Discriminating between chronic fatigue syndrome and depression : a cognitive analysis - Core

Page created by Brenda Lopez
 
CONTINUE READING
Psychological Medicine, 2001, 31, 469–479.              Printed in the United Kingdom
" 2001 Cambridge University Press

     Discriminating between chronic fatigue syndrome
           and depression : a cognitive analysis
                                RONA M O S S - M O R R I S"    KEITH J. P E T R I E
   From the Health Psychology Research Group, Faculty of Medical and Health Science, The University of
                                   Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

ABSTRACT
Background. Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) and depression share a number of common
symptoms and the majority of CFS patients meet lifetime criteria for depression. While cognitive
factors seem key to the maintenance of CFS and depression, little is known about how the cognitive
characteristics differ in the two conditions.
Methods. Fifty-three CFS patients were compared with 20 depressed patients and 38 healthy
controls on perceptions of their health, illness attributions, self-esteem, cognitive distortions of
general and somatic events, symptoms of distress and coping. A 6 month follow-up was also
conducted to determine the stability of these factors and to investigate whether CFS-related
cognitions predict ongoing disability and fatigue in this disorder.
Results. Between-group analyses confirmed that the depressed group was distinguished by low
self-esteem, the propensity to make cognitive distortions across all situations, and to attribute their
illness to psychological factors. In contrast, the CFS patients were characterized by low ratings of
their current health status, a strong illness identity, external attributions for their illness, and
distortions in thinking that were specific to somatic experiences. They were also more likely than
depressed patients to cope with their illness by limiting stress and activity levels. These CFS-related
cognitions and behaviours were associated with disability and fatigue 6 months later.
Conclusions. CFS and depression can be distinguished by unique cognitive styles characteristic of
each condition. The documented cognitive profile of the CFS patients provides support for the
current cognitive behavioural models of the illness.

                                                                     no clear distinguishing organic agent has been
INTRODUCTION
                                                                     found (Evengard et al. 1999). The lack of a
Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is a disorder of                      defining pathophysiology has led others to
uncertain aetiology, characterized by de-                            suggest that CFS is a somatic form of depression
bilitating fatigue which has been present for at                     (Manu et al. 1993).
least 6 months (Fukuda et al. 1994). The                                The argument for CFS as a form of depression
idiopathic nature of CFS has led to an ongoing                       is based on the significant overlap between these
debate as to the organic or functional aetiology                     two disorders. CFS shares a number of cardinal
of this disorder. Those favouring an organic                         symptoms with depressive disorders including
cause have linked CFS to viral pathogens, muscle                     extreme fatigue, sleep disturbance, diminished
abnormalities, and immunological and neuro-                          concentration, and problems with memory. In
logical changes. While a number of abnor-                            fact, up to 85 % of CFS patients report depressed
malities have been documented in CFS groups,                         mood as a key symptom (Komaroff & Buchwald,
                                                                     1991) and around two-thirds of patients meet
  " Address for correspondence : Dr Rona Moss-Morris, Health         lifetime criteria for depression (Abbey, 1996).
Psychology Research Group, The Faculty of Medical and Health
Science, The University of Auckland, Private Bag 92 019, Auckland,
                                                                     Studies comparing CFS patients to patients with
New Zealand.                                                         a range of medical illnesses, including rheu-
                                                                 469
470                                 R. Moss-Morris and K. J. Petrie

matoid arthritis, multiple sclerosis, neuro-          MacLeod, 1994). Thus, if CFS is distinct from
muscular disorders and myopathies have in-            depression, CFS patients should not report these
variably reported significantly higher rates of       classic errors of thinking.
depression in CFS (Wessely & Powell, 1989 ;              Our previous work has shown that CFS
Katon et al. 1991 ; Wood et al. 1991 ; Pepper et      patients appear to have a particularly negative
al. 1993 ; Johnson et al. 1996).                      view of their illness and to be less likely to
   Despite this overlap, there is evidence that       attribute their illness to internal factors when
relevant differences may exist. Studies of the        compared to patients with other chronic medical
neuroendocrine system have demonstrated sig-          conditions (Weinman et al. 1996). They also
nificantly different physiological abnormalities      tend to make catastrophic interpretations of the
in each of these disorders, although results have     consequences of their illness (Petrie et al. 1995).
not always been consistent (Schwartz et al.           Accordingly, we hypothesized that while de-
1994 ; Cleare et al. 1995 ; Goldstein et al. 1995 ;   pressed patients ’ schema are dominated by
Fischler et al. 1996). In addition, unlike de-        negative self-perceptions, CFS patients ’ per-
pressed patients, CFS patients do not seem to         ceptions are dominated by their views of
respond to antidepressant medication (Natelson        themselves as seriously physically ill people.
et al. 1996 ; Vercoulen et al. 1996).                 Therefore, we anticipated that while depressed
   A closer look at the psychological symp-           patients would demonstrate negative distorted
tomatology of these groups shows that CFS             thinking in a range of situations, CFS patients
patients consistently report lower mean scores        would only demonstrate these distortions when
on depression inventories, although their scores      interpreting somatic information.
are still within the depressed range (Hickie et al.      Comparing CFS and depressed patients’ cog-
1990 ; Johnson et al. 1996 ; Wessely & Powell,        nitions may not only help in differentiating the
1989). These differences are largely accounted        phenomenology of the disorders. Recent models
for by depressed patients scoring significantly       of CFS suggest that patients’ cognitions play an
higher on the self-reproach or cognitive              important role in maintaining this disorder
symptoms including feelings of guilt, low self-       (Wessely et al. 1991 ; Surawy et al. 1995). In
esteem, and suicidal ideation (Powell et al. 1990 ;   particular, a precipitating event such as a virus is
Johnson et al. 1996).                                 seen to trigger a cycle of responses whereby
   A more detailed investigation of the cognitions    patients interpret ongoing symptoms as signs of
of the two groups may provide another avenue          physical illness. This results in limiting activity
to determine whether depression and CFS are           levels and the development of cognitions and
indeed distinct. Not only do cognitive symptoms       behavioural responses which are thought to
form part of the DSM-IV diagnosis of depression       perpetuate the level of disability and fatigue
but cognitive theories of depression suggest that     experienced by these patients. To date, the
specific thought processes define the disorder        nature of CFS patients’ cognitions have largely
(Beck, 1964). The characteristic cognitive profile    been documented through clinical observation
of depressed patients includes a negative self-       and there is little empirical evidence to support
concept, and themes of loss, abandonment and          the unique nature of these belief structures.
defeat (Beck, 1964). This negative self-schema is        Thus, the aims of the current study were as
thought to generate a series of distortions in        follows. First, to compare the cognitions and
thinking such as selective abstraction – focusing     behavioural responses of CFS and depressed
on the negative aspects of an experience,             patients to determine whether these disorders
catastrophising – expecting the worst outcome         have unique cognitive profiles. In particular, we
to occur, personalization – seeing oneself as         hypothesized that CFS patients’ negative cog-
responsible for negative events, and overgeneral-     nitions would be specific to health and illness,
izing – assuming the negative consequences of         that they would have a tendency to view their
one experience apply to another (Beck, 1964 ;         illness as organic, and to limit their activity
Beck et al. 1978). These distorted thought            levels in order to cope with their illness.
processes and negative self-concept distinguish       Secondly, to investigate whether this cognitive
depressed patients from patients with other           profile remains stable over time and thirdly to
forms of psychopathology (Mathews &                   determine whether the CFS-related cognitions
Cognitions in chronic fatigue syndrome                                    471

and behavioural responses predict ongoing                             The CIDI-Auto has demonstrated procedural
disability and fatigue in this disorder.                              validity against expert clinical diagnoses (Peters
                                                                      & Andrews, 1993) and the items have good
METHOD                                                                reliability (Wittchen, 1994).
Participants                                                             Fourteen (26 %) of the 53 subjects had a
                                                                      concurrent DSM-III-R diagnosis of major de-
Because of the phenomenological overlap be-
                                                                      pression or dysthymia, similar to a number of
tween depression and CFS, an important com-
                                                                      other studies which have identified concurrent
ponent of this study was ensuring the ap-
                                                                      depression in CFS (Katon et al. 1991 ; Bom-
propriate allocation to groups. Standardized
                                                                      bardier & Buchwald, 1995). In this study the
diagnostic interviews were used in conjunction
                                                                      estimate of depression may have been con-
with self-report measures to confirm patient
                                                                      servative, as the CIDI excludes somatic symp-
diagnoses. The CFS group was also divided
                                                                      toms from diagnostic criteria if they have been
into those with and those without a concurrent
                                                                      attributed to physical illness by a medical
diagnosis of depression, to avoid the possible
                                                                      practitioner. As the role of somatic symptoms in
confounding of dual diagnoses.
                                                                      diagnosing depression in CFS is a controversial
   CFS group                                                          issue (Ray, 1991), the conservative estimate was
The CFS patients were recruited from a general                        deemed preferable. The final sample consisted of
medical practice specializing in the treatment of                     39 CFS patients without depression (CFS) and
CFS. Patients aged between 18–65 who were                             14 CFS patients with a concurrent diagnosis of
diagnosed by the general practitioner as having                       depression (CFS-depressed). Fifty-one per cent
CFS, and who provided informed consent to                             of this group belonged to a CFS support group.
participate in the study were interviewed by one                      The demographic details of the two CFS groups
of the investigators. Of the 65 patients who                          together with the comparison groups are pre-
provided informed consent, eight did not meet                         sented in Table 1. Analysis of variance
criteria, and four dropped out of the study.                          (ANOVA) and chi-square tests were used to
   The diagnostic interview assessed whether                          confirm that the groups were comparable with
patients met current research criteria for CFS                        regard to demographic features.
(Fukuda et al. 1994) and whether they had a
concurrent diagnosis of depression. The inter-                          Depressed group
viewer-administered computerized version of the                       Inclusion criteria for the depressed group,
Composite International Diagnostic Interview                          included a current primary DSM-III-R diagnosis
(CIDI-Auto) (Health, WHO, 1993) was used to                           of major depression or dysthymia, a Beck
diagnose depression as it has been recommended                        Depression Inventory (Beck, 1978) score  10,
for use in CFS research (Fukuda et al. 1994).                         no evidence of psychosis, organicity, addiction,

                                 Table 1. Characteristics of group participants
                                    CFS                  CFS-depressed                Depressed      Healthy controls
                                  (N l 39)                 (N l 14)                   (N l 20)          (N l 38)

Gender, % Women (N)              82 (32)                  85n7 (12)                   65 (13)          74 (28)
                                                                      χ# l 2n97, P l 0n40
Age, mean (..)                 43n3 (12n7)              47 (12n07)                   38n8 (12n5)     44n9 (10n8)
                                                               F l 1n64 ; df l 3, 107 ; P l 0n18
Tertiary qualified, % (N)        51 (20)                  55 (11)                     43 (6)           45 (17)
                                                                      χ# l 4n1 ; P l 0n90
BDI scores, mean (..)          11n68 (5n45)             14n12 (6n02)                22n47 (6n7)       4n00 (2n47)
                                                             F l 61n74 ; df l 3, 107 ; P 0n001
Length of illness, mean (..)    8n9 (8n7)                7n8 (10n1)                  14n1 (14n1)
                                                               F l 2n11 ; df l 2, 70 ; P l 0n13
Unemployed*, % (N)               59 (23)                  57 (8)                     30 (6)
                                                                      χ# l 4n7, P l 0n09

                                                    * Unemployment due to illness.
472                                             R. Moss-Morris and K. J. Petrie

                                                                and\or a current chronic illness. A score  10
      Table 2. Current and past diagnoses of
                                                                on the BDI is indicative of possible depressive
         depression across illness groups
                                                                disorder (Beck et al. 1978). Eight of the 46
                       CFS        CFS-depressed    Depressed    healthy volunteers scored  10 on the BDI
                     (N l 39)       (N l 14)       (N l 20)     resulting in a final sample of 38 healthy controls.
                      % (N)          % (N)          % (N)
                                                                Measures
Current diagnoses
  No diagnosis       100 (53)                                   Two types of measures were included in this
  Major depression                  85n7 (12)       90 (18)     study, those measuring cognitive behavioural
  Dysthymia                         14n2 (2)        10 (2)
                                                                factors and those assessing the specific symptom
Past history
  No history          42n1 (16)     14n3 (2)        10 (92)     profiles and level of disability of the groups.
  Major depression    50 (19)       78n6 (11)       85 (17)
  Dysthymia            7n9 (3)       7n1 (1)         5 (1)         The Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1995)
                                                                This was used to operationalize negative self-
                                                                schema. It is the most frequently used measure
or chronic physical illness. To be comparable                   of self-esteem and has good internal reliability,
with the CFS group, the depressed patients                      test–retest reliability, and convergent and dis-
needed to be receiving out-patient rather than                  criminate validity (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991).
in-patient treatment and to be between 18- and                  High scores represent greater self-esteem.
65-years old. Patients were recruited through
private psychologists and community mental                        Self-rated health
health centres. Subjects who provided informed                  Self-rated health was used as a measure of illness
consent were interviewed with the CIDI to                       schema. It is a widely used single-item measure
confirm that they met current criteria for a                    of individuals’ perceptions of themselves as
depressive disorder. Three of 23 patients inter-                healthy or sick people (Johnston et al. 1995).
viewed did not meet inclusion criteria. The final               The Short Form Health Survey version of the
primary depressed group consisted of two                        scale (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992) was used in the
patients with dysthymia and 18 with major                       current study, where low scores represent more
depression.                                                     healthy perceptions.
   The CIDI data from all three patient groups
are presented in Table 2. Most of the CFS-                         Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ,
depressed and primary depressed patients met                       Weinman et al. 1996)
criteria for major depression with only two                     Two subscales of the IPQ were used to measure
patients in each group meeting criteria for                     dimensions of patients’ illness beliefs. The illness
dysthymia. The majority of the patients had a                   identity subscale measures the number of so-
history of depressive disorders, including 68 %                 matic symptoms patients associate with their
of the non-depressed CFS patients.                              illness. The identity scale in this study consisted
   Table 1 shows that the mean BDI score for                    of 20 symptoms, including the 12 core IPQ items
the primary depressed group was in the mod-                     which are symptoms commonly experienced by
erately depressed range (Kendall et al. 1987) and               the general population and eight additional
was substantially higher than the scores for the                symptoms commonly reported by depressed and
two CFS groups. The means for the CFS groups                    CFS patients (Moss-Morris et al. 1996). The
both fell within the mildly depressed range,                    causal subscale measures patients’ beliefs about
although the score for the CFS-depressed group                  the causes of their illness. For the purpose of this
was higher than that of the CFS group.                          study patients were presented with a list of 18
                                                                possible causes of their illness. Half of these
   Control group                                                were psychological causes such as ‘recent stress-
The healthy controls were recruited through the                 ful events’ and ‘my mental attitude ’ while the
university and the community on the basis that                  other half were physical causes such as ‘a virus’,
they matched the patient groups as far as possible              ‘immune dysfunction ’ or ‘a neurochemical im-
for the demographic features identified in Table                balance ’. Subjects were asked to assign a
1. Exclusion criteria were a BDI score  10, a                  percentage to each factor, so that the overall
current or past history of depression or CFS,                   assignment of causes equalled 100 %. If they
Cognitions in chronic fatigue syndrome                               473

believed the factor was unrelated to their illness
they were asked to assign the factor 0. Two                 Symptoms of distress and fatigue
scores were computed, one which measured the             Two subscales of the Mental Health Inventory
percentage patients assigned to physical causes          (Viet & Ware, 1983) that discriminate between
and the other the percentage assigned to psycho-         the somatic and affective components of distress
logical causes.                                          were used to substantiate the symptom dif-
                                                         ferences between the four subject groups. The
   Cognitive Errors Questionnaire-Revised                MHI-5 (Ware et al. 1993) is a measure of the
   (CEQ-R, Moss-Morris & Petrie, 1997)                   affective dimensions of anxiety, depression, and
The CEQ-R was devised specifically to assess             psychological well-being. It has demonstrated
cognitive distortions in this study. The CEQ-R           high internal consistency (Ware et al. 1993), and
is divided into two subscales : the 12-item              is a valid measure of psychiatric dysfunction
General CEQ-R and the 9-item Somatic CEQ-                (Berwick et al. 1991). The Vitality Scale (Ware et
R. Each of the 21 items describes an everyday            al. 1993) is a 4-item measure of energy and
situation involving either work, recreation, or          fatigue. The vitality scale has a sound record of
family experiences. The general items focus on           empirical validity, item discriminant validity,
interpersonal experiences, while the somatic ones        and scale reliability (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992).
include the experience of common symptoms,               Both of these subscales are scored so that high
such as fatigue, aches and pains, and muscle             scores indicate greater psychological well-being
weakness. These vignettes are followed by a              and vitality.
thought that a person in that situation may
have. For example, one of the general items                 Disability
states ; ‘ You hand in a report to your boss that        The Sickness Impact Profile (SIP, Gilson et al.
has taken you four hours to write. Your boss,            1979) was used as a measure of sickness related
however, doesn’t say anything about it. You              disability. The SIP has been found to be a
think to yourself, ‘‘ (S)he must think I did a lousy     convincing measure of functional status in a
job ’’ ’. An example of a somatic item is ‘You           range of chronic illnesses (De Bruin et al. 1992).
have been feeling very weak and tired of late, but       The questionnaire’s validity, test–retest re-
have continued to work. Although you got quite           liability and internal consistency are well es-
a bit done today, you finished work early because        tablished (De Bruin et al. 1992). Because the
you were feeling particularly exhausted. You             scale is very lengthy and the reliability and basic
think to yourself, ‘‘What a terrible day. It seems       construct validity appear to be unaffected by
like I can’t get anything done ’’ ’. The thoughts        administering only selected subscale categories
are worded to represent the cognitive errors of          (Bergner et al. 1981), six subscales were includ-
catastrophizing, overgeneralizing, personali-            ed in the current study. Five of these
zation, and selective abstraction (Beck et al.           subscales – social interaction, alertness behav-
1978). An in depth analysis of this questionnaire        iour, sleep and rest, home management, and
which included the current samples showed that           recreational pastimes – have been shown to
the subscales have high internal consistency             most clearly represent CFS-related disability
across groups and good test–retest reliability           (Schweitzer et al. 1995). These scores were
over 6 months (Moss-Morris & Petrie, 1997).              summed and divided by five to produce a SIP
                                                         dysfunction score out of 100. We also included
   Coping                                                the SIP work subscale as a separate variable as
A three-item measure of limiting coping was              it was only relevant to the percentage of people
included in the questionnaire (Sharpe et al.             who were working before their illness.
1992). Patients were asked the extent to which
they limited exercise, activity, and stress in order     Procedure
to cope with their illness on a 4-point scale rated      Items determining the demographic features of
from ‘I usually didn ’t do this at all ‘to ’ I usually   the groups and the self-report measures were
did this a lot ’. These items have been shown to         compiled into a single questionnaire. The IPQ
predict ongoing disability in patients with              subscales, SIP, and limiting coping scale, which
chronic fatigue (Sharpe et al. 1992).                    are specific to illness, were included only in the
474                                            R. Moss-Morris and K. J. Petrie

patient questionnaires. All participants provided                     one and P 0n007 (0n05\7) at time two. To
informed consent before they were given a copy                        ascertain whether covariates should be included
of the questionnaire and a self-addressed return                      in these equations, correlations were computed
envelope. The patient groups also completed a                         between age, gender, level of education, marital
diagnostic interview to determine their eligibility                   status, length of illness, and the various de-
for the study. All subjects were asked to complete                    pendent variables (DVs). There were no sig-
the questionnaire within a week and to post it                        nificant correlations, and as there were no
back to the investigators.                                            significant differences between the groups on
   Six months after completing the initial ques-                      these factors, no covariates were included. Post-
tionnaire, participants in the patient groups                         hoc analyses were conducted with Tukey’s test.
completed a shortened version of the ques-                            Stepwise multiple regression was used to in-
tionnaire included the MHI-5, Vitality, SIP,                          vestigate the relationships between the cognitive
Self-Rated Health, CEQ-R and Illness Identity                         behavioural factors and ongoing disability and
scales. Two of the depressed patients could not                       fatigue in the CFS group.
be contacted and one depressed patient and two
                                                                      Symptoms and disability across groups
CFS patients did not return the questionnaire.
The overall response rate of the follow-up                            A summary of the ANOVA results from time
questionnaire was 85 % for the depressed group,                       one are presented in Table 3. The first stage of
95 % for the CFS group and 100 % for the CFS-                         the analyses involved clarifying the validity of
depressed group.                                                      the four groups by comparing their scores on the
                                                                      MHI-5 and Vitality scales. Vitality scores were
                                                                      significantly different between groups. Post-hoc
RESULTS
                                                                      tests suggested that this difference was due to the
Data analysis was performed on the SPSS                               higher scores for the control group when
version 8.0 computer software program                                 compared to all three patient groups, who had
(Norus) is, 1993). Results of the evaluation of the                   equivalent scores. MHI-5 scores also differed,
assumptions of normality of sampling dis-                             with post-hoc analyses suggesting a number of
tributions, linearity, and homogeneity of vari-                       significant contrasts between the four groups.
ance were satisfactory for all of the variables                       As with the Vitality scale, healthy controls
and there were no obvious outliers. A series of                       scored significantly higher on the MHI-5 than
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) were used to                             all the patient groups. CFS non-depressed
test the hypotheses regarding differences between                     patients also scored significantly higher than
the groups. Due to the large number of planned                        both the depressed groups and CFS-depressed
analyses, a Bonferonni adjusted alpha was used                        patients scored higher than primary depressed
in these equations ; P 0n0045 (0n05\11) at time                       patients.
                     Table 3. Analysis of variance of the self-report measures at Time 1
                                CFS                CFS-depressed          Depressed               Controls
                              Mean (..)           Mean (..)           Mean (..)            Mean (..)    df       F

Vitality                     26n54 (18n54)†         20n71 (18n69)†       26n50 (14n96)†      68n69 (15n14)     3,107   55n46*
MHI-5                        68n10 (15n60)†‡        47n43 (3n75)†‡§      36n40 (15n57)†      79n58 (10n26)     3,107   51n05*
SIP dysfunction              42n17 (13n79)          47n59 (14n14)        44n82 (13n82)                         2,67     0n83
SIP work                     58n48 (17n48)          59n69 (33n64)        52n29 (24n94)                         2,54
Self-esteem                  31n25 (4n73)           27n71 (5n67)†§       24n86 (5n76)†§      33n11 (4n12)      3,107   14n85*
Self-rated health             4n05 (0n79)†‡          4n00 (0n96)†‡        3n15 (1n14)         1n76 (0n68)      3,107   52n86*
General CEQ-R                21n69 (8n99)‡          26n03 (7n79)‡        35n15 (11n85)†      21n47 (6n58)      3,107   10n66*
Somatic CEQ-R                21n79 (8n16)†          21n36 (6n22)†        23n24 (9n1)†        14n83 (4n65)      3,107   12n97*
Somatic illness identity     17n59 (2n20)‡          18n21 (2n33)‡        13n40 (3n94)                          2,70    17n82*
Psychological attributions   25n39 (24n99)‡         17n86 (21n81)‡       62n13 (23n42)                         2,68    19n31*
Physical attributions        62n11 (28n62)‡         77n86 (21n81)‡       15n90 (16n12)                         2,68    31n04*
Limiting coping               9n87 (1n80)‡           8n79 (2n61)‡         7n05 (2n48)                          2,70    11n04*

                                         *   P 0n0045.
                                         †   Significantly different from the control group.
                                         ‡   Significantly different from the depressed group.
                                         §   Significantly different from the CFS group.
Cognitions in chronic fatigue syndrome                             475

   The ANOVA of the SIP data showed there
were no differences in the level of sickness            Coping
related disability reported by all three groups.     As predicted, both CFS groups were more likely
Thus, in terms of presentation, CFS and de-          to deal with their illness by limiting activity and
pressed patients appear to report equivalent         stress than were the depressed group. There was
levels of energy loss and disability. Depressed      no difference on this measure between the CFS
patients however report higher levels of negative    groups.
mood than do CFS patients.
                                                     Stability of cognitions over 6 months
Cognitive behavioural factors across groups          Identical analyses were conducted on the
  Self-esteem                                        measures included in the second questionnaire.
Comparisons confirmed that the two depressed         Table 4 shows that there was almost no change
groups had significantly lower self-esteem than      in the pattern of results. The three groups were
the CFS non-depressed and healthy control            still indistinguishable on measures of vitality
groups. There were no significant differences        and sickness-related disability. Both the de-
between the two depressed groups, or between         pressed groups scored lower on psychological
the CFS non-depressed and healthy control            well-being (MHI-5) than did the CFS group.
groups.                                              However, unlike at time one, the CFS-depressed
                                                     group did not score significantly higher than the
  Self-rated health                                  depressed group on this scale. Both of these
The ANOVA for self-rated health showed that          groups scored higher on this scale than at time
both CFS groups rated themselves as signifi-         one, suggesting that their mood had improved
cantly less healthy than depressed patients and      over the 6-month period.
controls. In turn, the depressed group also rated       The prototype of cognitive differences was
themselves as less physically healthy than           unchanged. The CFS groups rated themselves as
controls.                                            less healthy and experiencing more physical
                                                     symptoms as part of their illness. The depressed
   Illness beliefs                                   group scored higher on the General CEQ-R but
Both CFS groups showed a significantly stronger      not the Somatic CEQ-R. Interestingly, although
somatic illness identity as they endorsed a higher   the groups showed improvements in their levels
number of physical symptoms to their illness         of disability and symptom reports over the 6-
than did depressed patients. There was no            month period, within-group analyses showed
differences between the two CFS groups on this       that there were no significant changes in scores
measure. Similarly, the two CFS groups made          on the CEQ-R subscales in either of the groups.
significantly more physical attributions and         For the CFS group the results of the paired
significantly fewer psychological attributions for   samples t tests on Somatic CEQ-R were t l 0n83
their illness than did depressed patients.           (50) P l 0n41 and on the General CEQ-R, t l
                                                     k0n74 (50) P l 0n46. For the depressed group
   Cognitive distortions                             the results on the Somatic CEQ-R were t l
To assess whether level and type of cognitive        k0n16 (16) P l 0n88 and on the General CEQ-
distortion varied with group membership, the         R, t l 0n47 (16) P l 0n64.
two subscales of the CEQ-R were entered into
two separate ANOVA equations. There was a            Cognitive-behavioural variables and ongoing
highly significant difference for group on both      disability and fatigue in CFS
subscales. Post-hoc analyses revealed that the       Three separate stepwise regression equations
depressed group scored significantly higher than     were used to investigate the relationships be-
all the other three groups on the General CEQ-       tween the CFS-related cognitive-behavioural
R. There was no difference between the two CFS       factors measured at time one, and disability and
groups and healthy controls on this subscale.        fatigue measured 6 months later in the CFS
However, all three patient groups scored signifi-    group. The dependent variables included the
cantly higher on the somatic CEQ-R when              Vitality scale as a measure of fatigue and the two
compared with healthy controls.
476                                         R. Moss-Morris and K. J. Petrie

             Table 4. Analysis of variance of the self-report measures at 6 months follow-up
                              CFS                 CFS-depressed             Depressed
                            Mean (..)            Mean (..)              Mean (..)      df         F

Vitality                   30n14 (21n97)           30n36 (18n76)           41n18 (18n50)     2,65      1n82
MHI-5                      69n62 (13n77)           56n57 (15n11)‡          52n00 (25n38)‡    2,65      6n85*
SIP dysfunction            37n03 (17n87)           37n50 (17n05)           26n93 (16n21)     2,65      2n27
SIP work                   46n95 (26n18)           34n80 (28n32)           41n03 (25n71)     2,53      1n04
Self-rated health           3n59 (0n83)†            3n71 (0n83)†            2n76 (0n90)      2,65      6n67*
General CEQ-R              22n84 (11n53)†          24n71 (9n29)†           33n29 (11n11)     2,65      5n34*
Somatic CEQ-R              21n65 (9n10)            22n50 (4n60)            23n49 (9n66)      2,65      0n27
Somatic illness identity   17n03 (4n54)†           17n00 (3n38)†            9n50 (6n10)      2,65     17n64*

                                       * P 0n007.
                                       † Significantly different from the depressed group.
                                       ‡ Significantly different from the CFS group.

SIP variables, general dysfunction and work                            The symptom profiles of each of the patient
disability.                                                         groups to some extent reflected the differences in
   In each of the equations, age and length of                      these self-schemas. The groups were clearly
illness were entered as independent variables                       delineated on the affective dimension of psycho-
together with physical attributions, somatic                        logical well-being with the primary depressed
illness identity, the Somatic CEQ-R, and limiting                   patients being the most distressed and the
coping. For vitality, both somatic illness identity                 healthy controls the least distressed. CFS
and age entered the equation and accounted for                      patients fell in between these groups with the
a unique 21 % of the variance, F (1,41) l 12n52,                    CFS-depressed patients reporting more distress
P 0n001). Somatic illness identity was the                          than the non-depressed ones. The three patient
strongest predictor (β l k0n48, P 0n001) fol-                       groups were indistinguishable on the somatic
lowed by age (β l 0n31, P 0n05). Somatic illness                    dimension of distress, with all three groups
identity (β l k0n61, P 0n001) and the Somatic                       reporting substantially lower levels of vitality
CEQ-R (β l 0n26, P 0n05) were the only sig-                         than healthy controls.
nificant predictors of dysfunction. Together                           With regard to illness beliefs, there were no
these variables accounted for 38 % of the                           differences between the two CFS groups, but
variance in dysfunction, F (1,41) l 24n60,                          substantial differences between CFS and de-
P 0n001. Somatic illness identity (β l 0n56,                        pression. In accordance with previous research,
P 0n001) was once again the first variable to                       CFS patients made significantly more physical
enter the equation for work dysfunction with                        attributions but fewer psychological attributions
limiting coping (β l 0n51, P 0n001) also a                          for their illness than did depressed patients
significant predictor. Taken together these two                     (Powell et al. 1990). Both CFS groups also
variables accounted for a unique 24 % of the                        ascribed a significantly larger number of somatic
variance in work dysfunction, F (1,32) l 10n32,                     symptoms to their illness than did the depressed
P 0n01.                                                             group. While this difference may merely reflect
                                                                    the physical status of the CFS patients there are
DISCUSSION
                                                                    arguments against this possibility. Earlier work
The results generally supported the hypothesis                      has shown that CFS patients ascribe a larger
that depressed patients’ self-schema are domin-                     number of somatic symptoms of their illness
ated by a negative view of the self, while CFS                      than do patients with other chronic medical
patients are primarily concerned with their poor                    conditions (Weinman et al. 1996). In the current
health. Both depressed groups had lower self-                       study, out of a total of 20 symptoms, the CFS
esteem than healthy controls and CFS non-                           patients on average reported experiencing 18 as
depressed patients, while both CFS groups rated                     a result of their illness. While some of these
themselves as significantly less healthy than the                   symptoms such as fatigue and muscle pain are
other two groups. The primary depressed                             characteristic of CFS, others such as pins-and-
group’s ratings of health lay midway between                        needles and sore eyes are symptoms commonly
those of controls and the CFS groups.                               experienced in the population as a whole. This
Cognitions in chronic fatigue syndrome                                                477

suggests that CFS patients may misattribute           which encourage rather than limit activity, may
common symptoms to their illness.                     help to alleviate some of the negative disabling
   CFS patients’ preoccupation with their             effects of the illness. It may be particularly
symptoms rather than with their sense of self is      important to explore alternative labels for
also reflected in the CEQ-R data. CFS patients        symptoms. For instance, some of the somatic
only displayed distorted thinking in situations       symptoms patients attribute to their CFS could
where they might experience symptoms and not          be re-labelled as signs of deconditioning, while
in general interpersonal situations. Concurrent       others such as headaches could be attributed to
depression did not appear to alter this pattern of    stress. It appears to be less important to alter
thinking. Only the depressed group could be           patients’ beliefs that their illness is caused by
distinguished from the healthy controls on the        physical factors. This factor may in fact be
tendency to distort the meaning of interpersonal      adaptive in that it may help to maintain a
situations. It is interesting that although the       healthy self-esteem.
depressed patients do not view their health as           Taken together, the results of this study are
negatively as the CFS group, and do not respond       consistent with a growing body of evidence that
to their illness by limiting stress and activity to   argues against CFS being a version of de-
the same extent, they have a similar tendency to      pression. It also provides some support for the
report somatic errors in thinking. For this group,    role cognitions and behaviour play in the
somatic errors may be reflective of a generalized     maintenance of CFS. However, a limitation of
tendency to see the world in a negative fashion.      the current study was the small sample sizes
   The differences between CFS and depression         and the convenience sampling method make it
were maintained over the 6 month period. There        difficult to generalize the results to all CFS and
were no significant changes in the scores on          depressed patients. It is also difficult to confirm
either the Somatic or General CEQ-R, sug-             the directional links of these models from this
gesting that these may be relatively stable           comparative study. It is possible that the
thought processes or ways of viewing the world.       cognitive profile in CFS is a reflection of the fact
It is also worth noting that CFS-depressed            that these patients have a serious ongoing
patients showed greater overlap with CFS              physical illness. However, two findings argue
patients than with primary depressed patients at      against this possibility. First, CFS patients
both time points.                                     appear to have even more negative views of their
   In support of the cognitive behavioural            illness than do patients with other chronic
models, the CFS-related cognitive behavioural         physical illnesses and these beliefs are associated
factors predicted ongoing disability and fatigue      with ongoing disability and fatigue. Secondly,
in this group, even when controlling for age and      although numerous physiological abnormalities
length of illness. Somatic illness identity was the   have been documented in CFS, these are seldom
most significant predictor of both ongoing            associated with the magnitude of the symptoms
dysfunction and fatigue. Coping by limiting           experienced by CFS patients (Wessely, 1996).
stress and activity was associated with work-         Prospective research in this area and studies that
related dysfunction, while somatic cognitive          compare CFS patients to patients with other
errors were associated with disability in the         physical illnesses could help to address the nature
other domains. Interestingly, consistent with         of CFS patients’ cognitions further.
previous work in this area (Moss-Morris et al.
1996 ; Heijmans & de Ridder, 1998), physical          This research was supported by the Health Research
attributions which are one of the key defining        Council of New Zealand.
features of the illness, failed to predict outcome
in this group. Changes in physical attributions
also fail to predict successful outcome in CFS        REFERENCES
patients following cognitive behavioural therapy      Abbey, S. E. (1996). Psychiatric diagnostic overlap in chronic fatigue
(Deale et al. 1998). These results suggest that         syndrome. In Chronic Fatigue Syndrome : An Integrated Approach
therapy approaches which assist CFS patients to         to Evaluation and Treatment (ed. M. A. Demitrack and S. E.
                                                        Abbey), pp. 49–71. Guilford Press : New York.
reinterpret the meaning of their symptoms in          Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression : II. Theory and therapy.
conjunction with exploring coping responses,            Archives of General Psychiatry 10, 561–571.
478                                                 R. Moss-Morris and K. J. Petrie

Beck, A. T. (1978). Depression Inventory. Centre for Cognitive               Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding the
  Therapy : Philadelphia.                                                    use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. & Emery, G. (1978). Cognitive          Research 11, 289–299.
  Therapy of Depression : A Treatment Manual. AT Beck :                    Komaroff, A. L. & Buchwald, D. (1991). Symptoms and signs of
  Philadelphia.                                                              chronic fatigue syndrome. Reviews of Infectious Diseases 13,
Bergner, M., Bobbitt, R. A., Carter, W. B. & Gilson, B. S. (1981).           S8–S11.
  The Sickness Impact Profile : validation of a health status              Manu, P., Lane, T. J. & Mathews, D. A. (1993). Chronic fatigue and
  measure . Medical Care 19, 787–805                                         chronic fatigue syndrome : Clinical epidemiology and aetiological
Berwick, D. M., Murphy, J. M., Goldman, P. A., Ware, J. E.,                  classification. In Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Ciba Foundation
  Barsky, A. J. & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance of a five-            Symposium, Vol. 173 (ed. E. Bock and J. Whelan), pp. 23–42.
  item mental health screening test. Medical Care 29, 169–176.               John Wiley : Chichester.
Blascovich, J. & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self esteem. In            Mathews, A. & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to
  Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes (ed.            emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology 45,
  J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver and L. S. Wrightsman), pp. 115–128.           25–50.
  Harcourt Brace Jovanovich : San Diego.                                   Moss-Morris, R. & Petrie, K. J. (1997). Cognitive distortions of
Bombardier, C. H. & Buchwald, D. (1995). Outcome and prognosis               somatic experiences – revision and validation of a measure. Journal
  in patients with chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome.             of Psychosomatic Research 43, 293–306.
  Archives of Internal Medicine 155, 2105–2110.                            Moss-Morris, R., Petrie, K. J. & Weinman, J. (1996). Functioning in
Cleare, A. J., Bearn, J., Allain, T., McGregor, A., Wessely, S.,             chronic fatigue syndrome : do illness perceptions play a regulatory
  Murray, R. M. & O ’Keane, V. (1995). Contrasting neuroendocrine            role. British Journal of Health Psychology 1, 15–25.
  responses in depression and chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of         Natelson, B. H., Cheu, J., Pareja, J., Ellis, S. P., Policastro, T. &
  Affective Disorders 34, 283–289.                                           Findley, T. W. (1996). Randomized, double blind, controlled
De Bruin, A. F., De Witte, L. P., Stevens, F. & Diedericks, J. P. M.         placebo-phase in trial of low dose phenelzine in the chronic fatigue
  (1992). Sickness impact profile : the state of the art of a generic        syndrome. Psychopharmacology 124, 226–230.
  functional status measure. Social Science and Medicine 35,               Norus) is, M. J. (1993). SPSS for WindowsTM Professional Statistics
  1003–1014.                                                                 TM Release 6.0. SPSS Inc : Chicago.
Deale, A., Chalder, T. & Wessely, S. (1998). Illness beliefs and           Pepper, C., Krupp, L., Friedberg, F., Doscher, C. & Coyle, P. (1993).
  treatment outcome in chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of                  A comparison of neuropsychiatric characteristics in chronic fatigue
  Psychosomatic Research 45, 77–83.                                          syndrome, multiple sclerosis and major depression. Journal of
Evengard, B., Schacterle, R. S. & Komoroff, A. L. (1999). Chronic            Neuropsychiatry Clinical Neurosciences 5, 200–205.
  fatigue syndrome : new insights and old ignorance. Journal of            Peters, L. & Andrews, G. (1993). Reprocedural validity of the CIDI-
                                                                             Auto. Unit report 93\2. Clinical Research Unit for Anxiety
  Internal Medicine 246, 255–469.
                                                                             Disorders : Sydney.
Fischler, B., D’Haenen, H., Cluydts, R., Michiels, V., Demets, K.,
                                                                           Petrie, K. J., Moss-Morris, R. & Weinman, J. (1995). Catastrophic
  Bossuyt, A., Kaufman, L. & De Meirleir, K. (1996). Comparison
                                                                             beliefs and their implications in the chronic fatigue syndrome.
  of 99m Tc HMPAO SPECT scan between chronic fatigue
                                                                             Journal of Psychosomatic Research 39, 31–37.
  syndrome, major depression and healthy controls : an exploratory
                                                                           Powell, R., Dolan, R. & Wessely, S. (1990). Attributions and self-
  study of clinical correlates of regional cerebral blood flow.
                                                                             esteem in depression and chronic fatigue syndromes. Journal of
  Neuropsychobiology 34, 175–183.
                                                                             Psychosomatic Research 34, 665–73.
Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M. C., Dobbins, J. G.
                                                                           Ray, C. (1991). Chronic fatigue syndrome and depression : conceptual
  & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome : a
                                                                             and methodological ambiguities. Psychological Medicine 21, 1–9.
  comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International
                                                                           Rosenberg, M. (1995). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image.
  Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Annals of Internal                   Princeton University Press : Princeton, NJ.
  Medicine 121, 953–959.                                                   Schwartz, R. B., Komaroff, A. L., Garada, B. M., Gleit, M.,
Gilson, B. S., Bergner, M., Bobbit, R. A. & Carter, W. B. (1979). The        Doolittle, T. H., Bates, D. W., Vasile, R. G. & Holman, B. L.
  Sickness Impact Profile : Final Development and Testing. University        (1994). SPECT imaging of the brain : comparison of findings in
  of Washington, Department of Health Services : Seattle.                    patients with chronic fatigue syndrome, AIDS dementia complex,
Goldstein, J., Mena, I., Jouanne, E. & Lesser, I. (1995). The                and major unipolar depression. American Journal of Roentgenology
  assessment of vascular abnormalities in late life chronic fatigue          162, 943–951.
  syndrome by brain SPECT : comparison with late life major                Schweitzer, R., Kelly, B., Foran, A., Terry, D. & Whiting, J. (1995).
  depressive disorder. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome 1, 55–79.         Quality of life in chronic fatigue syndrome. Social Science and
Health, WHO (1993). World Health Organization Composite In-                  Medicine 41, 1367–1372.
  ternational Diagnostic Interview : CIDI-Auto version 1.1. St.            Sharpe, M., Hawton, K., Seagroatt, V. & Pasvol, G. (1992). Follow
  Vincent’s Hospital : Sydney.                                               up of patients presenting with fatigue to an infectious diseases
Heijmans, M. & de Ridder, D. (1998). Assessing illness represent-            clinic. British Medical Journal 305, 147–152.
  ations of chronic illness : explorations of their disease-specific       Surawy, C., Hackmann, A., Hawton, K. & Sharpe, M. (1995).
  nature. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 21, 485–503.                        Chronic fatigue syndrome : a cognitive approach. Behaviour
Hickie, I., Lloyd, A., Wakefield, D. & Parker, G. (1990). The                Research and Therapy 33, 535–44.
  psychiatric status of patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome.        Vercoulen, J., Swanink, C. M. A., Zitman, F. G., Vreden, S. G. S.,
  British Journal of Psychiatry 156, 534–540.                                Hoofs, M. P. E., Fennis, J. F. M., Galama, J. M. D., Vandermeer,
Johnson, S. K., Deluca, J. & Natelson, B. H. (1996). Depression in           J. W. M. & Bleijenberg, G. (1996). Randomised, double-blind,
  fatiguing illness – comparing patients with chronic fatigue syn-           placebo-controlled study of fluoxetine in chronic fatigue syndrome.
  drome, multiple sclerosis and depression. Journal of Affective             Lancet 347, 858–861.
  Disorders 39, 21–30.                                                     Viet, C. T. & Ware, J. E. (1983). The structure of psychological
Johnston, M., Wright, S. & Weinman, J. (1995). Measures in Health            distress and well-being in general populations. Journal of Consulting
  Psychology : A User’s Portfolio. NFER-Nelson : Windsor.                    and Clinical Psychology 51, 730–742.
Katon, W. J., Buchwald, D. S., Simon, G. E., Russo, J. E. & Mease,         Ware, J. E. & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short-
  P. J. (1991). Psychiatric illness in patients with chronic fatigue and     form health survey. Medical Care 30, 473–483.
  those with rheumatoid arthritis. Journal of General Internal             Ware, J. E., Snow, K. K., Kosinski, M. A. & Gandek, B. (1993). SF-
  Medicine 6, 277–285.                                                       36 Health Survey : Manual and Interpretation Guide. The Health
Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L. &                  Institute : Boston.
Cognitions in chronic fatigue syndrome                                                     479

Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Moss-Morris, R. & Horne, R. (1996). The    Wessely, S., Butler, S., Chalder, T. & David, A. (1991). The cognitive
 Illness Perception Questionnaire – a new method for assessing the      behavioural management of the post-viral fatigue syndrome. In
 cognitive representation of illness. Psychology and Health 11,         Postviral Fatigue-Syndrome (ed. R. J. Mowbrey and J. Mowbrey),
 431–445.                                                               pp. 305–334. John Wiley & Sons : Chichester.
Wessely, S. (1996). Chronic fatigue syndrome. Summary of a report      Wittchen, H. U. (1994). Reliability and validity studies of the WHO-
 of a joint committee of the Royal Colleges of Physicians,              Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) : a critical
 Psychiatrists and General Practitioners. Journal of the Royal          review. Journal of Psychiatric Research 28, 57–84.
 College of Physicians of London 30, 497–504.                          Wood, G. C., Bentall, R. P., Go$ pfert, M. & Edwards, R. H. T.
Wessely, S. & Powell, R. (1989). Fatigue syndromes : a comparison of    (1991). A comparative psychiatric assessment of patients with
 chronic ‘ postviral ’ fatigue with neuromuscular function. Journal     chronic fatigue syndrome and muscle disease. Psychological
 of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 52, 940–948.                Medicine 21, 619–628.
You can also read