The Effects of Music Genre on Scores in Different Exam Types: A Pilot Study

Page created by Linda Rhodes
 
CONTINUE READING
Kwantlen Psychology Student Journal
Issue 3, July 2021
Kwantlen Polytechnic University

              The Effects of Music Genre on Scores in Different Exam
                                          Types: A Pilot Study
                                       Daphnnie Robyn A. Flores1

                                                    Abstract
    The Mozart Effect is a theory that suggests Mozart’s music results in an improvement in spatial
    performance and a change in brain activity. Lo-Fi (or “Low-Fidelity”) music is a new musical
    genre that incorporates elements from different existing musical genres (Casumbal et al., 2019;
    Jaušovec & Habe, 2005). While substantial research has been conducted on the Mozart Effect,
    little research has examined the impact of Lo-Fi music on cognitive performance. Using an
    experimental design with a sample of 86 participants, this study examined whether there were
    performance differences on both spatial ability and reading comprehension test under conditions of
    Classical music (n = 22), Lo-Fi music (n = 32), or no music (n = 32). It was hypothesized that
    participants in both the Classical and Lo-Fi music conditions would have higher scores on both
    the spatial ability and reading comprehension tests compared to the no music condition. The
    results found no differences between musical genre groups for either exam type. This suggests
    that both conditions were neither better nor worse than the silent control. This also seems to
    imply that neither musical condition was too much of a distraction.
           Keywords: Mozart Effect, Classical Music, Lo-Fi Music, Exam Performance, Reading
    Comprehension, Spatial Ability, Background Music

1
    Written for Applied Research I (PSYC 3400); A special thank you to Dr. Shayna Rusticus for guidance and
    recommendation for submitting to the journal.
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                              87

            The Effects of Music Genre on Scores in Different Exam
                                   Types: A Pilot Study
       It is very easy to access different kinds of media and entertainment today, and one great
example of this is music. The consumption of music has been continuously increasing from
about 1 hour and 48 minutes per day in 1999 to around 2 hours and 31 minutes per day in 2010
to probably much higher in 2021 (Harris, 2013). According to several studies, listening to music
while studying and working is a common activity among students (Ballard, 2003; Casumbal et
al., 2019; Harris, 2013). However, these studies show mixed results on whether listening to
music while engaged in academic or cognitive tasks truly results in enhanced or negative effects
on performance, making it difficult to determine its true effect.
The Mozart Effect
       The Mozart Effect suggests that listening to music by Mozart results in an improvement
in spatial performance and a change in brain activity (Jaušovec & Habe, 2005). For quite some
time, Classical music by Mozart (especially the Sonata K448) has been associated with
improved performance in various tasks, including spatial ability (Jaušovec & Habe, 2005;
Rauscher et al., 1994). However, other studies that have attempted to replicate the Mozart Effect
have shown mixed results (Roth & Smith, 2008; Wilson & Brown, 1997). One study identified
that listening to Mozart’s Classical music did lead to increased performance on some spatial
reasoning measures; however, increased performance was also found in the group that had
listened to repetitive relaxation music (Wilson & Brown, 1997). This study concluded that
although the effect of Mozart’s music on spatial reasoning tasks was not as powerful as that of
the findings of Rauscher and colleagues, it can result in some increase in performance on
spatial-related tasks (Wilson & Brown, 1997). Another study by Roth and Smith (2008) found
that both the groups that heard the musical (Mozart, rhythm, and melody conditions) and the
non-musical stimuli (traffic condition) yielded better performance on the GRE’s analytic
section as compared to silence, which did not reflect the Mozart-Specific Effect.
       Critics of the Mozart Effect argue that the positive findings are due to appreciation of
the music by the participants or enjoyment arousal. To provide evidence against the of
enjoyment arousal claim, researchers conducted animal experiments using rats (Jenkins, 2001).
Rats were exposed to one of four conditions before their birth: Sonata K448 by Mozart, Philip
Glass’ minimalist music, white noise, or silence. These rats were then released into a maze and
88                                                                         FLORES, D. R. A.

examined for their ability to complete it. Rats exposed to Mozart’s music completed the maze
significantly faster and more accurately than the other groups. This finding suggests that
enjoyment arousal is not likely to be the source of the Mozart Effect’s influence on task
improvement (Jenkins, 2001).
       Some researchers found that music by certain musicians did not yield results resembling
that of the Mozart Effect, such as the works of Philip Glass, but music by other musicians did,
such as works by a Greek American musician called Yanni (Jenkins, 2001). Therefore, an
assessment was conducted on what characteristics of music produce a similar result to the
Mozart Effect’s influence on task performance. Eventually, it was determined that one of these
musical characteristics was high long-term periodicity—a musical event’s repetition (Jenkins,
2001)—which appears to be a common characteristic found in Lo-Fi music.
Lo-Fi Music
       Lo-Fi, also known as low fidelity music, is a relatively new music genre that involves a
synthesis of elements from other music genres, such as classical, jazz, light hip hop, and many
others (Casumbal et al., 2019). It generally has limited use of vocals or spoken words, a slow
tempo, and highlights soothing instrumental music from instruments such as piano, acoustic
guitar, ukulele, violin, bongos, among many others (Casumbal et al., 2019; Winston &
Saywood, 2019).
       As Lo-Fi music is available in an online format for free; it is quick and easy to access
this kind of music. In fact, when searched on YouTube, a vast array of Lo-Fi channels can be
found from all over the world. Some examples include ChilledCow, which is France-based;
College Music, which is from the United Kingdom; Ambition, which is from the United States
of America; and Mellowbeat Seeker, which is from South Korea (Winston & Saywood, 2019).
The most popular stream for Lo-Fi music is entitled “lofi hip hop radio - beats to relax/study
to” from the YouTube channel, ChilledCow. This stream features an anime girl writing in her
notebook with her wireless earphones on, implying she is also listening to music as she does
her studies (ChilledCow, 2020). According to Winston and Saywood (2019), ChilledCow has
over three million subscribers; however, as of writing this in 2021, the same channel currently
has over seven million subscribers (ChilledCow, n.d.). This rapid increase in subscribers
highlights the popularity of this kind of musical genre. However, given it is a relatively new
genre, existing research on Lo-Fi music and performance is limited.
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                               89

       Casumbal et al. (2019) conducted a study to examine the effects of the presence of lyrics
in Lo-Fi music and font types on recall through a 3 (Lo-Fi with Lyrics, Lo-Fi without Lyrics,
and silence) x 2 (Arial and Sans Forgetica) factorial design. The results showed that Lo-Fi music
without lyrics resulted in significantly better scores on recall as compared to the lyrical
condition, while font alone showed no significant difference on recall.
       Closer in similarity to the present study, Benzonana and de Dios (2020) examined the
effects of different musical genres (Lo-Fi music, Classical music, and silence) on episodic
memory and attention division. From this study, the researchers found no significant differences
between the Lo-Fi music group on episodic memory and attention division in comparison to
the other musical groups (Classical music and silence). Although the results show no significant
results, it would still be interesting to see whether the present study will have the same results
but with reading comprehension and spatial ability rather than episodic memory and attention
division.
Background Music and Exam Types
       Research indicates that many students use music in the background as they engage in
their studies and work (Chou, 2010; Harris, 2013). However, a report presented at the National
Media Education Conference found that 80% of students polled believed the use of media while
studying would decrease their academic performance. In addition, 40% of these students found
that music can at times be a distraction from their work (Ballard, 2003).
       When examining the effects of music on cognitive performance, spatial ability has been
commonly used to measure performance (e.g., Jaušovec & Habe, 2005; Rauscher et al., 1994;
Stephenson, 2001; Wilson & Brown, 1997). For instance, Stephenson (2001) conducted a study
to assess spatial ability performance in different musical conditions, in which he found that all
musical conditions had no significant difference as compared to a silent control. Additional
studies have also assessed the effect of background music on reading comprehension (Adams
& McNair, 2018; Anderson & Fuller, 2010; Chou, 2010; Freeburne & Fleischer, 1952; Harris,
2013). These studies have shown either a decline in performance (Anderson & Fuller, 2010) or
no effect (Freeburne & Fleischer, 1952) when participants were exposed to background music.
However, Chou (2010) identified that the type of music does influence performance on a
cognitive activity. They found that hip hop music served more as a distraction in reading
comprehension performance compared to both a silent control and light Classical music
90                                                                            FLORES, D. R. A.

condition. It was also to their surprise that the control resulted in better performance compared
to the light Classical music condition, as they found opposite results from their own literature
review. This study seems to indicate that background music acts as a distraction for some
cognitive activities, particularly reading comprehension, though this effect is stronger for some
genres and weaker for others.
Purpose of the Present Study
       As described previously, substantial research has been conducted on the Mozart Effect,
particularly in the area of spatial ability performance, wherein participants were exposed to the
music before taking the test (Rauscher et al., 1994; Jaušovec & Habe, 2005; Wilson & Brown,
1997). Given this, it is of interest to examine this but with the music being exposed during the
exams instead of before. Also, unlike studies on the Mozart Effect, the musical genre of Lo-Fi
is quite new in the music scene and limited studies on the topic currently exist. However, given
some similarities between Classical and Lo-Fi musical styles, such as high long-term
periodicity, one might expect them to have similar effects. Therefore, it was hypothesized that
participants who were exposed to either musical condition (Classical music or Lo-Fi) would
perform better on measures of spatial ability and reading comprehension compared to the silent
control condition.
                                               Methods
Participants
      Participants were recruited through the research pool of a medium Western Canadian
university and through snowball sampling via word of mouth, Reddit (r/SampleSize,
r/SurveyExchange, and r/takemysurvey), and the Survey Exchange Facebook Group. A total
of 112 participants were recruited; however, 26 were removed due to incomplete or blank
responses, resulting in a final sample of 86 participants. The Classical group consisted of 22
participants, while both the Lo-Fi and control groups consisted of 32 participants each.
      Among the 86 participants, 68.6% were female, 27.9% were male, 1.16% were non-
binary, and 2.33% preferred not to say. The participants had various ethnicities with Asian
(50.0%) and Caucasian (27.9%) as the majority. In addition, these participants came from
different years in their studies: 15.1% were in their first year, 26.7% were in their second
year, 26.7% were in their third year, 19.8% were in their fourth year, and 11.6% selected
“other.” Students taking psychology courses were granted bonus credit toward eligible
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                               91

courses for their participation. Students in non-applicable courses and community participants
did not receive compensation.
Materials
       There were three conditions in the study: Classical music, Lo-Fi music, or No Music.
Specifically, the Classical music used in this study was Mozart’s “Sonata K. 448” (Classical
Music, 2014) and the Lo-Fi music used was the live YouTube stream by ChilledCow called
“lofi hip hop radio—beats to relax/study to” (ChilledCow, 2020). The control group completed
the exams in silence.
       The dependent variable of this study was participant performance on two types of
exams: a spatial ability test (Newton & Bristol, n.d.a) and a reading comprehension test
(Newton & Bristol, n.d.b). Three out of four passages from the original reading comprehension
exam (Newton & Bristol, n.d.b) were presented in this study’s questionnaire, making this
section worth a total of 15 rather than 20 points. Each passage had five questions associated
with it. After reading a passage, participants were to answer the questions by selecting one of
three options: true, false, or can’t say. The “can’t say” option refers to when they believe there
was not enough information within the passage to truly be sure whether the statement was true
or false. Similarly, 15 out of the 45 questions were selected from the original spatial ability
exam (Newton & Bristol, n.d.a); there were four sections. In the first section, participants were
presented with five diagrams. They were instructed to find the diagram that was identical to the
first but had been rotated. In the second section, participants were presented with four diagrams,
and they decided which among them would form the shape from the reference image. In the
third section, participants were presented with four images of a disassembled cube with
different patterns, of which they were asked to select one that would match the assembled cube.
In the last section, participants were shown a map of a small area, which they must use to answer
a small set of questions. The reduction of questions from the original tests was done in order to
have approximately the same amount of focus on each exam type on the questionnaire as well
as to prevent the likelihood of high experimental mortality.
Procedure
       All data was collected anonymously and online through Qualtrics. After providing
electronic consent, participants were directed to a set of demographic questions (age, gender,
ethnicity, year in university, whether they listen to music while they study, and whether they
92                                                                              FLORES, D. R. A.

listen to music during online exams or tests for online classes). They were then randomly
assigned to one of the three conditions and shown instructions about what to do with the
embedded video (if they were in one of the two music conditions) and to prepare their earphones
or headphones. The following was stated in the instructions:
            Please prepare your earphones or headphones now. Once you click to
            proceed to the next page, you may see a video embedded at the very top of
            the questionnaire. If you do find a video, please play it and let it run with
            the audio turned on as you complete the rest of the questionnaire. If you
            do not find a video, please continue on with the questionnaire in silence.
            You may continue to the next page when you are ready.
          Once they proceeded to the next page, the questionnaire began. The order of the exam
type was randomized, with some answering the reading comprehension exam first and others
the spatial ability exam first. After answering the questionnaires, participants were brought to
a page with a debriefing statement, informing them about the research study that they had just
completed.
                                             Results
Analysis
          Data was analyzed using two one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA), one for each
exam type (reading comprehension and spatial ability). All analyses were conducted in IBM
SPSS Statistics (Version 24). Prior to analysis, the following assumptions were checked:
normality and homogeneity of variance. Normality was checked by conducting a skewness and
kurtosis analysis, while homogeneity of variance was assessed by running a Levene’s Test of
Homogeneity of Variances as well as Welch and Brown-Forsythe tests if the Levene’s test
failed.
          The assumption for spatial ability’s homogeneity of variance were met, whereas the
assumption for reading comprehension’s homogeneity of variance was not met. Given this, a
Welch correction was used for the reading comprehension analysis. An examination of
skewness and kurtosis indicated that all assumptions of normality were met.
Result of ANOVAs
          The results of the one-way ANOVA for the reading comprehension exam, with a Welch
correction, showed no statistically significant difference between musical genre groups, F(2,
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                                 93

46.8) = 2.27, p = .114, η2 = .08. However, the medium effect size suggested that there may be
a difference and that the study did not have sufficient power to detect this. A follow-up Games-
Howell post-hoc test was conducted due to its medium effect size. However, the results still
showed no difference between the conditions (p > .10).
       The one-way ANOVA for spatial ability also indicated that there was no statistically
significant difference between musical genre groups, F(2,83) = 1.28, p = .285, η2 = .03. This
analysis had a small effect size. See Table 1 for the means and standard deviations for the
reading comprehension and spatial ability tests.
                                          Discussion
       Overall, the hypothesis that Classical and Lo-Fi musical conditions would have
increased music increases performance in spatial ability and reading comprehension were not
supported. Based on the mean scores, there was a non-significant decrease in reading
comprehension exam performance in the Classical music condition compared to the control and
Lo-Fi conditions. There was also a non-significant increase in spatial ability exam performance
from the control to the Lo-Fi conditions, and from the Lo-Fi to Classical conditions. However,
the results indicate that these means are not statistically different enough to conclude a
significant increase or decrease in performance. Therefore, it appears that both musical
conditions (Classical and Lo-Fi) resulted in neither poorer nor enhanced reading comprehension
or spatial ability performance, suggesting neither condition was better nor worse than the
control. This seems to also indicate that neither musical condition was too distracting.
       Altogether, the results of the present study support the findings of Freeburne and
Fleischer (1952), Stephenson (2001), as well as Benzonana and de Dios (2020). As previously
mentioned, Freeburne and Fleischer (1952) assessed reading comprehension performance
between different musical genre groups, in which they found no significant differences between
groups. Similarly, Stephenson (2001) conducted a study to look at the effect of background
music on spatial ability performance, which did not indicate any statistically significant
differences between musical groups either. The study by Benzonana and de Dios (2020), of
which had consisted of the same musical groups as the present study (Lo-Fi music, Classical
music, and silence), examined their effect on episodic memory and attention division, they also
found no statistically significant differences.
       Based on the results of this study, it appears that neither Lo-Fi music nor Classical music
94                                                                              FLORES, D. R. A.

led to a significant change in reading comprehension or spatial ability performance as compared
to silence. However, this conclusion must be taken with a grain of salt as there are some
limitations in this study.
Limitations
        A rather significant limitation of this study was the low control the researcher had over
what the participants saw and how they responded due to the online nature of this study, which
was mandatory due to the COVID-19 pandemic. This was a problem as the researcher did not
know whether certain responses were taken seriously and answered properly, nor whether the
audio was working properly on the participants’ end. Although participants were instructed to
use headphones or earphones during the study, the researcher had no way to be certain if all
participants had the same listening experience, which also may act as an extraneous variable.
Another limitation was experimental mortality, which may be due to the length of the overall
questionnaire as well as its exam-like nature. In addition, this study may have been
underpowered, possibly due to its small sample size. In future studies, it should be a goal to
obtain data from a much larger sample. Additionally, the Lo-Fi music used in this study varied
depending on when the participants completed the study as it was from a live stream video,
which may have also affected the results. Although the Lo-Fi YouTube channel used here is
the most popular, it might be best to utilize a single Lo-Fi song of the same length as the
Classical music condition to maintain consistency for future replication studies. It must also be
acknowledged that most of the previous research on the Mozart Effect exposed participants to
the musical conditions before the exam (Rauscher et al., 1994; Roth & Smith, 2008; Wilson &
Brown, 1997), whereas these were done simultaneously in the present study. The presence of
background music during the testing might have served as a noise distraction for some
individuals rather than the “Mozart Effect” in the brain’s cognition if this effect is truly present.
        Despite these weaknesses, there were also some strengths. For instance, previous studies
typically focus on only one exam type, whereas this study compared two exam types. This is
beneficial as it makes it easier to properly compare the performance on each exam type as
everything else is kept constant, preventing possible alternative explanations.
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                     95

                                       Tables
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Reading Comprehension and Spatial Ability Exams

                                              M                        SD
Reading Comprehension
       Control                               7.00                     2.60
       Lo-Fi                                 7.00                     2.26
       Classical                             5.18                     3.71
Spatial Ability
       Control                               8.63                     4.05
       Lo-Fi                                 9.31                     3.43
       Classical                            10.23                     3.21
96                                                                          FLORES, D. R. A.

                                        References
Adams, O., & McNair, M. (2018). Noise and neurons: Effects of background music on
       reading comprehension. Proceedings of the 2018 International Summit of the Music &
       Entertainment Industry Educators Association, 142–152.
       https://doi.org/10.25101/18.36
Anderson, S. A., & Fuller, G. B. (2010). Effect of music on reading comprehension of junior
       high school students. School Psychology Quarterly, 25(3), 178–187.
       https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021213
Ballard, K. D. (2003). Media habits and academic performance: Elementary and middle
       school students’ perceptions. Proceedings of the National Media Education
       Conference, Baltimore, MD. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED479205
Benzonana, P., & de Dios, I. R. (2020). “lofi hip hop radio – beats to relax/study to”: A Study
       on the effects of Lo-Fi as background music on episodic memory and attention
       division. [Master’s Thesis, University of Amsterdam].
       https://scripties.uba.uva.nl/search?id=712252;setlang=en
Casumbal, K. J. S., Chan, C. K. T., de Guzman, F. Y. V., Fernandez, N. V. G., Ng, A. V. N.,
       & So, M. C. (2019). The effects of Low-Fidelity music and font style on recall.
       ResearchGate. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.31182.41286
Chou, P. T.-M. (2010). Attention drainage effect: How background music effects
       concentration in Taiwanese college students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching
       and Learning, 10(1), 36–46. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ882124
ChilledCow (n.d.). Lofi Girl [YouTube channel]. YouTube. Retrieved February 17, 2021,
       from https://www.youtube.com/channel/UCSJ4gkVC6NrvII8umztf0Ow
ChilledCow. (2020, February 22). lofi hip hop radio—Beats to relax/study to [Video].
       YouTube. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5qap5aO4i9A
Classical Music. (2014, March 2). Mozart—Sonata for two pianos in D major, K. 448
       [Video]. YouTube https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=C5sc7_MjQmY
Freeburne, C. M., & Fleischer, M. S. (1952). The effect of music distraction upon reading rate
       and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 43(2), 101–109.
       https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054219
Harris, M. J. (2013). The effects of music mode on actual and perceived school-related task
MUSIC GENRE AND EXAM TYPES                                                            97

       performance (Publication No. 3579975.) [Doctoral dissertation, Fairleigh Dickinson
       University]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.
       https://www.proquest.com/openview/53db70d83dd349f5ceabc26ba2c2950a
Jaušovec, N., & Habe, K. (2005). The influence of Mozart’s sonata K. 448 on brain activity
       during the performance of spatial rotation and numerical tasks. Brain Topography,
       17(4), 207–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-005-6030-4
Jenkins, J. S. (2001). The Mozart Effect. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 94(4),
       170–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/014107680109400404
Newton, P., & Bristoll, H. (n.d.a). Spatial Ability Practice Test 1 [Measurement Instrument].
       Retrieved from https://psychometric-success.com/downloads/download-spatial-ability-
       practice-tests
Newton, P., & Bristoll, H. (n.d.b). Verbal Comprehension Practice Test 1 [Measurement
       Instrument]. Retrieved from https://psychometric-success.com/downloads/download-
       verbal-comprehension-practice-tests
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., & Ky, K. N. (1994). Music and spatial task
       performance: A causal relationship. Proceedings from the 1994 American
       Psychological Association 102nd Annual Convention, Los Angeles, CA.
       https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED390733
Roth, E. A., & Smith, K. H. (2008). The Mozart Effect: Evidence for the arousal hypothesis.
       Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107(2), 396–402. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.107.2.396-
       402
Stephenson, V. P. (2001). The effect of classical background music on spatial reasoning skills
       as measured by completion of a spatial task: A study of selected college
       undergraduates (Publication No. 3043266) [Doctoral dissertation, University of
       Idaho]. Proquest Dissertations Publishing.
       https://www.proquest.com/openview/dda79c9b23ca2e1079450d5b7447858e/
Wilson, T. L., & Brown, T. L. (1997). Reexamination of the effect of Mozart's music on
       spatial-task performance. The Journal of Psychology, 131(4), 365–370.
       https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989709603522
Winston, E., & Saywood, L. (2019). Beats to relax/study to: Contradiction and paradox in lofi
       hip hop. IASPM Journal, 9(2), 40–54. https://doi.org/10.5429/2079-
       3871(2019)v9i2.4en
You can also read