Seeing Disorder: Neighborhood Stigma and the Social Construction of "Broken Windows"

Page created by Charlie Aguilar
 
CONTINUE READING
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                                                                 Social Psychology Quarterly
                                                                                 2004, Vol. 67, No. 4, 319–342

                                Seeing Disorder: Neighborhood Stigma
                    and the Social Construction of “Broken Windows”*
                                                                               ROBERT J. SAMPSON
                                                                                      Harvard University
                                                                      STEPHEN W. RAUDENBUSH
                                                                                  University of Michigan

       This article reveals the grounds on which individuals form perceptions of disorder.
       Integrating ideas about implicit bias and statistical discrimination with a theoretical
       framework on neighborhood racial stigma, our empirical test brings together personal
       interviews, census data, police records, and systematic social observations situated with-
       in some 500 block groups in Chicago. Observed disorder predicts perceived disorder,
       but racial and economic context matter more. As the concentration of minority groups
       and poverty increases, residents of all races perceive heightened disorder even after we
       account for an extensive array of personal characteristics and independently observed
       neighborhood conditions. Seeing disorder appears to be imbued with social meanings
       that go well beyond what essentialist theories imply, generating self-reinforcing process-
       es that may help account for the perpetuation of urban racial inequality.

     The concept of neighborhood disorder                  that residents are indifferent to what goes on
once again has assumed priority in the social              in the neighborhood. Few ideas have become
sciences.1 The most visible inquiry has been               more influential than “broken windows”
played out in urban sociology and criminolo-               (see, e.g., Duneier 1999; Harcourt 2001;
gy. According to the “broken windows” theo-                Taylor 2001).
ry of urban decline (Wilson and Kelling                         Although perhaps less noticed, it is
1982), minor forms of public disorder lead to              equally significant that the concept of disor-
serious crime and a downward spiral of urban               der has penetrated social psychology. Again
decay (Kelling and Coles 1996). The pre-                   the notion is that disorder is negative, this
sumed reason is that visual cues such as graf-             time with harmful consequences for individ-
fiti, public intoxication, garbage, and                    ual health and personal well-being. For
abandoned cars are thought to attract crimi-               example, a number of recent studies have
nal offenders, who assume from these cues                  linked perceived disorder to physical decline,
                                                           depression, psychological distress, and per-
                                                           ceived powerlessness (e.g., Cutrona et al.
   * Funding from the American Bar Foundation and
                                                           2000; Geis and Ross 1998; Linares et al. 2001;
the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation
is gratefully acknowledged, as is the research assis-      Mitchell and LaGory 2002; Ross et al. 2000).
tance of Bart Bingenheimer and Nigel Gannon. The           On these accounts, residents read signs of dis-
Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral                order as evidence of a deeper neighborhood
Sciences provided terrific sabbatical and intellectual     malaise, undermining personal health.
support. We also thank Doug McAdam, Steve Levitt,
Tony Bryk, Felton Earls, Mark Suchman, Per Olof
                                                                Yet there remains a puzzling first-order
Wikström, Cecilia Ridgeway, and the reviewers of           question about what triggers our perceptions
SPQ for helpful comments on an earlier version.            of disorder. Is “seeing” disorder only a matter
Address correspondence to Robert J. Sampson,               of the objective level of cues in the environ-
Department of Sociology, Harvard University,               ment? Or is disorder filtered through a rea-
Cambridge, MA 02138; rsampson@wjh.harvard.edu.
   1 Fascination with disorder has a long history. From    soning based on stigmatized groups and
early observers of London in the 1800s such as             disreputable areas? Simply put, what makes
Charles Booth (see Pfautz 1967) and Henry Mayhew           disorder a problem? We find that most
(1862), to authors of modern classics such as Jane         research on disorder is based on individual-
Jacobs (1961), visual signs of disorder in public spaces
                                                           level perceptions decoupled from a systemat-
have been viewed as central to understanding the
civic fabric of cities.                                    ic concern with the disorder-generating
                                                       319
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

320                       SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
environment. This is unfortunate for our            whether disorder is a problem, combining
understanding of how perceptions line up            uncertain evidence with prior beliefs and cul-
with independently observed ecological              tural stereotypes.2 Evidence from cognitive
assessments, and how such perceptions are           psychology suggests that categories are espe-
socially structured. As Harcourt (2001)             cially important for the organization of infor-
argues, theoretical reflection and empirical        mation in everyday life (Fiske 1998;
evidence bearing on the meaning of disorder         Huttenlocher, Hedges and Vevea 2000). If
are remarkably thin.                                cultural stereotypes are pervasive and if resi-
     Thus we set aside the usual questions:         dents have uncertain information about dis-
whether disorder does or does not cause             order, then they may, in a Bayesian way,
crime, or whether disorder is linked causally       augment that information with contextual
to poor health. Instead we examine what pre-        cues about categories of people who can be
dicts individuals’ perceptions that disorder,       seen on the streets.
defined in the manner of “broken windows,”               What categories matter? Research on
is a problem. Drawing on independent sets of        implicit bias and cultural stereotyping sug-
linked data, we examine how the racial, eth-        gests that Americans hold persistent beliefs
nic, and socioeconomic structure of neigh-          linking blacks and disadvantaged minority
borhood contexts shapes perceptions above           groups to many social images, including but
and beyond observable conditions of disor-          not limited to crime, violence, disorder, wel-
der.                                                fare, and undesirability as neighbors (e.g.,
                                                    Bobo 2001; Bobo and Kluegel 1997; Quillian
Implicit Bias and the Social Meaning of             and Pager 2001). Beliefs about disorder are
Disorder                                            reinforced by the historical association of
                                                    nonvoluntary racial segregation with concen-
  In the first instance, “race” is a mode of per-   trated poverty, which in turn is linked to insti-
  ceptual categorization people use to navigate     tutional disinvestments and neighborhood
  their way through a murky, uncertain world        decline (Massey and Denton 1993; Skogan
  (Loury 2002:17).                                  1990; Wilson 1987). As Loury (2002) argues
                                                    in his theory of racial inequality, dark skin is
     We begin by noting that the “broken win-       an easily observable trait that has become a
dows” theory assumes an essentialist notion         statistical marker in American society, one
both of disorder and its connection to per-         imbued with meanings about crime and dis-
ception: visual cues are unambiguous and            order that stigmatize not only people but also
natural in meaning (also see Harcourt 2001).        the places in which they are concentrated.
A similar train of thought is seen in the                The use of racial context to encode disor-
health and social psychological literature,         der does not necessarily mean that people
where perceived disorder is thought to reflect      are racially prejudiced in the sense of person-
external problems that influence local resi-        al hostility. The power of cultural stereotypes
dents’ mental and physical health. This is all      is that they operate beneath the radar screen,
quite reasonable, especially if residents’ per-     forming what has been termed implicit bias
ceptions map neatly onto objectively observ-        (Banaji 2002; Bobo 2001; Fiske 1998).
able aspects of disorder such as garbage,           Suppose, for example, that someone without
broken bottles, litter, graffiti, abandoned cars,   racial animus has nonetheless been exposed
and drug paraphernalia.                             to the historically and structurally induced
     Objective cues of “disorder” are certain-      inequality that is urban America: on average,
ly salient. So also, we argue, are cultural         levels of observable disorder are higher in
stereotypes about disorder in American soci-        black neighborhoods than in white. Implicit
ety. Stereotypes loom large when residents          bias arises when this person automatically
are not trained as systematic or neutral            concludes from such a statistical generaliza-
observers and are likely to have uncertain or       tion that a specific all-black neighborhood
ambiguous information about the neighbor-
hood as a whole. It follows that individuals          2
                                                        For an overview of Bayesian reasoning see
may act somewhat like Bayesians in judging          Sedlmeier and Gigerenzer (2001).
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                               NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                             321
has a disorder problem, even when empirical        Neighborhood Racial Stigma
inquiry would lead to contrary evidence.
                                                         We believe that the literature on implicit
Research in social psychology has shown that
                                                   bias can be linked productively with Loury’s
automatic racial stereotypes can persist
                                                   (2002) emphasis on racial stigma, as well as
regardless of conscious or personal rejection      with previous but often neglected writings on
of prejudice toward blacks (Devine 1989).          the “ecological contamination” of places.
This situation leads to what Bobo (2001:292)       Although Goffman’s (1963) concept of stig-
calls “laissez-faire racism.” Negative cultural    ma originally was advanced at the individual
stereotyping, rather than normative preju-         level, its contextual or group forms are equal-
dice, also has been shown to account for           ly compelling. A contextual stance was taken
white Americans’ widespread unwillingness          some time ago by Werthman and Piliavin
to share residential space with blacks and         (1967), who argued that the police divide up
other minority groups (Bobo and Massagli           the territories they patrol into readily under-
2001). We argue that such cognitive bias, via      standable, and racially shaped, categories.
the stereotypical association of blacks with       The result is a process of what they called
disorder, works in a similar, implicit way.        ecological contamination, whereby all per-
     The power of implicit bias is seen in a       sons encountered in “bad” neighborhoods
recent and fascinating experimental study          are viewed as possessing the moral liability of
(Correll et al. 2002). Researchers used a          the neighborhood itself. This process has gen-
videotaping strategy to examine the effect of      eral implications insofar as citizens them-
                                                   selves impute the character of disreputability
race on shoot/don’t shoot decisions in scenar-
                                                   (Hagan 1994:150) to neighborhoods contain-
ios where subjects were told to shoot armed
                                                   ing stigmatized minorities and the “rabble
targets and not to shoot unarmed targets.
                                                   class” (Irwin 1985; Wacquant 1993). Such
Participants made the correct decision to          stigmatization appears to be an enduring
shoot an armed target more quickly if the tar-     mechanism. In the 1800s, for example,
get was African American than if he was            Charles Booth produced color-coded maps
white. Interestingly, the magnitude of this        of London’s poor, with the lowest grade
racial bias in shooting decisions varied with      “inhabited principally by occasional labour-
perception of cultural stereotypes but not         ers, loafers, and semi-criminals—the ele-
with personal racial prejudice. In fact, the       ments of disorder” (quoted in Pfautz
study revealed equivalent levels of shooting       1967:191).
bias in African American and in white partici-           That the social structure of public places
pants. This finding underscores the potential-     is tied to race and class sharply reinforces the
ly far-reaching consequences of statistical        production of disrepute (Duneier 1999). As
discrimination and cultural stereotypes that       Stinchcombe (1963) argued, access to private
reside below the level of conscious racial         space is structured such that disorder by the
prejudice (also see Devine 1998). As the           disadvantaged consists of doing many things
authors argue, ethnicity can influence the         in public that would be (and are) legitimate
decision to shoot because cultural traits asso-    in private (e.g., drinking, hanging out). That
                                                   is, privileged status enhances private access,
ciated with African Americans, namely “vio-
                                                   reducing exposure to public disorder. The
lent” or “dangerous,” act as a schema to
                                                   resulting social structure of public spaces
influence perceptions of an ambiguously
                                                   reinforces the stereotype that disorder is a
threatening target (Correll et al. 2002:1325).     problem mainly in poor, African-American
African Americans are unlikely to be racially      communities (Fagan and Davies 2000); this
prejudiced against their own ethnic group,         stereotype feeds racial stigma and the cre-
but they are exposed, as is everybody, to          ation of a durable spoiled identity for the
dominant cultural stereotypes.3                    modern American ghetto (Wacquant 1993).
                                                         The insidious quality of implicit bias is
  3 On implicit bias, also see https://implicit.   realized further when predictions become
harvard.edu/implicit/ and Banaji (2002).           self-confirming, leading to actions that
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

322                       SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
increase the statistical association between        status of persons rather than on effects of
race and the observable behavior (Loury             neighborhood racial contexts.
2002:23). For example, if more affluent resi-            The evidence on disorder relevant to our
dents use a neighborhood’s racial composi-          theoretical approach thus dwindles to a
tion as a gauge for the level and seriousness       handful of studies, but we find support to
of disorder, unconsciously or not, they may         motivate further inquiry. One effort is pre-
disinvest in predominately minority areas or        sent in Taylor’s (2001) long-term study of the
move out; such actions would tend to                city of Baltimore. In 1981–1982 he and his
increase physical disorder in those neighbor-       colleagues rated street blocks and conducted
hoods. In this way implicit bias leads to rein-     a survey of residents in each of 66 neighbor-
forcing mechanisms that perpetuate the              hoods. In 1994, Taylor returned to 30 of the
connection of race to disorder, therefore           original neighborhoods to conduct on-site
helping to explain the dynamics reinforcing         ratings and another round of interviews. A
racial segregation (Charles 2002, 2003).            key finding was that the perception of disor-
                                                    der varied widely between individuals even
                                                    within the same neighborhood. Perkins et al.
Prior Evidence and Hypotheses
                                                    (1993), using similar procedures in New York
     Surprisingly little research bears on our      City, found that residents’ perceptions and an
theoretical framing of the social structures        independent rating of physical disorder were
that shape perceptions of disorder as a prob-       not significantly correlated.
lem. One stream of research has been stimu-              A second and perhaps more relevant
lated by the theory of broken windows and           study was conducted by Quillian and Pager
crime (Wilson and Kelling 1982), but tests of       (2001), who capitalized on residential surveys
this theory (e.g., Sampson and Raudenbush           and police data from Seattle, Chicago, and
1999; Taylor 2001) do not adjudicate our con-       Baltimore to assess the association of racial
cerns.                                              composition with perceptions of neighbor-
     A second stream of studies explores the        hood crime. Using individual- and neighbor-
connection of neighborhood conditions to            hood-level measures, they found a positive
individual-level health and psychological           association between perceived neighborhood
functioning. An important finding has been          crime and the percentage of young black
                                                    men. This finding was robust when the
the persistent correlation of perceived disor-
                                                    authors controlled for police-recorded crime
der with mental health outcomes such as
                                                    rates, survey-reported victimization, and
depression, psychological distress, and per-
                                                    measures of neighborhood deterioration.
ceived powerlessness (e.g., Aneshensel and
                                                    Quillian and Pager (2001) argue that the rela-
Sucoff 1996; Cutrona et al. 2000; Geis and
                                                    tionship between race and perceived crime
Ross 1998; Mitchell and LaGory 2002; Ross
                                                    may identify a key mechanism in the process
2000; Ross et al. 2000; for a recent review, also   of white flight and residential segregation
see Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-                  (Massey and Denton 1993; Schelling 1971).
Rowley 2002). Similarly, measures of hostili-       In addition, they underscore the importance
ty, mistrust, and conduct disorder have been        of conceptualizing perceived crime and dis-
linked to perceived disorder (e.g.,                 order as distinct from actual crime rates.
Aneshensel and Sucof, 1996; Ewart and                    A third, ethnographic study also suggests
Suchday 2002). Nearly all of the studies in the     the symbolic importance attached to the
health field measure disorder at the individ-       intersection of race and disorder. In a study
ual rather than the contextual level, however;      of a white working-class Chicago neighbor-
thus it is difficult to isolate the neighborhood-   hood, Kefalas (2003) sought to understand
level mechanisms at work in shaping per-            the fastidiousness with which residents kept
ceived disorder.                                    up their property and why they seemed to be
     A third stream of studies, as noted above,     obsessed with physical signs of order. She
connects race to implicit bias in stereotypes       found that homeowners fretted about “the
about crime and violence. The literature on         last Garden” and the threats that disorder
implicit bias, however, focuses on the racial       were thought to bring on the neighborhood
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                             323
(2003:11, 14, 62, 74). No act of vandalism was      Denton 1993). Thus our main hypothesis is
too minor; no unkempt yard was too trivial to       that neighborhood racial/ethnic and class
escape notice. On the basis of numerous             composition predicts perceptions of neigh-
interviews, Kefalas argues that residents did       borhood disorder, even with adjustments for
not care so much about disorder per se, but         disorder as measured systematically in those
were threatened subjectively by the urban           neighborhoods by video cameras and trained
underclass, blacks in particular. In Chicago,       observers. This does not imply a simple “rela-
residents perceive the history of the               tivist” stance: we expect residents’ percep-
Southwest Side as a long westward march of          tions of disorder to be based in part on their
decline, where the first signs were the visual      observations of objective indicators such as
cues of disorder. Thus in many ways the resi-       trash in the streets, broken bottles, graffiti,
dents of Kefalas’s Beltway had a “broken            abandoned cars, drug paraphernalia in gut-
windows” theory in mind, but one with a             ters, and the presence of loitering, drunken,
decidedly black face (2003:43). The impor-          or hostile adults. Our larger point, however, is
tance attached to disorder went well beyond         that the social and especially the racial com-
the level of its occurrence in the neighbor-        position of a local area, which is associated
hood, so much so that the deeper meaning of         statistically with disorder, is highly salient in
disorder for residents motivated Kefalas’s          contemporary culture and deeply imbued
study originally. Pattillo-McCoy’s (1999)           with stereotypes. If a Bayesian view of
study of a black middle-class neighborhood          human reasoning is integrated with the
reveals a similar concern with the perceived        research on implicit bias, perceptions of dis-
threat of disorder, although in this case the       order then would be constituted as a combi-
threat came from lower-class residents within       nation of inferences based on observable
the black community.                                evidence of disorder and correlated informa-
     A small but intriguing body of research        tion, especially skin color. Because racial
thus motivates our general proposition that         composition is observed easily and carries
perceptions of disorder are socially con-           powerful stereotypes (even if subconscious-
structed and are shaped by much more than           ly), by this logic it will generate a compara-
actual levels of disorder. We assess this theo-     tively large weight; social context may even
retical framework by combining census and           “trump” actual disorder.
police data on key neighborhood structural               If racial composition independently pre-
characteristics with original data collection in    dicts the subjective perception of disorder, a
the form of personal interviews and the sys-        question then arises: whether this perception
tematic social observation of city streets. If      reflects pure racial prejudice rather than
essentialist theories of disorder are correct,      what social psychologists call implicit or
we should find considerable agreement on            automatic bias (e.g., Bobo and Massagli
perceived disorder within neighborhoods,            2001:93), or what Loury (2002) would call
few if any systematic variations by social          statistical discrimination.Antiblack prejudice
position within the same neighborhood, and,         would be likely to affect the perceptions of
most important, few if any between-neigh-           whites (or Latinos or Asians) more strongly
borhood variations in perceived disorder            than blacks. That is, insofar as nonblacks are
linked to social structure when interpersonal-      particularly infected with negative stereo-
ly observed disorder is taken into account.         types of blacks as a group, they might be
     By contrast, insofar as disorder is socially   expected to report more disorder in predom-
encoded, we expect that perceptions of disor-       inately black neighborhoods while overlook-
der are shaped by individual social position        ing similar levels of disorder that might exist
and by neighborhood stratification—espe-            in a predominately white (or nonblack) area.
cially by race and class. Disorder, in our          Prejudice, in other words, would be the dri-
framework, is part of a larger cultural narra-      ving force. By contrast, drawing on the idea
tive or generalized stereotype that is tightly      of implicit bias coupled with the closely allied
bound up in American cities with racially and       notion of statistical discrimination, we argue
spatially understood meanings (Bobo and             that the association between racial composi-
Massagli 2001; Loury 2002; Massey and               tion and perceptions of disorder ought also to
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

324                      SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
be independent of the perceiver’s ethnicity.       block-group boundaries typically are drawn
As an analogy, consider the black citizen who      on the basis of street use, such that pedestrian
crosses the street when walking late at night      streets connect with one another without
to avoid a group of approaching young black        crossing major thoroughfares or highways.
males. Therefore we predict no interaction of           Perceptions of disorder are measured
individual-level race with racial composition      from six questions put to respondents. In
in perceiving disorder.                            keeping with the disorder literature (see
                                                   especially Skogan 1990:51–53; Taylor
             DATA SOURCES                          2001:56), residents were asked “how much of
                                                   a problem” (“a big problem,” “somewhat of a
    Our theoretical framework calls for a          problem,” “not a problem”) they considered
focus on how individual perceptions of disor-      litter/trash, graffiti, and vacant housing/store-
der vary within and between neighborhoods,         fronts (defined as physical disorder), as well
linked to independent assessments of disor-        as drinking in public, selling or using drugs,
der that are reliable and ecologically valid.      and teenagers causing a disturbance (social
We achieve this by integrating four sources of     disorder). 4 We constructed scales of per-
data.                                              ceived physical and social disorder with relia-
                                                   bilities at the block-group level of .65 and .67
Neighborhood Survey                                respectively; the combined scale is .70.5 These
     The first source is a neighborhood survey     reliabilities are relatively high, given that the
conducted in 1995 as part of a larger study. A     average block-group sample is under 10, and
stratified probability sample of approximate-      reflect the fact that 34 percent of the varia-
ly 500 block groups nested within clusters of
196 Chicago census tracts was selected to             4 Specifically, interviewers told respondents: “I’m

maximize variation by race/ethnicity and           going to read a list of things that are problems in some
SES. Within areas, households then were            neighborhoods. For each, please tell me how much of
                                                   a problem it is in your neighborhood. (a) How much
selected according to a multistage probability
                                                   of a problem is litter, broken glass, or trash on the
sample. At stage 1, city blocks were sampled       sidewalks and streets? Would you say it is a big prob-
randomly within tracts; at stage 2, dwelling       lem, somewhat of a problem, or not a problem in your
units were sampled randomly within blocks;         neighborhood?” This question was repeated for (b)
at stage 3, one adult resident (age 18 or older)   “graffiti on buildings and walls,” (c) “vacant or desert-
                                                   ed houses or storefronts,” (d) “drinking in public,” (e)
was chosen randomly within each selected           “people selling or using drugs,” and (f) “groups of
dwelling unit and was interviewed personally.      teenagers or adults hanging out in the neighborhood
The final sample size for our purposes was         and causing trouble.” Hence all items refer to physical
3,585 persons living in 478 block groups,          or social aspects that are observed in public spaces, in
reflecting a response rate of 78 percent.          keeping with the coding scheme of our observational
                                                   approach. In a recent pilot test conducted in service of
     We chose the census block group as the        a planned replication of the Chicago community sur-
smallest and most ecologically meaningful          vey, interviewers asked the questions about perceived
context to assess our main hypotheses.             disorder in two ways. They asked first about the per-
Census block groups average about 1,300 res-       ceived volume of disorder (e.g., a lot, some, a little),
idents in the sample to be described, com-         and second, as in the earlier survey, about the extent
                                                   to which each aspect of disorder “is a problem.” At
pared with more than 4,000 for the more            the individual level, measures of perceived disorder
commonly used unit of census tracts. As            derived from the two ways of wording the questions
Grannis (1998) has shown, block groups also        were correlated at r = .95. On the basis of this
reflect surprisingly well the layout of pedes-     extremely high correlation, we conclude that resi-
                                                   dents perceive aspects of disorder “to be a problem”
trian streets and patterns of social interac-
                                                   primarily to the extent that they “see” those aspects of
tion. In fact, neighborhood networks tend to       disorder.
concatenate within what Grannis (1998) calls          5
                                                        Reliability is defined as Σ[τ2/(τ2 + σ2/ nk)] / K, the
tertiary communities, defined as areas within      average of block-group specific reliabilities across the
which houses can be connected without              set of areas (K = 478). This reliability is a function of
                                                   (1) the sample size (nk) in each of the block groups
crossing a nonresidential street or thorough-      and (2) the proportion of the total variance between
fare. Block groups are similar to or nest eco-     neighborhoods (τ2) relative to the amount within
logically within tertiary communities because      neighborhoods (σ2).
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                    NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                              325
tion in perceived disorder lies between block             Police Records
groups.
                                                               Third, we collected the police-recorded
    From the neighborhood survey we
                                                          number of violent offenses (robbery, homi-
selected a core set of 12 demographic or
                                                          cide, rape, aggravated assault) geocoded to
background characteristics at the person                  the block-group level. We then constructed
level that we believe influence perceptions of            the log rate of violent crimes.
disorder. A key concern is race/ethnicity,
which we measure with indicators for Latino               Systematic Social Observation (SSO)
American, non-Latino African American, and
other (primarily Asian); non-Latino whites                     The fourth data source is systematic
serve as the reference category. This strategy            observation conducted within each of the
allows us to directly compare how blacks and              block groups. Building on Reiss (1971) and
Latinos assess disorder in relation to whites.            Raudenbush and Sampson (1999), by system-
                                                          atic we mean that observation and recording
Other controls include a composite measure
                                                          are conducted according to explicit rules that
of socioeconomic status (first principal com-
                                                          permit replication, and that the means of
ponent of the factor analysis of education,
                                                          observation are independent of that which is
income, and occupational prestige), sex (1 =              observed. During the time of the community
female, 0 = male), current marital status                 survey, observers trained at the National
(composed of separate indicators for mar-                 Opinion Research Center (NORC) drove a
ried, separated or divorced, and single),                 sport utility vehicle (SUV) at three to five
homeownership, residential mobility (number               miles per hour down every street within the
of moves in the past five years), residential             sample of almost 500 block groups. The origi-
tenure (years in the neighborhood), and age.              nal geographic unit of recorded observation
                                                          was the face block: the block segment on one
Census Data                                               side of a street. A unique geographic identifi-
                                                          cation code was created for each face block
     We collected a second set of data from               to permit linking to the block group. As the
the census at the block-group level for 1990,             SUV was driven down the street, a pair of
which includes the following theoretically                video recorders, one located on each side of
relevant measures for assessing disorder: pro-            the SUV, captured social activities and physi-
portion of families in poverty, population                cal features of both face blocks simultaneous-
size, 6 density of settlement (persons per                ly. At the same time, two trained observers,
square kilometer), and proportion black and               one on each side of the SUV, recorded their
Latino. Hence we define racial/ethnic com-                observations onto an observer log for each
position in the same fashion at both the per-             face block. Face blocks were observed ran-
son and census structural level. Unlike larger            domly and videotaped from 7 a.m. to 7 p.m.
aggregations, block groups provide the addi-                   In all, 23,816 face blocks were observed
                                                          and videorecorded for an average of almost
tional information necessary to address the
                                                          50 per block group. Because videotapes
well-known multicollinearity among ecologi-
                                                          required the expensive and time-consuming
cal variables. For example, poverty and per-
                                                          task of first viewing and then coding, we
cent black are correlated at only .37 at the              selected a random subsample of 15,141 face
block-group level; this allows us the statistical         blocks for videotape coding, an average of
efficiency to disentangle independent effects.            about 30 per block group. Observer logs were
Throughout all analyses we performed                      coded for all face blocks. Coders were trained
checks for multicollinearity and influential              in multiple sessions, including an intercoder
observations.                                             reliability training session where 90 face
                                                          blocks were double coded independently, dif-
   6 We estimated all models with a logged version of
                                                          ferences were resolved, and coding proce-
population size. The results were largely the same,
                                                          dures were revised. As a second check on
although in most cases raw size was a slightly stronger   agreement, new observers recoded a random
predictor.                                                10 percent of all coded face blocks, and the
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

326                          SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
results were compared. This test produced                  specific aspects of disorder than does the sur-
over 98 percent interpersonal agreement, an                vey, including gangs, prostitution, and multi-
essential condition for the present study                  ple types of graffiti.9
(Carter, Dougherty, and Grigorian 1996;                         Our observational scale of physical dis-
NORC 1995).                                                order is limited to behavioral manifestations
     We constructed six theoretically motivat-             (e.g., graffiti, garbage in the streets) that can
ed scales based on the SSO face-block obser-               be conceptually decoupled from structural
vations. These scales purposefully measure                 resources. Therefore we examine the sepa-
not only traditionally defined disorder but                rate contribution, to perceived disorder, of
also physical decay, land use, commercial                  physical decay that can arise from institution-
building security, alcohol/tobacco advertis-               al disinvestments. We coded whether on each
ing, and bars/liquor stores. This strategy                 face block there was evidence of any of the
allows us to assess whether residents are                  five following conditions: vacant houses;
influenced more strongly by easily observed                burned-out, boarded-up, or abandoned com-
physical conditions (e.g., security fences on              mercial/industrial buildings; burned-out,
commercial buildings, abandoned houses)                    boarded-up or abandoned houses; badly
than by comparatively rare social conditions               deteriorated residential units; and badly
                                                           deteriorated recreation facilities. The decay
(e.g., prostitution, gangs in public) when
                                                           scale was derived from videotapes; again,
making inferences about disorder. Physical
                                                           double-blind coding produced interrater reli-
disorder is defined by 10 items from the
                                                           ability in the high 90s. Although physical
observer logs and videotaped coding that
                                                           decay and physical disorder are related posi-
capture the presence or absence of cigarettes
                                                           tively (r = .40), the data permit us to assess
or cigars in the street or gutter, garbage or lit-
                                                           how they predict perceived disorder inde-
ter on street or sidewalk, empty beer bottles
                                                           pendently. When the physical decay and
visible in the street, tagging graffiti, graffiti          physical disorder items are combined, no sur-
painted over, gang graffiti, abandoned cars,               vey disorder item is missing in the SSO mea-
condoms on the sidewalk, needles/syringes                  surement scheme.
on the sidewalk, and political message graffi-                  Commercial building security is mea-
ti.7                                                       sured by videotapes of each face block, with
     Social disorder was coded from the                    indicators for whether iron security gates or
videotapes. The scale items tap the presence               “pull-downs” were present on the building
or absence of adults loitering or congregat-               fronts and whether the windows were cov-
ing,8 drinking alcohol in public, peer group               ered with security gates. Alcohol/tobacco
with gang indicators, public intoxication,                 advertising is based on videotapes: simple
adults fighting or arguing in a hostile manner,            yes/no codes specify whether there were
selling drugs, and street prostitution. Taken              signs advertising either substance.
together, the SSO items cover many more                    Bars/liquor stores is a two-item scale based
                                                           on videotaped assessments of the presence or
   7 Graffiti not painted over were classified by type     absence of bars and establishments with visi-
on the basis of guidelines informed by gang research       ble signs of alcohol sales. Finally, we used one
in Chicago and internal Chicago police memos on            item from the SSO, the presence of commer-
gang identification (NORC 1995). Tag graffiti was
identified by stylized forms such as block-letter art or
by attempts to create some form of visual expression.
Gang graffiti ordinarily was distinguished by the             9 Again in line with prior research, the survey asked

absence of tag art and typically involved a combina-       about groups hanging out and “causing a distur-
tion of stars, crowns, emblems, and specific colors that   bance.” In the SSO we matched the survey as closely
distinguish among gangs. Political graffiti was defined    as we could, coding separate items not only for loiter-
as political messages and slogans.                         ing but also for peer groups with gang indicators,
   8
     We limited the definition of loitering to groups of   fighting and arguing on the street, intoxicated people,
three or more adults not waiting for scheduled activi-     and visible evidence of prostitution. Although per-
ties or business. For example, groups of adults waiting    haps we did not achieve a perfect match, any omitted
for public transportation or standing in line to enter a   SSO item that is an obvious manifestation of “causing
store would not be included, nor would children play-      a disturbance” would have to be uncorrelated with
ing in public.                                             the items we included to overturn the basic results.
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                     NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                              327
cial buildings in blocks with residential units,          As in structural equation models, our mea-
to calculate the percentage of face blocks in a           surement strategy for observed disorder thus
block group with mixed land use (see Table                corrects for unreliability in the scale scores in
1).                                                       assessing the estimated effects of other inde-
     Descriptive statistics on constructed                pendent predictors such as racial composi-
measures from the four data sources are                   tion (Whittemore 1989).
shown in Table 1. The full list of SSO items
for the six multi-item scales is also shown in             PREDICTING PERCEIVED DISORDER
the appendix and in Appendix Table A1,
along with parameter estimates for a multi-                   To address the nested structure of per-
level measurement model. We correct for                   ceived disorder in our research design, we
three major components of measurement                     adapt models that account for the depen-
error in constructing scales of observed dis-             dence of residents’ responses within block
order to be used as independent predictors:               groups. Each model can be conceived in
item inconsistency within a block group, ran-             terms of a within-neighborhood and a
domly missing data, and temporal variation.               between-neighborhood equation, though the

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations for Linked Data

                                                                            Mean                       SD
Survey Data, Person-Level Demographics
—Female                                                                       .58                       .49
—Married                                                                      .38                       .49
—Separated/divorced                                                           .17                       .38
—Single                                                                       .31                       .46
—Homeowner                                                                    .43                       .49
—Black                                                                        .34                       .47
—Latino                                                                       .33                       .47
—Other                                                                        .07                       .26
—Mobility                                                                    1.04                      1.48
—Age                                                                        41.79                     16.34
—Unemployed                                                                   .40                       .49
—SES scale                                                                   –.09                      1.36
Survey Data, Person-Level Perceived Disordera
—Litter/trash                                                                    .28                    .45
—Graffiti                                                                        .20                    .40
—Vacant houses                                                                   .13                    .33
—Public drinking                                                                 .25                    .43
—Selling drugs                                                                   .30                    .46
—Group loitering                                                                 .27                    .44
Census Data, Block Group
—Density                                                                 7,451.93                  4,732.33
—Poverty                                                                      .21                       .17
—Black                                                                        .36                       .42
—Latino                                                                       .26                       .30
—Population size                                                         1,335.63                    893.51
Police Data, Block Group
—Ln (violence rate)                                                             8.61                    .69
Systematic Social Observation Data, Block Group
—% mixed land use                                                           24.11                     11.31
—Bars/liquor storesb                                                          .01                       .98
—Alcohol/tobacco advertisementsb                                              .00                       .62
—Commercial building securityb                                                .00                       .81
—Physical decayb                                                              .00                       .87
—Physical disorderb                                                           .04                       .77
—Social disorderb                                                             .00                       .78
a   Proportion of respondents perceiving disorder item to be a “big problem.”
b
    See Appendix Table A1 for scale items.
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

328                            SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
two equations are estimated simultaneously              where ␪00 is the adjusted mean perceived dis-
by means of maximum likelihood                          order score, ␪01 through ␪05 are the regression
(Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). The within-                 coefficients for the census-based structural
neighborhood (person-level) model specifies             characteristics, and ␪06 through ␪12 are the
perceptions of disorder as a function of a              regression coefficients of the effects of SSO-
core set of individual characteristics that             based assessments. The random effects u0k, k
have been hypothesized or shown in prior                = 1,...K, are assumed to be identically and
research to be influential. Our person-level            independently distributed with mean zero
model is                                                and variance τ2. Thus our strategy permits
                     12

  (Disorder)jk = ␤0k +   Σ␤
                         q=1
                               qk
                                    Xqjk + rjk,   (1)
                                                        variations at both the person and the block-
                                                        group levels, with appropriate measurements
                                                        for each. Although it is possible to allow one
where (Disorder)jk is disorder in neighbor-             or more level 1 regression coefficients to vary
hood k as perceived by respondent j in that             over neighborhoods, in the interest of sim-
neighborhood, ␤0k is a neighborhood-specific            plicity we set ␤qk = ␪q0 for q = 1,...,12 in our
intercept, Xqjk is the value of covariate q asso-       initial models. All coefficients are estimated
ciated with respondent j in neighborhood k,             simultaneously.
␤qk is the neighborhood-specific partial effect
of that covariate on disorder, and rjk is a per-        Predictors
son-specific random error assumed to be dis-
tributed independently and normally with                    Person-level. Although we focus mainly
constant variance σ2. Covariates include indi-          on neighborhood variations, it is important to
cators for gender, ethnicity, socioeconomic             understand how individuals within the same
status, employment status, age, marital status,         environment perceive disorder. Thus in Table
homeownership, and mobility. To estimate                2 we begin to examine how perceptions of
how these covariates are associated with per-           disorder vary as a function of the perceiver’s
ceptions within block groups (in Table 2                personal characteristics, with all neighbor-
below), we center each Xqjk around its neigh-           hood conditions controlled.
borhood mean. This ensures that between-                    The results show that older residents per-
block-group variation in each Xqjk plays no             ceive less disorder than do younger residents,
role in the estimation. For this purpose, we            those who are separated or divorced perceive
allow the block-group intercept, ␤0k, to vary           more disorder than do widowed persons, and
randomly around its overall mean and fix
other coefficients ␤qk, q = 1,...,12 to be con-         Table 2. Within-Neighborhood Predictors of
stant.                                                  Perceived Disorder
     When we estimate associations between
                                                                                 Coeff.    SE        t-Ratio
block-group covariates and perceptions,
however, we center each Xqjk around its over-           Covariate
                                                        —Intercept               1.764     .019      92.69**
all mean, ensuring that contributions of
                                                        —Female                   .041     .020       2.06*
block-group covariates to perceptions will be           —Married                  .011     .033        .34
adjusted for person-level covariates (as in             —Separated/divorced       .081     .035       2.32*
Table 3 below). For this purpose our                    —Single                   .046     .036       1.29
between-neighborhood model is                           —Homeowner                .037     .022       1.61
                                                        —Black                   –.157     .035      –4.45**
 ␤0k = ␪00 + ␪01(poverty)k + ␪02(black)k                —Latino                  –.059     .035      –1.88
                                                        —Other                   –.116     .039      –2.89**
     + ␪03(Latino)k + ␪04(size)k + ␪05(density)k
                                                        —Mobility                 .010     .006       1.58
     + ␪06(SSO physical disorder)k                      —Age                     –.002     .001      –3.13**
     + ␪07(SSO social disorder)k                        —Unemployed              –.042     .022      –1.91
     + ␪08(SSO physical decay)k                (2)      —SES scale               –.014     .009      –1.56
                                                        Level 1 Variance          .231
     + ␪09(SSO building security)k                      Level 2 Variance          .123
     + ␪10(SSO bars/liquor stores)k
                                                        Notes: N = 3,116 persons and 478 block groups.
     + ␪11(SSO alcohol advertising)k
                                                        Covariates are centered within block groups.
     + ␪12(SSO mixed land use)k + u0k ,                 *p < .05; **p < .01
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                  NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                                  329
females tend to perceive more disorder than             order and whites more. The basic psychologi-
do males. Employment status, SES, mobility,             cal mechanism involves the perception of dis-
and homeownership, however, are unrelated               crepancies based on expectation,
to perceptions of disorder. Most relevant and           underscoring the fact that perceived disorder
most evident are the effects of ethnicity:              reflects more than meets the eye.
blacks perceive significantly less disorder                  Neighborhood-level. We now turn to the
than do whites living in the same block group           main question. How are perceptions of disor-
(coefficient = –.157, t = –4.45). This is true as       der influenced by neighborhood-level char-
well for “other” races and for Latinos (p <             acteristics? To answer this question, we
.10).                                                   introduce our independent measures of sys-
     This pattern makes sense if blacks and             tematically observed disorder to assess
other minorities have been exposed to more              between-neighborhood variations in per-
disorder in the past; such exposure in turn             ceived disorder (Table 3, Model 1). We adjust
influences the threshold for considering it a           for compositional differences in all person-
problem. In the segregated and racialized city          level covariates through grand mean center-
that is Chicago, for example, a white person            ing, but because of space limitations, the
living in an all-white area would expect to             individual-level coefficients are not repeated
see, on average, relatively small amounts of            in Table 3 (available on request). There is
disorder. In this case even minor amounts of            clear evidence that observed disorder strong-
disorder might be perceived as a problem. A             ly predicts perceptions of disorder, under-
black person living in an all-black neighbor-           scoring the validity of the measurement
hood, however, would see more disorder, on              scheme. For example, we see large and inde-
average; thus the level must rise to a higher           pendent contributions of observed physical
level to be perceived as a problem. In an inte-         and social disorder (t = 10.06 and 6.06 respec-
grated area, which (by definition) Table 2              tively), with 73 percent of the neighborhood
models, this argument implies that the two              level variance explained (unconditional vari-
groups are judging disorder by the norms                ance = .235). Furthermore, when we add the
that have been generated in past, segregated            SSO indicators of the physical aspects of pub-
environments: hence blacks perceive less dis-           lic space in Model 2, we see that a neighbor-

Table 3.Main-Effect Estimates for Neighborhood-Level Predictors of Total Perceived Disorder, With Separate
Indicators for Observed Physical and Social Characteristics

                                       Model 1                 Model 2                    Model 3
                                    (SSO Disorder)       (Adding SSO Physical)     (Adding Social Context)
                                    Coeff.    t-Ratio      Coeff.    t-Ratio             Coeff.   t-Ratio
Neighborhood Level
—Intercept                          1.747    115.12**      1.693     40.72**             1.261    25.46**
—Physical disorder                   .225     10.06**       .146      5.89**              .038     1.55
—Social disorder                     .123      6.06**       .096      4.74**              .044     2.24*
—Bars/liquor stores                                         .016       .43                .017      .56
—Alcohol/tobacco advertisements                             .045      1.36                .052     1.50
—Commercial building security                               .010       .43                .004      .18
—Physical decay                                             .129      6.98**              .068     3.77**
—% mixed land use                                           .002      1.34                .001      .73
—Poverty                                                                                  .756     7.43**
—Black                                                                                    .414     8.03**
—Latino                                                                                   .442     6.63**
—Population size                                                                     .015ⴒ10–3     1.10
—Density                                                                             .001ⴒ10–3      .44
Level 1 Variance                      .233                  .232                          .230
Level 2 Variance                      .063                  .051                          .031
Notes: N = 3,316 persons and 478 block groups. Neighborhood estimates are adjusted for compositional differ-
ences in the 12 person-level covariates shown in Table 2.
*p < .05; **p < .01
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

330                           SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
hood’s observable physical decay is related           variables. The first component appears to tap
positively and significantly to perceived dis-        commercial areas that sell and/or advertise
order (t = 6.98) net of observed disorder.            alcoholic beverages or tobacco, and where
     In Model 3 we add neighborhood ethnic            building security is extensive. The second
and social composition to test our main the-          component combines physical and social dis-
sis. With controls for observed disorder and          order with physical decay.
physical structure in addition to the person-              To achieve greater parsimony we thus
level predictors, neighborhood social and             substitute these two principal components
ethnic composition are linked powerfully to           for the six SSO predictors in predicting per-
perceptions of disorder. In particular, con-          ceived disorder. In addition, we disaggregate
centrated poverty, proportion black, and pro-         perceived disorder by type (social and physi-
portion Latino are related positively and             cal): presumably physical disorder is less sen-
significantly to perceived disorder.                  sitive to cultural stereotypes or statistical
Moreover, adjusting for racial context greatly        discrimination based on racial context than is
reduces the association between SSO and               the more nebulous concept of social disorder.
perceived disorder: the coefficient for social        This strategy simultaneously addresses biases
disorder is cut in half, and the association          that might emerge from item discrepancies
between physical observed disorder and per-           between the survey and the SSO method by
ceptions is eliminated entirely. (Compare             type of disorder. In particular, the overlap
coefficients for these variables moving               between survey and SSO items is not exact
between Model 2 and Model 3.) Thus much               within subscales (in the SSO scale, for exam-
of the explained variance of SSO disorder in          ple, vacant houses are considered signs of
Models 1 and 2 is spurious through its associ-        physical decay and disinvestment rather than
ation with racial and class composition.              of disorder), but in combination the coverage
                                                      is full.
Principal Components of Disorder                           We also control for two new variables to
                                                      test the robustness of our main findings. One
    The six scales derived from systematic            is the log violence rate at the neighborhood
social observation are correlated, to some            level.10 Violence, especially violent crimes
degree; they also vary in reliability (see            that make the news, may influence subjective
appendix). Therefore it is appropriate to             perceptions of threat. In particular, the “bro-
investigate the contributions of a reduced set        ken windows” thesis might be read to imply
of constructs that capture common sources of          that residents will perceive disorder as a
variation among these six measurements.               problem primarily when it is connected to
Table 4 displays the results of a principal-          public (or official) accounts of personal vio-
components analysis. We found two compo-
nents that are reasonable on conceptual                  10 Substituting a survey measure of violence pro-
grounds, accounting for approximately 65              duces quite similar but weaker results. We retain offi-
percent of the variation among the six SSO            cial violence because it provides the stronger test.

Table 4. Higher-Order Principal-Components Analysis of Systematic Social Observation (SSO) Scales

                                                              Rotated Loadings
                                                   Factor 1                      Factor 2
SSO Scale
—Bars/liquor stores                                   .88                           .06
—Alcohol/tobacco advertisements                       .90                           .09
—Commercial building security                         .62                           .46
—Physical decay                                       .03                           .74
—Physical disorder                                    .31                           .75
—Social disorder                                      .07                           .69
Eigenvalues                                          2.07                          1.82
% Variance Explained                                34.59                         30.32
Note: N = 478 block groups.
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

                                  NEIGHBORHOOD STIGMA                                                   331
lence. Because race and poverty are related           borhood are invited?” “When a neighbor is
to official crime, a spurious connection may          not at home, how often do you and other
exist between perceiving disorder as a prob-          neighbors watch over their property?” “How
lem and large concentrations of minority and          often do you and other people in this neigh-
poor residents (Quillian and Pager 2001).             borhood visit in each other’s homes or on the
    The other control stems from a long his-          street?” “How often do you and other people
tory of research in urban sociology on the            in the neighborhood ask each other advice
importance of social exchange and ties                about personal things such as childrearing or
among neighbors (e.g., Fischer 1982). We              job openings?” (scale mean = 2.47, sd = .73).
specifically introduce a scale measuring reci-        Overall this measure taps the flow of infor-
procated exchange among neighbors. The                mation exchange and network ties among
items comprising the scale are: “About how            neighbors.
often do you and people in your neighbor-                 Table 5 assesses the new model specifica-
hood do favors for each other? By favors we           tion at both the person and the neighborhood
mean such things as watching each other’s             level. The outcomes are total perceived disor-
children, helping with shopping, lending gar-         der (Model 1), perceived physical disorder
den or house tools, and other small acts of           (Model 2) and perceived social disorder
kindness” (never, rarely, sometimes, or               (Model 3). In every case, both SSO principal
often). “How often do you and people in this          components are positive and statistically sig-
neighborhood have parties or other get-               nificant predictors. Yet for all three out-
togethers where other people in the neigh-            comes, the neighborhood’s social and ethnic

Table 5. Main-Effect Estimates of Neighborhood- and Person-Level Predictors of Perceived Disorder, by
Type, With Principal Components for SSO Disorder/Decay and Alcohol Density, Plus Additional Controls

                                     Model 1                  Model 2                    Model 3
                                 (Total Disorder)        (Physical Disorder)         (Social Disorder)
                                 Coeff.     t–Ratio        Coeff.     t–Ratio         Coeff.     t–Ratio
Neighborhood Level
—Intercept                          .327     1.26             .635     2.45*              .060     .19
—Poverty                            .661     6.28**           .690     5.71**             .662    5.74**
—Black                              .334     5.77**           .250     4.12**             .426    6.09**
—Latino                             .386     5.90**           .329     4.66**             .441    5.74**
—Density                       .003ⴒ10–3      .92        .000ⴒ10–3      .10          .005ⴒ10–3    1.15
—Mixed land use                     .001      .52             .002     1.42              –.001    –.61
—Population size               .016ⴒ10–3     1.31       –.000ⴒ10–3     –.01          .034ⴒ10–3    2.14*
—SSO alcohol density                .040     2.91**           .040     2.57**             .045    2.89**
—SSO disorder/decay                 .080     3.84**           .078     3.45**             .084    3.54**
—Ln (violence rate)                 .116     3.71**           .077     2.51*              .150    3.97**
Person Level
—Female                              .026    1.35              .019     .93               .034    1.40
—Married                             .021     .67             –.026    –.78               .076    2.02*
—Separated/divorced                  .076    2.27*             .045    1.19               .118    3.02**
—Single                              .035    1.03             –.012    –.33               .096    2.3*
—Homeowner                           .039    1.84              .057    2.47*              .023     .86
—Black                              –.164   –4.95**           –.215   –5.97**            –.119   –2.89**
—Latino                             –.077   –2.60**           –.121   –3.84**            –.026    –.68
—Other                              –.128   –3.54**           –.166   –4.03**            –.079   –1.70
—Mobility                            .007    1.03             –.001    –.17               .017    1.99*
—Age                                –.003   –3.67**           –.003   –3.83**            –.002   –1.93
—Unemployed                         –.022   –1.06              .010     .44              –.055   –2.21*
—SES scale                          –.022   –2.64**           –.014   –1.68              –.033   –3.14**
—Exchange/ties                      –.061   –4.24**           –.056   –3.81**            –.068   –3.81**
Level 1 Variance                     .229                      .273                       .352
Level 2 Variance                     .029                      .034                       .034
Notes: N = 3,116 persons and 478 block groups.
*p < .05; **p < .01
#1821—Social Psychology Quarterly—VOL. 67 NO. 4—67401-sampson

332                           SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY
composition remains positively and highly                   isolated. It may be that those enjoying more
significantly related to perceived disorder.                social support are less likely to report more
Even for perceived physical disorder, which                 problems of a wide variety. In any case, the
is the strictest test of our thesis, the estimated          racial pattern remains intact.
effect of racial composition is substantively
large, with controls for observed disorder.                 Cross-Level Interactions With Race/Ethnicity
For example, if we use the model presented
in Table 5 to evaluate how perceived physical                    The results so far support the hypothesis
disorder changes as a function of increasing                that neighborhood racial context helps shape
observed physical disorder/decay and racial                 perceptions of disorder, with controls for
composition from the 25th to the 75th per-                  carefully observed disorder. Does this ten-
centile, the effect of racial composition is                dency depend on the resident’s race? Do the
approximately three times larger than that of               effects of an individual’s race on perceptions
observed disorder/decay, with controls for all              of disorder vary randomly across neighbor-
personal characteristics and neighborhood                   hoods? We tested this specification; once
ecology.11 The magnitude of the differential                again, black residents reported less disorder
by racial composition is so substantial that                than whites (coeff. = –.13, t = –3.11), an effect
measurement error is not a credible explana-                that varied randomly across neighborhoods.
tion, especially in this case, where observed               More interesting, however, the contextual
physical disorder is measured with reliability              effect of racial composition is largely inde-
greater than .90. Also, the simple correlation              pendent of the observer’s ethnicity. 13
of percent black with observed disorder is                  Specifically, blacks were not significantly
positive, as expected: r = .15 for SSO social               more or less likely than whites to view pre-
                                                            dominately black neighborhoods as high in
disorder and r = .11 for SSO physical disorder
                                                            disorder, with controls for observable disor-
(p < .05). The magnitude of the racial compo-
                                                            der and other covariates.
sition effect on perceptions is all the more
                                                                 A notable exception was the interaction
revealing in this light.12
                                                            between Latino ethnicity and block-group
     Two other findings presented in Table 5
                                                            percent black. Perceptions of disorder
are noteworthy. First, the officially reported
                                                            increase as a function of percent black for
violence rate in a block group contributes sig-
                                                            members of each ethnic group, but this ten-
nificantly to residents’ perceptions of disor-
                                                            dency is significantly pronounced for Latinos.
der, in line with Quillian and Pager (2001).
                                                            This relationship is graphed in Figure 1; pre-
Survey-measured reports of personal victim-
                                                            dictors not displayed are held constant at
ization also are linked positively to perceived
                                                            their means. In neighborhoods less than 25
disorder. Yet in neither case is the effect of
                                                            percent black, whites and Latinos essentially
racial composition eliminated. Second, resi-
                                                            do not differ in their perceptions of disorder.
dents embedded in networks of greater reci-
                                                            At roughly 25 percent black, however, a
procal exchange, and thus presumably with
                                                            threshold suggested by prior research as par-
access to better information, perceive less
                                                            ticularly salient (e.g., Schelling 1971), Latinos
disorder than those who are more socially
                                                            begin to diverge sharply from whites: when
                                                            neighborhoods reach 75 percent black or
   11 Moving from the 25th to 75th percentile of            more, Latinos perceive substantially more
observed physical disorder is associated with about a       disorder than do whites (also see Charles
.08 gain in the perceived physical disorder scale.          2000). Overall, as percent black in the block
Moving from the 25th to 75th percentile of percent
black, by contrast, shifts the perceived disorder scale
upward by about .25.
   12                                                          13 In testing cross-level interactions, we estimated a
      We also disaggregated at the item level to exam-
ine exact matches of physical disorder; again we            reduced model that retained significant predictors of
obtained similar results. For example, the t-ratio          total perceived disorder at the neighborhood level
reflecting the effect of racial composition on per-         shown in Table 5. We estimated a series of alternative
ceived graffiti, with controls for observed disorder        models with the same results. We also freed sequen-
(including three types of graffiti) and the other factors   tially the slope variance for each racial/ethnic group;
reported in Table 5, was 5.08 (p < .01). For trash it was   it was significant only for blacks. (Full table results are
3.41 (p < .01).                                             available on request.)
You can also read