement on two main dimen-
meaning (see Emoji Validation: Word Associa-           sions: arousal and valence (Baas, De Dreu, &
tion Task). Then, we further validated emoji as        Nijstad, 2008; Russell, 1980). Arousal happens
a method to evaluate visitors’ emotional               when a person ‘feels’ something, meaning their
responses through an emoji-based evaluation of         senses are stimulated or a physiological/psycho-
an augmented reality sandbox developed for the         logical state is awoken. In other words, it identi-
UK’s National Trust (see Validation of an              fies the level of reactivity to an event or stimuli
Emoji-based Tool to Capture Emotional Reac-            (Russell, 1980). Then, we interpret this arousal
tions). Based on findings from this study, we          to determine why it occurs and what it is, often in
discuss how emoji can capture visitors’ emo-           relation to context. For example, if we are having
tional experiences beyond happy-or-not (see            a negative experience, we may be sad or angry.
Discussion).                                           The differentiation between positive and nega-
                                                       tive experiences or events is known as valence
BACKGROUND                                             (Barrett, Lewis, & Haviland-Jones, 2008).
                                                       According to Ortony et al. (1988), every emotion
Defining Emotions                                       has a valence, a polarity. Thus, there is no neutral
                                                       emotion. However, an emotion can still be asso-
     The question ‘what is an emotion?’ is still       ciated with different valences (e.g. surprise).
very complex to answer (Solomon, 1993). Emo-           Moreover, emotions can present with different
tions are often confused with other terms such as      levels of intensity, which is the degree to which
feelings or other affective states such as attitudes   emotions are experienced regardless of valence
and moods that impact human behaviour less but         (van Goozen et al., 1994). Indeed, survey tools to
last longer (Scherer, 2005). Nevertheless,             evaluate emotions often use different levels of
researchers usually agree that emotions are            intensity to gain a deeper understanding of per-
prompt responses to events or stimuli. Indeed,         sonal emotions. For example, Scherer (2005)
while feelings and moods have long term effects,       arranges emotions with five degrees of intensity,
emotions are more immediate and can change             Bradley and Lang (1994) propose a nine-point
rapidly (Borod, 2000). For example, Borod              scale, and Desmet (2005) uses a three-point scale
(2000) defines emotions as changes in conscious        (not-felt, light, intense).
subjective feelings in response to an evaluation of
external or internal events. Scherer (2000) high-      A Visitor-Centred Museum
lights how emotions are short affective episodes
that change over time in response to internal or           Traditionally, museums considered their
external events. More recently, Del Chiappa            key mission to preserve human knowledge and
et al. (2014) defined emotions as affective vari-      educate (Murphy, 2007). However, recent
ables elicited by an experience or by the use of a     advances in digital technologies are pressuring

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                         3
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

museums into redesigning their exhibits to                       the museum is telling, more enjoyment, and
share knowledge through more enjoyable,                          ultimately a deeper and more frequent engage-
entertaining experiences in order to drive visits                ment with the institution. (Dindar, 2015)
and support traditional goals such as education
(Tallon & Walker, 2008). This is happening for                     In particular, Filippini-Fantoni explains
a variety of reasons. First, digital technologies             how a visitor-centred approach to exhibition
have driven changes in society influencing how                development can increase visitors’ satisfaction
we live, communicate, learn and how we per-                   (Dindar, 2015). Understanding visitors is key
ceive the world (Bryce, 2001; Greenfield, 2014;               when we consider that their museum experience
Siemens, 2005). Museums need to maintain                      is often based on personal experience rather
their relevance in a changing society as their                than objective values (Gallarza & Saura, 2006).
mission is not only to preserve knowledge but                 In particular, research has highlighted how
also to share it (Tallon & Walker, 2008). Sec-                emotions strongly influence visitors’ satisfaction
ond, digital technology offers us so many                     (de Rojas & Camarero, 2008). For example, Del
options for our entertainment, some passive                   Chiappa et al. (2014) interviewed 410 visitors at
(e.g. movies) other more interactive (e.g. games)             the National Museum of Archaeology “G.A.
(Greenfield, 2014). While traditional object-fo-              Sanna” in Sardinia (Italy) and discovered that
cused exhibitions tend to limit visitors’ interac-            higher positive emotions reported corresponded
tion to merely passive observation, this is not               to a higher level of satisfaction.
how most people choose to use their free time                      While the evolution of digital technology
(Simon, 2010). Hence, the simple display of                   has strongly influenced museums and their
facts and objects is no longer enough to attract              approach to exhibition design, the evaluation of
and engage visitors. As a consequence, muse-                  visitors’ experience and satisfaction is crucial
ums are embracing a more visitor-centred                      regardless of the nature of the exhibition.
approach where it is increasingly important to                Whether an exhibition is digital or not, interac-
understand visitors and how they interact with                tive or passive, the evaluation of visitors’ emo-
the artefacts in order to design narratives that              tional response to it is key to understanding how
are more engaging and meaningful (Galani                      they perceive the museum and ultimately to
et al., 2011; Hansen, Kortbek, & Grønbæk,                     increase engagement. However, evaluating the
2012; Samis, Michaelson, & Baird, 2017;                       impact of emerging technologies can be particu-
Simon, 2016).                                                 larly challenging as both their implementation
     According to Silvia Filippini-Fantoni, spe-              and impact are less known than long established
cialist in museum audience analysis and former                tools (Damala et al., 2013). Although museums
director of interpretation, media and evaluation              usually have well-established methods to evalu-
at the Indianapolis Museum of Art:                            ate traditional passive visits, they may not yet
                                                              have a standard protocol to evaluate new and
        Museums are institutions that serve the               emerging technologies:
    public and therefore knowing what people enjoy
    and don’t is fundamental to helping these insti-                  They have yet to cultivate standard protocol
    tutions create improved experiences for their                for measuring the success of the technologies
    visitors. It can result in a better appreciation of          they deploy. Exacerbating this challenge is the
    the art, better understanding of the stories that            notion that evaluation should occur both before

4                                  Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
and after technologies are implemented; staff      included four A4 pages of questions (Damala
  must have thorough understanding of how the        et al., 2013).
  tools correspond with the museum’s mission and           Observational methods such as direct obser-
  goals prior to being embraced at scale. Unfortu-   vation have also been used in museums, but it is
  nately, there are not always concrete precedents   difficult to acquire a deep understanding of visi-
  for the use of new technologies in the cultural    tors’ feelings just by observing them. Indeed, “re-
  heritage sector, and museums that are early        searchers . . . are limited by what they can
  adopters often gamble when trying them (John-      actually see” (Hein, 1998, 47:101). Often only
  son et al., 2015, 28)                              strong emotional reactions are clearly visible
                                                     (e.g. very angry), and people express and experi-
     In the next section we explore which meth-      ence emotions differently accordingly to their
ods and strategies museums are deploying to          cultural or personal background (Scherer, 1988).
understand their visitors’ experience, whether             Emotions can also be measured through
with traditional or emerging tools and tech-         interaction with gesture, gaze and auditory
nologies, and whether these methods are effec-       stimuli. For instance, Ramanarayanan et al.
tive in providing relevant and useful data.          (2015) used a variety of equipment and software
                                                     tools including Microsoft Kinect to evaluate the
Evaluation of Visitors’ Experiences                  quality of public presentations in relation to
                                                     speech, face, emotion and body movement. Lu
     Recent evaluation guidelines from cultural      and Petiot (2016) used a set of auditory stimuli
institutions such as the Smithsonian (“Visitor       to convey and assess a set of emotions such as
Evaluation Guidelines” 2015) and the East of         funny, serious, relaxed, and depressed. One
England Museum Hub (Foster, 2008) indicate           advantage of these techniques is that they are
that the most used methods to evaluate visitors’     unobtrusive as they do not require users to ver-
experiences were and still are observation, ques-    balize their feelings. However, the technology
tionnaires and interviews. For example, visitors’    used to sense non-verbal behaviour often has
experience with the exhibition The Hague and         limitations. For example, Mueller and Bianchi-
the Atlantic Wall: War in the City of Peace at       Berthouze (2015) noted that gesture recogni-
Museon, in the Netherlands, was evaluated            tion technology is unpredictable and the set of
using a mixed methodology of observations,           movements and gestures cannot be predefined.
questionnaires, and interviews (Damala et al.,       Moreover, such methods cannot usually mea-
2013).                                               sure mixed emotions and their range is limited
     However, subjective and subtle emotions         to few basic emotions (Desmet, 2005). Finally,
are difficult to articulate and measure with ver-    it is typically not feasible for museums to buy,
bal questionnaires (Desmet, Overbeeke, & Tax,        install or persuade visitors to wear devices such
2001; Mehrabian, 1995). Moreover, standard           as body trackers, brain or temperature sensing
scales such as Likert tend to gain positive, non-    devices.
candid responses (Benedek & Miner, 2002).                  Visitors’ behaviour and interactions may
While longer questionnaire may provide richer        also be video recorded. This method can be
amount of data, they would also be time con-         unobtrusive and can provide rich data about vis-
suming for visitors; for example, a questionnaire    itors’ behaviour and interactions with the exhi-
used to evaluate visitors’ experience at Museon      bition. A video may subsequently be analysed in

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                      5
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
Figure 2. Range of Emotions Wheels. (a) Geneva Emotion Wheel. Source: (Tran, 2004). (b) Plutchik’s Wheel. Source:
(Garcia & Hammond, 2016). (c) Plutchik’s Wheel with cartoons. Photo credit: Copy Press. (d) Plutchik’s Wheel with
emoji. Source: https://usabilla.com/blog/best-ux-articlesaugust/.
ARTICLE Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors' Emotional Experience
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

detail in order to collect and interpret informa-       power and valence. Each emotion is repre-
tion about visitors and their experience. Never-        sented by a different colour and five circles of
theless, this method remains little used, often         different size indicating five degrees of inten-
due to ethical concerns or the difficulties in          sity, so participants can also indicate the inten-
installing appropriate recording equipment              sity of their emotions. Plutchik’s Wheel of
within the museum (Damala et al., 2013).                Emotions is also well known and has been used
     All of these methods have their merits and         for example to detect emotions on social media
limitations, meaning that combinations of differ-       (Tromp & Pechenizkiy, 2015). It includes 8
ent techniques (i.e. mixed methods) are often           basic emotions that are considered key to our
preferred to evaluate visitor experiences. By           survival. These emotions come in pairs and are
using combinations of methods for data collec-          located opposite to each other: Joy and Sad-
tion, museums can cover a wider range of people         ness; Acceptance and Disgust; Fear and Anger;
and data. For example, interviews are often used        Surprise and Anticipation. This arrangement
to support both direct observations and ques-           is due to Plutchik’s belief that opposite emo-
tionnaires, providing more in-depth informa-            tions cannot be felt at the same time (Plutchik,
tion. For example, the Smithsonian uses a mix           1980).
of qualitative and quantitative methods includ-              However, the time taken to apply these
ing interviews, surveys, and direct observation         tools including multiple emotions and intensity
to support their visitors’ evaluation (“Visitor         levels can be challenging (van Goozen et al.,
Evaluation Guidelines” 2015). Still, combina-           1994) and too time consuming in some contexts
tions of traditional methods provide limited            such as museums where evaluation methods
data about emotions and, as discussed above,            should not disrupt the visit. Moreover, the out-
visitors’ experience is strongly influenced by          comes of verbal methods rely on people’s ability
their emotions, which are not easy to verbalize         to express their emotions (Fox, 2008). A way to
(Reijneveld et al., 2003) or to observe (Hein,          overcome this limitation of language-based
1998).                                                  methods is to visualize emotions as images
                                                        rather than words (Foglia, Prete, & Zanda,
Methods to Evaluate Emotions                            2008). Indeed, there have been adaptations of
                                                        emotion wheels using images such as cartoons
    As noted in the previous section, tradi-            or emoji rather than text (Figure 2). In general,
tional methods such as interviews and direct            image-based methods tend to be more fruitful
observation may struggle to capture visitors’           to capture emotions than verbal ones. For exam-
emotional experiences. However, there are               ple, a widely accepted image-based tool is Brad-
other fields such as consumer evaluation where          ley and Lang’s (1994) graphical version of
emotions have been successfully evaluated. For          Mehrabian’s PAD which used Manikins
example, the Geneva Emotion Wheel is a                  (graphical characters) combined with a nine-
well-known method to evaluate emotions                  point scale to make the tool more intuitive to
(Scherer, 2005). Participants indicate which            participants. However, it uses graphic charac-
emotion(s) they are experiencing from a                 ters to represent emotions that are not familiar
wheel-shaped emotion scale (Scherer, 2005).             to the general public, and they may require too
Emotions are arranged in this circular pattern          much time and effort from visitors to under-
according to two major dimensions: control/             stand. Moreover, this method cannot measure

8                            Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
differentiated emotions (Caicedo & Van Beu-           cartoon face that best identifies their experience
zekom, 2006).                                         (Desmet, 2005).
     Emoji have become increasingly familiar               One of the most successful non-verbal
to the general public, notably through mobile         self-report methods is PrEmo (Product Emo-
messaging systems. Consequently, they are             tion Measuring Instrument), which uses a
increasingly used to express and capture emo-         three-point scale (not-felt, light, intense) with
tions in a variety of fields including for            12 cartoon animations to represent emotions
example food consumption (Jaeger et al.,              (Desmet, 2005) (Figure 2). Each of these
2017; Vidal, Ares, & Jaeger, 2016) and well-          emotions was selected to represent emotions
being (Fane et al., 2018). The ‘Emoji-o-              elicited by consumer products: desire, satisfac-
meter’ uses emoji to evaluate children’s experi-      tion, pride, hope, joy, fascination, disgust, dis-
ences with technology (Read & MacFarlane,             satisfaction, shame, fear, sadness and boredom.
2006). ‘Emoji-face’ assessment scales have also       Each cartoon was animated and includes sound
been deployed in museums to augment tradi-            effects designed to facilitate their interpreta-
tional questionnaire scales (Loizeau, K€     undig,   tion. However, this could potentially disrupt
and Oppikofer, 2015; Mittelman and Epstein,           visitors’ experience within the museum. It also
2009). Similar emoji systems are often seen in        means that PrEmo requires the use of a com-
kiosks at airports as a method to empower             puter or mobile device, with corresponding
customers and measure their level of satisfac-        oversight and maintenance requirements if
tion (Dickinson, 2018). On one hand, these            deployed in museums. Even if the cartoon
tools are a very rapid way to capture users’          were instead printed on paper, PrEmo was
basic experience. On the other hand, they             designed exclusively to measure emotions eli-
usually evaluate users’ satisfaction simply in        cited by consumer products. While it can be
term of happiness and unhappiness. As a               used in other fields by selecting only relevant
result, they capture a very limited range of          emotions, emotions relevant to museums (e.g.
emotions, while people’s emotional experiences        achievement and social engagement) are miss-
are far more complex and go beyond happy or           ing from this set. The purpose of our emoji
unhappy.                                              collection is not to evaluate the product satis-
     Pictorial representations of facial expres-      faction of a generic consumer but to under-
sion, such as cartoons and photos, have been          stand museum visitors’ satisfaction beyond
used effectively to communicate a wider variety       simply happy-or-not. In the next section we
of emotions and have been researched as evalua-       describe the reasoning behind the design of
tion tools in a variety of fields (Bradley and        each emoji in our set and the emotional states
Lang, 1994; Desmet, 2005). For example,               they represent.
Microsoft tested a questionnaire using pictures
of six faces as stimuli to get user input on intan-   DESIGN PROCESS FOR EMOJI
gible properties such as “desire” and “fun” (Ben-
edek & Miner, 2002). Emofaces uses a series of             The overall quality of the museum experi-
female and male faces to represent emotions           ence is often based on personal experience rather
ranging from pleasant to unpleasant, and              than objective values (Gallarza & Saura,
intense to calm (Posner, Russell, & Peterson,         2006). Personal experience includes social con-
2005); Emocards ask people to choose the              text, personal motivation, education and
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

Figure 3. The emoji icons used to capture visitors’ emotional experience.

expectations (D. Bryce et al., 2014; de Rojas &             & Chen, 2010), where people feel part of the
Camarero, 2008). Visitors’ perceptions of                   museum, and the museum part of the commu-
the museum experience are affected by                       nity. To close the circle, if visitors feel involved
whether their expectations are met, they feel               and a sense of achievement, then they will prob-
engaged and involved and they perceive the                  ably enjoy their visit. Thus, in this paper we
museum service as adequate (Bride, Disegna, &               argue that visitors feel satisfied not only if they
Scuderi, 2014; Bryce et al., 2014; Lu, Chi, &               have received an excellent service but also if
Liu, 2015).                                                 they:
     We draw on Thurley’s (2005) account of
the heritage experience as a cycle of understand-             •   Enjoy their visit;
ing, valuing, caring and enjoying. If the                     •   Achieve and/or acquire new knowledge;
museum narrative is clearly communicated,                     •   Feel inspired;
then visitors are more likely to enjoy it and find            •   Feel socially involved;
it relevant. If visitors find the narrative is valu-          •   Feel entertained/engaged;
able to them, they may also learn and feel a                  •   Find the narrative clear, communicated
sense of achievement. If visitors feel a sense of                 clearly
achievement and that the content they are
interacting with is relevant, then they feel more               Accordingly, we designed a set of 9 emoji to
involved and they are more inclined to care for             mirror emotional states directly related to the
what the museum has to offer. Involvement                   above principles of visitors’ satisfaction or
implies some kind of social participation (Chen             indicative of dissatisfaction (Figure 3):

10                               Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
•   Basic enjoyment is illustrated by the emoji    and WhatsApp render these icons differently
      Happy, Sad, and Angry.                         (Miller et al., 2016). In order to address this
  •   Learning outcome is represented by the         issue, we ran a validation study to investigate
      emoji Achieved;                                whether participants recognised our emoji’s
  •   Feeling overwhelmed by information is          intended concept or if instead they misinter-
      illustrated by Tired;                          preted the emotion we intended to depict
  •   Inspiration is illustrated by the emoji        with a given emoji. We developed an online
      Inspired;                                      word association test using Google Forms. The
  •   Lack of engagement is illustrated by the       survey included standard demographic
      emoji Bored;                                   questions such as gender and age. Then, the
  •   Feeling involved is illustrated by the emoji   emoji were displayed one after another. Each
      Socially Engaged;                              emoji was followed by a text box where par-
  •   Lack of clarity is represented by the emoji    ticipants were invited to type the first word
      Confused;                                      that occurred to them in response to the
                                                     stimulus.
     The emoji illustrating happiness, anger and           Our method was inspired by Prada et al.
sadness were the easiest to design, possibly         (2016) and Rodrigues et al. (2017) who asked
because they were the most familiar ones. For        participants to state the first meaning or emo-
example, they are often seen in smiley terminals     tion that came to mind related to an emoji.
at airports. The design of other emoji, such as      Moreover, as in Pejtersen (1991), we used word
the ones representing social experience or           association to identify the meaning of images.
achievement, was more challenging. We                Word association is a well known method
researched existing emoji including those used       (Jung, 1910; Nielsen & Ingwersen, 1999) which
by Apple iOS, Facebook Messenger and Skype           consists of presenting a stimulus and the partici-
in order to facilitate our design but also to make   pant answering as quickly as possible with the
them more recognizable. While these icons            first word that occurs to her. The responses cre-
were already familiar to many people as they are     ate a cluster of associative representations of the
proliferating on mobile messaging apps, they         stimulus (Nielsen & Ingwersen, 1999). Word
were also protected by copyright, encouraging        association is used to collect information on
us to design our own emoji for use by researchers    people’s perceptions, emotional states, mental
and practitioners. The designs were produced         models and vocabulary (Nielsen & Ingwersen,
using a Wacom Bamboo graphics tablet and             1999; Roininen, Arvola, & L€ahteenm€aki,
Adobe Illustrator.                                   2006), and has been used to capture the mean-
                                                     ing of icons for graphical user interfaces (Pejter-
EMOJI VALIDATION: WORD                               sen 1991) and food (Roininen, Arvola, &
ASSOCIATION TASK                                     L€ahteenm€aki, 2006).
                                                           Participants were recruited through Ama-
     Pictograms such as emoji do not always          zon MTurk. The survey took about 5 minutes
clearly depict a specific emotion (Caicedo &         to complete, for which each participant received
Van Beuzekom, 2006). Moreover, they can be           $1.30. We had 121 participants, 77 males, 43
open to interpretation because social networks       females, and 1 who preferred not to say. Partici-
and mobile messaging apps such as Facebook           pants were mostly 26-35 years old (63

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                      11
Table 1.
List of emoji (ID) with their intended meaning. The table also includes the words most frequently associated with each emoji (i.e. Associated meaning) and world clouds displaying all the words
associated with each emoji

ID: Intended               Associated                                                           ID: Intended                  Associated
meaning                    meaning                    Word clouds                               meaning                       meaning                      Word clouds

A: Anger                   Angry                                                                F: Inspired                  Idea, inspired,
                                                                                                                               enlightened
                                                                                                                                                                                                   CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

B: Happiness               Happy                                                                G:                           Educated,
                                                                                                 Achievement                  proud,
                                                                                                                               graduated

C: Sadness                 Sad, crying                                                          H: Tiredness                 Tired, sick

                                                                                                                                                                                   (continued)
Table 1 . Continued

                                                   ID: Intended          Associated                    ID: Intended   Associated
                                                   meaning               meaning         Word clouds   meaning        meaning         Word clouds

                                                   D: Boredom            Bored, tired,                 I: Social      Friends, love
                                                                          sleepy                        experience

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill
                                                   E: Confusion          Confusion

13
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

participants), followed by 18-25 (23), 36-45           capture emotional aspects of a visitor experi-
(22), 46-55 (7), and the remaining 6 partici-          ence: an augmented reality (AR) sandbox
pants were over 56 years old.                          developed for the National Trust in the UK.
     The responses were recorded in a spread-          We ran this study to investigate whether the
sheet which was then imported into NVivo.              emoji can indeed be used in practice to evalu-
We used NVivo to calculate word frequency              ate visitors’ emotions. We also wanted to con-
and to group responses into categories. Syno-          firm which emotional states each emoji can
nyms (e.g. angry, anger, and annoyed were              capture to further validate how the meanings
included in the same category) and singular/           of the emoji are perceived. Can complicated
plural forms (e.g. idea, ideas) were included in       concepts such as personal achievement and
the same category. Some responses could not            social engagement be represented through
be related to others, so they were not included        emoji?; and are they selected by participants
in any category (e.g. yellow, eye, and carrot).        when there has been some educational out-
The main results illustrating the most frequent        come or positive social experience?
words associated with each emoji are summa-
rized in Table 1.                                      VALIDATION OF AN EMOJI-BASED TOOL
     The icons illustrating anger, happiness,          TO CAPTURE EMOTIONAL REACTIONS
sadness, and confusion were clearly associated
with one specific category. For example, emoji              We further validated the perceived mean-
A was associated with the category including           ing of these emoji by designing an emoji-based
words such as anger and angry (#anger) 96 out          survey. The survey was used to evaluate how
of 121 times. Emoji B (#happiness) was asso-           people perceived an interactive sandbox that the
ciated with happy 116/121 times. Emoji C               authors developed for the UK’s National Trust
(#sadness) with sad 114/121 times. Emoji E             to commemorate the tercentenary of the land-
(#confusion) was most frequently associated            scaper Capability Brown in 2017 (Figure 4).
with confusion, 90/121 times. Other emoji              The system was devised to illustrates how
were associated with more than one category.           Brown implemented his landscapes and is based
This was true in particular for the emoji illus-       on the AR sandbox developed by Reed et al.
trating the concept of social experience, which        (2014), which allows users to create topographic
was associated with words such as friend (33/          models by shaping sand. The system augments
121) and love (46/121). Emoji D (#boredom)             the sandbox by projecting a topographical map
was frequently associated with both bored (66/         onto the sand using a projector connected to a
212) but also with tired (31/121), which may           Microsoft Kinect 3D camera. When the sand is
link to mental tiredness. While emoji I                moved around, the Kinect senses the changes in
(#tiredness) was mostly associated with tired          the sand’s elevation and changes its projection
(34/121) and sick (23/121), which may relate           accordingly. For example, if someone digs a hole
to a physical tiredness. Emoji G (#inspired)           in the sand, the system projects a blue surface
was described using words such as idea (70/            representing water in that location. In our study
121) while emoji H (#achievement) with edu-            we used two versions of the sandbox: (1) Reed
cated (83/121).                                        et al.’s (2014) digital version augmented by
     Having validated the emoji as conveying           Microsoft Kinect and a projector; (2) a more
their intended meanings, we used them to               traditional ‘analogue’ version without depth

14                          Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
Figure 4. Photo (left) and schematic (right) of the augmented reality sandbox.

sensing and projection, where participants could            identified this as a gap in their audience. The
create a landscape with props such as little                introduction of new technologies is seen as a
houses and signs illustrating trees and water. By           way to attract a younger audience that is cur-
using two versions of the same sandbox, we                  rently not visiting their properties. As we were
could evaluate our emoji-based tool with both               looking specifically for young adult participants
digital and analogue experiences.                           who were not necessarily visitors to the National
                                                            Trust, we recruited participants on campus,
Participants                                                through university mailing lists and word of
                                                            mouth. All participants were students and
    We had 24 participants in total: 12 partici-            members of the staff at the University of Bath.
pants interacted with the digital version (4 male
and 8 female) and 12 with the analogue one (6               Method
females and 6 males). Participants were aged 22
to 34 years. We were particularly interested in                 An interactive sandbox was installed in a
this age range because the National Trust                   room at the University of Bath so that we could

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                           15
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

collect data in a controlled environment. Each              their experience. The emoji were presented in a
participant was given a printed copy of a topo-             paper survey. The survey was initially designed
graphical map. First, they were asked to use the            with the emoji in a circular patter opposite each
sandbox to replicate the landscape in the map. If           other similar to the Geneva Emotion Wheel (K.
they were interacting with the digital version,             R. Scherer, 2005). However, we did not want to
they simply moved the sand around and the pro-              imply an opposite valence. Rather, we acknowl-
jection on to the sand automatically changed to             edged that different emotions can happen at the
match the landscape they created. If they were              same time and do not necessarily exclude each
using the analogue version, they moved the sand             other. Thus, we displayed the 9 emoji (Figures 1
and placed the props to add landscape elements              and 5) with 3 levels of intensity in order to keep
such as trees and houses. They were then asked              the survey short and rapid. PrEmo (Desmet,
to make changes of their choosing to the land-              2005) successfully used a 3 point scale where
scape they had created. For example, they could             participants could pick low, medium or high
move the sand around to create a hill or dig a              intensity for a specific emotional state. Hence,
lake, or change the position of the props. Each             we decided to use a simple three intensity levels
participant interacted with the sandbox for                 scale for our survey. Similarly to other image-
about 15 minutes.                                           based tools such as the Geneva Emotion Wheel,
     At the end of the session, a researcher asked          intensity levels were displayed as circles of dif-
participants to complete an emoji-based survey              ferent sizes (Figure 5).
and carried out a semi-structured interview.                     After completing the survey, participants
Each participant was assigned an ID that was                were interviewed for about 15 minutes. Each
associated with their survey. The interviews                interview was guided by a list of questions to
were audio recorded and saved with the ID of                evaluate visitors’ experience and satisfaction
the participant.                                            with the sandbox. Questions ranged from
     First, participants completed a emoji-based            learning outcomes to usability of the system,
survey by selecting the emoji that best illustrated         including: Did you enjoy the experience? What

Figure 5. The survey with nine emoji and three levels of intensity each.

16                               Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
did you like/not like? Was the system easy to             between what participants said during the inter-
use? Do you have any questions? Did you need              view and the emoji they chose.
any help? Would you like to play it with your
friends?                                                  Results

Data Analysis                                                  The most frequently selected emoji were
                                                          those representing Happiness, Achievement and
     The interviews were transcribed into a               Inspired, chosen respectively 21, 19 and 14 times
Word document by the researcher who ran the               out of 24 (Figure 6). This general positive atti-
interviews. The participant’s ID was also                 tude was confirmed during the interviews where
included in the document. Meanwhile, a differ-            all participants declared that they had a pleasant
ent researcher (i.e. an analyst) organised the            experience. Pt 14 selected happiness and during
emoji selected into a spreadsheet: the document           the interview described the sandbox as a magical
included participant ID, emoji selected and at            experience: “so funny . . . easy to use. I have
which level. The analyst then carried out a qual-         enjoyed it”. Pt 16 selected both Achievement and
itative content analysis of the interviews to gain        Inspired, feelings that were confirmed by the
an understanding of the participants’ experience          interview, in which the participant explained
with the sandbox. Comments related to emo-                how a textbook can give you more knowledge
tional states and subjective experiences were             “but you cannot really know what a topographi-
identified. Using participants’ ID as a reference,        cal map is [from a book]”.
these comments were then compared with the                     While traditional surveys tend to attract
selected emoji to investigate the correspondence          only positive responses, our emoji were also able

Figure 6. Emoji selected by each participant (1 to 24).

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                          17
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

to capture negative emotions. 3 participants had           is confusing” and could be improved to better
very negative feelings. Pt 11 selected both Con-           indicate “heights”. However, the emoji repre-
fused and Angry, stating: “It is not clear, I am           senting boredom was selected instead of the
really confused”, adding later: “It is boring . . . it     confusion emoji. This could be because the
is just sand . . . it is like homework”. The fact          interaction itself was clear and easy to under-
that this participant “felt tired because it is bor-       stand but that the colour mapping was slightly
ing” was also confirmed by the emoji survey                annoying and made the experience less engag-
where maximum level of Boredom was selected                ing. Pt 2 liked the experience and selected hap-
but not Tiredness. Pt 20 was also not happy with           piness, probably because it “helped me
the overall experience, selecting Boredom and              understanding different altitudes”, however, “I
Sadness rather than Happiness and Achievement.             did not know the exact altitude of each colour”
During the interview, this participant stated              and could not “understand what the different
that the sandbox could be “slightly more inter-            colours mean”. This was reflected in the emoji
esting” and a textbook would be better, more               survey as the participant selected Confusion. Pt 9
educational. Pt 20 also complained that the                argued that the sandbox was “quite interesting”
sandbox was too small and about the consis-                but that a textbook would be more useful to gain
tency of the sand. Pt 6 and 4 also selected Bore-          a deeper understanding of the subject. At the
dom. Pt 6 was “not very excited . . . little bit           end, pt 9 was “neither tired nor excited”. These
tired” and selected both Tiredness and Boredom.            mixed feelings were also reflected on the selec-
Pt 4 said that the sandbox was “just sand” and             tion of maximum intensity of Happiness
could be improved. While participants felt con-            together with average intensity of Confusion. Pt
fused almost equally with both the digital and             22 also selected Confusion and during the inter-
analogue versions of the sandbox, the second               view confirmed the participant found the tasks
version attracted the majority of negative emo-            not so clear. Pt 21 considered the sandbox a
tions. Indeed, the emoji Boredom, Angry, Tired             “more direct way to understand (topographical
and Sadness were selected only by participants             maps)” but expressed a wish for more specific
who interacted with the analogue sandbox.                  guidelines. Again, the emoji representing Con-
     Participants also selected different levels of        fusion was also selected. Pt 19 found the experi-
intensity. In some cases, positive emotions were           ence more interesting than a textbook but
selected together with other positive ones. For            initially found the colour mapping a “little bit
example, usually people who selected the maxi-             confusing”. By the end, that was “not a prob-
mum level of Happiness choose exclusively                  lem” and did not affect the overall experience.
other positive emoji such as Educated and                  Indeed, the emoji indicating Confusion was not
Inspired. However, participants also selected              selected.
positive and negative emotional states at the
same time, a mixed experience that was con-                DISCUSSION
firmed during the interview. For example, par-
ticipants 2, 4 and 22 selected maximum                          Our study suggests that emoji can be used
Happiness together with a minimum level of                 effectively to capture visitors’ subjective experi-
Confusion or Boredom. During the interview, Pt             ences beyond a simple dichotomy of happiness
4 stated that the sandbox “is fine and a good              and unhappiness. It does so by providing an
learning tool, but the colour coding (of the map)          understanding of why emoji were selected, how

18                              Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
they were perceived and how they may be               whether the visitor had a playful and happy
deployed as a survey tool. Moreover, the digital      experience together with others.
and analogue sandboxes were clearly experi-                Only 3 participants interacting with the
enced differently, a point that was successfully      analogue sandbox selected Anger, at levels 1, 2
recorded by both interviews and emoji. In par-        and 3 respectively. However, participants did
ticular, emoji clearly displayed how one version      not describe such a negative experience during
was more enjoyable that the other. This sug-          the interview. Participants found the analogue
gests that emoji can capture emotions elicited by     sandbox particularly boring and they did not
a range of different museum experiences, and          like the fact they could not wash the sand from
also that they can capture both positive and neg-     their hands at the end. The results from the vali-
ative emotions, giving candid results. Further-       dation study indicate a very clear association
more, our findings demonstrate how emoji can          between this emoji and the concept of anger.
capture mixed emotions as in some cases partici-      One possibility is that interviewees were trying
pants selected negative emotions (e.g. Bored and      to be polite during the interviews but more fully
Confusion) at the same time and at different          expressed their emotions through the survey. It
levels.                                               is also important to remember that the emoji
     The experiences described during the inter-      survey was completed immediately after partici-
views further validated how the emoji are per-        pants interacted with the sandbox. By the time
ceived. For example, the emoji illustrating           they did the interview, their memory of the
Confusion was usually selected by participants        experience and their related emotions may have
who were frustrated with an interface or who          already changed somewhat.
did not understand some content, e.g. users who            The study raised questions around the opti-
expressed usability or clarity issues with the        mal number and individual distinctiveness of
sandbox during the interview. Participants typi-      emoji. The set of 9 emoji enabled greater
cally selected Boredom if they found the sandbox      expressive ‘bandwidth’ than the simple happy-
not engaging or interesting. Tiredness was            or-not dichotomy, while retaining sufficient
selected when a participant felt physical, rather     distinctiveness between similar or related emo-
than mental, fatigue. During the validation pro-      tions to facilitate participants’ selection of the
cess, this emoji was also associated with sick-       emoji that most closely matched a given emo-
ness, so it could be interesting to test whether it   tion. For example, the study confirmed the rela-
might be also used as part of evaluating whether      tionship between Achievement and Inspired.
an experience is likely to make visitors physically   Participants who completed the tasks with the
sick, such as motion sickness in virtual reality      sandbox felt a strong sense of achievement and
experiences (LaViola, 2000). Our results also         selected Achievement, while Inspired was
clarify the perception of the emoji illustrating      selected when participants also felt they had
Social experience. During the validation study        learned something new. In principle, creating
(section 4), this emoji was associated with words     an even larger set of emoji is appealing since the
such as friends, friendship, love and happiness.      further increased bandwidth could represent an
The participants in our case study selected this      even wider range of emotions and allow for rep-
emoji when they thought the sandbox was               resenting more fine-grained distinctions
potentially fun to play with friends. This sug-       between similar or related emotions. However,
gests that this emoji could be used to investigate    there are at least two potential problems with

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                      19
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

adding more emoji. The first is simply that a key            Through the case study we gained a better
requirement for our development of the emoji-           understanding of how the emoji can be
based approach was speed and ease of use in the         deployed to capture visitors’ emotional experi-
museum visitor setting. This requirement                ences. In particular, our results provided
would become harder to meet as the set of emoji         insights on the use of polarity (i.e. opposite
grew, almost inevitably leading to less participa-      valence of emotions) and intensity levels. Emo-
tion and therefore less visitor experience data         tions are dynamic and can present at different
collection in practice.                                 intensities (van Goozen, van de Poll, and Ser-
     The second challenge exists with any set of        geant 1994), which is why emoji surveys should
emoji and becomes even more challenging if we           allow visitors to select multiple emotions and
attempt to use additional emoji to represent            different intensity levels. While established
more fine-grained distinctions between similar          smiley terminals usually allow users to select
or related emotions. Any two emoji must be suf-         only one icon (e.g. happy, neutral or angry), our
ficiently distinct that they are reliably recog-        participants could select multiple emoji. Indeed,
nised and distinguished from each other. The            our survey recorded a variety of emotional
work reported here validated that this require-         responses, positive and negative, as well as their
ment was met for the set of 9 emoji.                    intensity level. Not only did participants often
     However, as with any representational sys-         choose more than one emoji, they also selected
tem, there is a trade off between the set of sym-       combinations of emotions of different valence,
bols, their semantics, and their expressive             such as happiness and confusion. Apparently, par-
power. Having only 9 emoji facilitated the rapid        ticipants enjoyed the sandbox despite not always
use of the survey but came at the expense of            regarding the system as very usable. This cor-
some expressiveness. For example, participants          roborates the claim that usability cannot fully
often selected Sadness to express dissatisfaction       explain users’ experience without taking emo-
rather than sadness or grief per se. This makes         tions into account (Agarwal and Meyer, 2009).
sense since the sandbox was not designed to             Furthermore, participants selected different
provoke strong emotional states or to upset visi-       intensity levels, ranging from low (1) to high
tors. This finding suggests that we may need            (3). Thus, the tool was able to record emotional
more emoji than this basic set of 9 as users could      layers where different emotions happened at the
not distinguish between sadness and dissatisfac-        same time, at different levels of intensity, for
tion. However, such an extension of the set of          example, a high level of Happiness together with
emoji would require further careful design and          low levels of Boredom, Confusion or Sadness.
validation, and is likely to necessitate creating            Lastly, our study confirmed that while ver-
visually very distinct emoji even for similar emo-      bal methods can be used to describe an experi-
tions. For example, we might propose emoji              ence, they are not optimal to capture emotions
depicting Thumbs up and Thumbs Down to illus-           (Desmet, Overbeeke, & Tax, 2001; Mehrabian,
trate satisfaction/dissatisfaction in order to          1995; Reijneveld et al., 2003). Indeed, during
maintain a reliable distinction from sadness.           the interview participants often talked about
Further work is needed to determine the extent          their general experience with the sandbox rather
to which using a wider range of emoji would             than their emotional state during the interac-
adversely affect the method’s reliability as well       tion. The emoji-based survey can provide a clear
as one of its main strengths, its speed.                indication of which emotions were felt, which is

20                           Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
something interviews can struggle to do. How-          expressions differently according to their cul-
ever, the interviews helped to understand why          tural background (Jack, Caldara, & Schyns,
emotions were felt. Thus, we suggest than emoji        2012; Park et al., 2013). Thus, further studies
and other traditional methods such as inter-           are needed to investigate emoji validity across
views are used together so that they can comple-       visitors with different cultural and social back-
ment each other. Data from the interviews can          grounds.                                      END
be mapped to the emoji selected. These com-
bined methods can provide a rich understanding         ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
of which emotional states were felt and why.
                                                       This work is part of EPSRC Centre for Digital
CONCLUSION AND FUTURE WORK                             Entertainment (grant EP/G037736/1) and the
                                                       National Trust funded research project to investigate
     In this paper we tested the ability of emoji      next-generation cultural heritage user experiences. A
                                                       special thanks go to Xindan Wang who helped col-
as a tool to evaluate visitors’ emotional experi-
                                                       lecting data. Daniel J Finnegan and Malcolm Holley
ence. While other non-verbal self-report meth-         who helped to develop and installing the sandbox.
ods have been used to evaluate emotions, this is       Eamonn O’Neill’s research is partly funded by CAM-
the first tool of this kind designed specifically to   ERA, the RCUK Centre for the Analysis of Motion,
measure emotions elicited by museum experi-            Entertainment Research and Applications (EP/
ences. Before this study, there was little formal      M023281/1).
research to support whether emoji can effec-
                                                       REFERENCES
tively capture visitors’ emotions. Hence, we
designed a set of 9 emoji: Happy, Sad, Angry,
                                                       Agarwal, A., & Meyer, A. (2009). Beyond usability.
Confused, Achieved, Inspired, Bored, Tired, and            In Proceedings of the 27th International Conference
Socially Engaged. We validated their meaning               Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in
and confirmed that these emoji were indeed able            Computing Systems – CHI EA ’09, 2919. New
to capture the relevant emotions in a controlled           York, NY: ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.
environment. This helped us to test the method,            1145/1520340.1520420
ensuring it was rapid, intuitive, and effective        Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A.
                                                           (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-
before deploying it in the field. Our emoji are
                                                           creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or
freely available under Creative Commons
                                                           regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6),
license from: https://drive.google.com/open?               779–806.
xml:id=1onitMwbFF9echTCBb0QHBBcqil                     Barbieri, F., Ronzano, F., & Saggion, H. (2016).
vzwp1w                                                     What does this emoji mean? A vector space skip-
     We are currently developing an online sur-            gram model for twitter emojis. In Proceedings of
vey editor that will allow museums to create               the Tenth International Conference on Language
their own emoji survey. We intend this tool to             Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2016). ELRA
                                                           (European Language Resources Association).
be generalizable and applicable to a wide range
                                                           Accessed January 08, 2019. Retrieved from
of museums and visitors. While emoji are a glo-
                                                           https://repositori.upf.edu/handle/10230/33776?
bal phenomenon and are often interpreted in                locale-attribute=en
the same way across different cultures and lan-        Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. M.
guages (Barbieri, Ronzano, & Saggion, 2016),               (2008). Handbook of emotions. New York, NY:
people may still understand images and facial              Guilford Publications.

Daniela De Angeli, Ryan M. Kelly, Eamonn O’Neill                                                            21
CURATOR: THE MUSEUM JOURNAL

Benedek, J., & Miner, T. (2002). Measuring                  Damala, A., Schuchert, T., Rodriguez, I., Moragues,
    desirability: New methods for evaluating                    J., Gilleade, K., & Stojanovic, N. (2013).
    desirability in a usability lab setting. In                 Exploring the affective museum visiting
    Proceedings of Usability Professionals’ Association.        experience: Adaptive augmented reality (A 2 R)
    Accessed March 01, 2015. Retrieved from                     and cultural heritage. International Journal of
    http://sites.google.com/site/danzinde/Desirab               Heritage in the Digital Era, 2(1), 117–42.
    ilityToolkit.pdf                                        Desmet, P. (2005). Measuring emotion:
Borod, J. C. (2000). The neuropsychology of emotion.            Development and application of an instrument
    Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.                        to measure emotional responses to products,
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring                 January, 111–23. Accessed October 24, 2015.
    emotion: The self-assessment Manikin and the                Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?
    semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy          xml:id=1139008.1139023
    and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.              Desmet, P., Overbeeke, K., & Tax, S. (2001).
Brent Ritchie, J. R., Wing Sun Tung, V., & Ritchie,             Designing products with added emotional value:
    R. J. B. (2011). Tourism experience management              Development and appllcation of an approach for
    research. International Journal of Contemporary             research through design. The Design Journal, 4
    Hospitality Management, 23(4), 419–38.                      (1), 32–47.
Brida, J. G., Disegna, M., & Scuderi, R. (2014). The        Dickinson, G. (2018). Those smiley feedback
    visitors’ perception of authenticity at the                 buttons do actually work – and they are changing
    museums: Archaeology versus modern art.                     the way we travel. The Telegraph, 2018.
    Current Issues in Tourism, 17(6), 518–38.                   Accessed May 11, 2018. Retrieved from https://
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13683500.2012.                      www.telegraph.co.uk/travel/news/happyornot-
    742042                                                      smiley-buttons-at-airports/
Bryce, D., Curran, R., O’Gorman, K., & Taheri, B.           Dindar, S. (2015). Silvia Filippini-Fantoni Q&A:
    (2014). Visitors’ engagement and authenticity:              How the indianapolis museum of art uses visitor
    Japanese heritage consumption. Tourism                      feedback. QuickTapSurvey. 2015. Accessed
    Management, 46, 571–81. https://doi.org/10.                 November 19, 2019. Retrieved from https://
    1016/j.tourman.2014.08.012                                  www.quicktapsurvey.com/blog/silvia-filippini-
Bryce, J. (2001). The technological transformation of           fantoni-qa-how-the-indianapolis-museum-of-
    leisure. Social Science Computer Review, 19(1), 7–          art-uses-visitor-feedback/
    16.                                                     Fane, J., MacDougall, C., Jovanovic, J., Redmond,
Caicedo, D. G., & Van Beuzekom, M. (2006). ‘How                 G., & Gibbs, L. (2018). Exploring the use of
    do you feel?’ An assessment of existing tools for           emoji as a visual research method for eliciting
    the measurement of emotions and their                       young children’s voices in childhood research.
    application in consumer products. Delft                     Early Child Development and Care, 188(3), 359–
    University of Technology. Accessed November                 74.
    10, 2015. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.          Foster, H. (2008). Evaluation toolkit for museum
    psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.506.                     practitioners. Accessed November 12, 2015.
    8025                                                        Retrieved from http://visitors.org.uk/wp-conte
Chen, C.-F., & Chen, F.-S. (2010). Experience                   nt/uploads/2014/08/ShareSE_Evaltoolkit.pdf
    quality, perceived value, satisfaction and              Fox, E. (2008). Emotion science : Cognitive and
    behavioral intentions for heritage tourists.                neuroscientific approaches to understanding human
    Tourism Management, 31(1), 29–35.                           emotions. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Del Chiappa, G., Andreu, L., & Gallarza, M. G.              Galani, A., Maxwell, D., Mazel, A., & Sharpe, K.
    (2014). Emotions and visitors’ satisfaction at a            (2011). Situating cultural technologies outdoors:
    museum. International Journal of Culture,                   Design methods for mobile interpretation of
    Tourism and Hospitality Research, 8(4), 420–31.             rock art in rural Britain. In J. Trant &

22                               Article: Beyond Happy-or-Not: Using Emoji to Capture Visitors’ Emotional Experience
You can also read