Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis and examination of six moderator variables

Page created by Valerie Montgomery
 
CONTINUE READING
British Journal of Social Psychology (1998),37, 231-250   Printed in Great Britain                     231
0 1998 The British Psychological Society

      Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-
      analysis and examination of six moderator
                      variables

                                Paschal Sheeran* and Sheina Orbell
                    Department of Pyhology, University of Shefield, Shefield S10 ZTN, UK

            This study used meta-analysis to quantify the relationship between intentions and
            behaviour in prospective studies of condom use. The effects of six moderator
            variables were also examined : sexual orientation, gender, sample age, time interval,
            intention versus expectation and condom use with ‘steady’ versus ‘casual’
            partners. Literature searches revealed 28 hypotheses based on a total sample of 2532
            which could be included in the review. Overall, there was a medium to strong
            sample-weighted average correlation between intentions and condom use (r+=
            .44), and this correlation was similar to the effect sizes obtained in previous reviews.
            There were too few studies of gay men to permit meaningful comparison of effect
            sizes between homosexual versus heterosexual samples. Gender and measurement
            of intention did not moderate the intention-behaviour relationship. However,
            shorter time intervals, older samples and condom use with ‘steady’ rather than
            ‘casual ’ partners were each associated with stronger correlations between intentions
            and condom use. Factors which might explain the significant effects of moderator
            variables are discussed and implications of the study for future research on
            intention-behaviour consistency are outlined.

Unprotected penetrative sex is the primary transmission route for human
immunodeficiency virus (HIV), the agent which causes AIDS (acquired immuno-
deficiency syndrome). Condom use can prevent sexual transmission of HIV and is
more effective than reducing numbers of sexual partners (Reiss & Leik, 1989).
Although time trend analyses show that condom use has increased among both
heterosexuals (e.g. Catania, Stone, Binson & Dolcini, 1995; DeVroome, Paalman,
Dinglestad, Kolker & Sandfort, 1994; Robertson, 1995) and gay men (e.g. Flowers,
Sheeran, Smith & Beail, 1997; Hospers & Kok, 1995; Stall, Coates & Hoff, 1988),
the absolute level of condom use remains low. For example, a nationally
representative survey of people in the UK and France found that 40-60% of the
sexually active sample had never used a condom in the previous 12 months (Bajos e t
al., 1995).
    Social psychology can contribute to reducing the spread of HIV/AIDS by
identifying psychological prerequisites of HIV-preventive behaviours such as
condom use (Abraham & Sheeran, 1993, 1994). There have been several applications

*   Requests for reprints.
232                            Paschal Sbeeran and Sbeina Orbell
of social psychological models of behaviour to condom use. These accounts propose
that a person’s intention to use a condom is the most immediate, and important,
predictor of that behaviour. Given the complexity of sexual behaviour, however, it
remains an open question whether intentions do indeed predict future condom use.
The present study uses meta-analysis (e.g. Rosenthal, 1984) to address this question
by quantifying the extent to which behavioural intentions have been associated with
condom use in prospective studies to date.

Social pybological models of condom use
Perhaps the most important social psychological models of behaviour that have been
applied to condom use are the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980)
and the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1985, 1991). These models specify
different predictors of intention. According to the theory of reasoned action, people’s
intentions to perform a behaviour are predictable from their attitude towards the
behaviour, their positive or negative evaluation of their performing the behaviour
(e.g. ‘For me, using a condom would be good/bad’), and from their subjective norm,
their beliefs about what significant others think that they should do (e.g. ‘Most
people who are important to me think that I should/should not use a condom’). The
theory of planned behaviour posits an additional variable which influences intention
and which may also directly affect behaviour : perceived behavioural control (Ajzen &
Madden, 1986). Perceived behavioural control refers to the person’s perceptions of
the ease or difficulty of performing the behaviour (e.g. ‘Whether or not I use a
condom is entirely under/outside my control ’) and is closely related to the notion of
self-eflcacy (Bandura, 1992; see, however, Terry & O’Leary, 1995 for a study of the
distinctiveness of the two constructs).
   While the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour differ on the
proposed determinants of intention, both models regard forming an intention (e.g.
‘I intend using a condom the next time I have sex with someone new’) as the
prerequisite of behavioural performance. Behavioural intentions are presumed to
mediate the effects of variables extraneous to the models such as demographic
characteristics as well as attitudes and subjective norms (though not necessarily
perceived behavioural control). The intention construct therefore provides a
summary of the person’s motivational orientation towards performing a behaviour.
Ajzen (1991), for example, states that:
      Intentions are assumed to capture the motivational factors that influence a behavior; they are
      indicators of how hard people are willing to try, of how much effort they are planning to exert,
      in order to perform the behavior (p. 181).

  Previous meta-analytic reviews have shown that intentions are good predictors of
behaviours in a variety of domains. Across 88 studies, Sheppard, Hartwick &
Warshaw (1988) obtained an average correlation of .53 between intention and
behaviour, while Randall & Wolff’s (1994) review of 98 studies obtained an average
correlation of .45. While these findings are encouraging, there are reasons to suspect
that intentions may not predict condom use as well as intentions predict other
behaviours. Condom use is less an individual than a joint behaviour which requires
Intentions and condom use                            233
the cooperation of a sexual partner (Kashima, Gallois & McCamish, 1993).
Because sexual partners may have different intentions regarding condom use,
intentions obtained from one partner may not be predictive of their joint behaviour.
Condom use also requires resources (e.g. having a condom available) and opportunity
(e.g. a prospective sexual partner). Both of these factors are thought to attenuate the
relationship between intentions and behaviour (Liska, 1984).
   The first aim of the present study, therefore, is to systematically examine the extent
to which behavioural intentions are associated with condom use among heterosexual
and gay men using meta-analytical procedures. The second aim is to examine a
number of potential moderators of the intentionxondom use relationship.
Specifically, sample factors such as sexual orientation, gender and age, and
methodological issues such as the time interval between measures of intention and
behaviour and the measurement of both intention (intention versus expectation) and
condom use (‘casual ’ versus ‘steady ’ partner) could each influence the relationship
between intentions and condom use.

Sexual orientation
Since there are no theoretical grounds for supposing that the correlations between
intentions and condom use should differ for gay versus heterosexual samples, no
predictions were made regarding the influence of this variable.

Gender
Several researchers have suggested that men possess greater power in heterosexual
relationships than women (Holland, Ramazaglou & Scott, 1990a; Holland,
Ramazaglou, Scott, Sharpe & Thomson, 19906; Wight, 1992). This may mean that
women are less able to translate their intentions to use a condom into action than
men. Consistent with this view, Abraham, Sheeran, Abrams & Spears (1996) found
that while intentions were significantly associated with condom use among men in
their sample, the correlation between intention and condom use was not significant
among women.
   Researchers have argued that sexual scripts (Gagnon & Simon, 1974) which
provide implicit understandings of gender-appropriate roles and behaviours in sexual
contexts accord men a more ‘agentic’ role in terms of initiating and coordinating
sexual relations (Rose & Frieze, 1989) and that men’s sexual pleasure is privileged in
such scripts (Nicolson, 1993). Women may also face emotional and/or physical
coercion from men when they try to use a condom. Experiences of frequently being
‘pressured’ to engage in intercourse by men have been reported by women from a
wide variety of backgrounds (Biglan, Noel, Ochs, Smolkowski & Metzler, 1995;
Holland et al., 19906) and these reports of pressured intercourse are associated with
condom non-use (Biglan et al., 1995).
   A related consideration is women’s self-efficacy to use condoms. Morrison,
Rogers Gillmore & Baker (1995) point out that a woman depends more on a male
partner’s cooperation than vice versa, since using a condom is a behaviour that he,
rather than she, performs. This lack of direct control over the behaviour may have
234                         Paschal Sbeeran and Sbeina Orbell
a negative impact upon women’s self-efficacy to use condoms (Kasen, Vaughan &
Walter, 1992). Since self-efficacy or perceived behavioural control can have a direct
impact upon behaviour which is not mediated by intention (Ajzen & Madden, 1986),
this might mean that women are less able to act upon their intentions to use a
condom than men.

Evidence also suggests that adolescents may be less able to translate their intentions
to use condoms into action compared to undergraduate and adult samples (Fisher,
Fisher & Rye, 1995). Two factors might be responsible. First, intentions to use
condoms may be very unstable among this group. For example, Stanton et al. (1996)
found that 58 per cent of their sample of 9-15-year-olds changed their intentions over
a six-month interval. Similarly, Reinecke, Schmidt & Ajzen (1996) found relatively
small correlations (.39 < rs < .46) between measures of intention taken one year
apart among a representative sample of German youth. This temporal inconsistency
is important because unstable intentions have been found to attenuate the relationship
between intentions and behavioural performance (Bagozzi & Yi, 1989; Doll 8c
Ajzen, 1992).
    Second, age and sexual experience are positively correlated (Dunne, Donald,
Lucke, Nilsson, Ballard & Raphael, 1994). This means that older samples are likely
to have greater knowledge of sexual scripts and greater condom use self-efficacy and
may be better able to implement their intentions to use a condom. Kashima e t a/.
(1993) have shown that previous experience of condom use increases the consistency
between intentions and subsequent use. In their study, intenders with direct
experience of condom use were more likely to use a condom than intenders who had
no prior experience. Thus, intention stability and lack of direct experience may mean
that adolescents’ intentions are less predictive of condom use than the intentions of
older samples.

Time interval between measurements of intention and behaviour
Ajzen (1985) and Ajzen & Fishbein (1980) have argued that stronger relationships
between intention and behaviour will be obtained when the time interval between the
two measures is shorter than when it is longer. The time interval may influence the
intention-behaviour correlation because intentions may become unstable over time
or because unforeseen obstacles prevent action (Ajzen, 1985; Cote, McCullough &
Reilly, 1985). In a meta-analytic review of this issue, however, Randall & Wolff
(1994) found no significant relationship between the length of delay between
assessment of intention and behaviour and the strength of the intention-behaviour
correlation (r = - .06, n.s.).
   Examination of the data employed in Randall & Wolffs (1994) study suggests
caution in accepting their conclusion that time interval does not affect intention-
behaviour relations. Randall & Wolff examined 98 hypotheses which were distributed
across five time intervals (less than one day, less than one week, less than one month,
less than one year, greater than one year) and seven ‘types of behaviour’
Intentions and condom use                       235
(food/beverage, sexual/reproductive, drug/alcohol, political/voting, leisure/
exercise, school/work/job/career, and ‘other behaviours’). This yields a 5 x 7
matrix of time interval by behaviour type. Inspection of the numbers of behaviours
in each cell reveals that there are no data available for 10 of the cells while a further
8 cells contain just one datum. Thus, time interval and behaviour type would seem
to be confounded.
   Randall & Wolff also analysed the impact of time interval within each behaviour
type (excluding ‘other behaviours’) and found a significant association between time
interval and the intention-behaviour correlation for just one of the six behaviour
types-drugs/alcohol behaviours. Even within each behaviour type, however, there
is at least one empty time interval cell for each of the six types of behaviour. Since
the missing time interval cell varies for different types of behaviour, time interval and
behaviour type would again seem to be confounded.
   We would argue that a stronger test of the effects of temporal contiguity on
intention-behaviour relations would be provided by examining the effects of time
interval in the context of a single behaviour than in the context of several different
behaviours of the same ‘type’. Consistent with Azjen’s (1985) analysis, we
hypothesize that longer time intervals will attenuate the strength of the
intention-condom use relationship here.

Behavioural intention versus behavioural expectation
Sheppard et a/. (1988) and Warshaw & Davis (1985) have drawn attention to a
distinction between behavioural intentions and behavioural expectations. Whereas
intention refers to what one intends or plans to do (e.g. ‘ I intend using a condom
the next time I have sexual intercourse 7, behavioural expectation refers to self-
predictions about what one is likely to do (e.g. ‘How likely is it that you will use a
condom the next time you have sexual intercourse?’). Measures of behavioural
expectation are thought to encompass people’s perceptions of factors which may
impede performance of a behaviour, such as situational constraints or lack of ability,
and may therefore provide better predictors of behaviour than traditional measures
of intention (Warshaw & Davis, 1985). Support for this view comes from Sheppard
e t al.’s (1988) meta-analysis of the theory of reasoned action. They found that
behavioural expectations were more strongly correlated with behaviour than
behavioural intentions. Randall & Wolff (1994), on the other hand, found that
intention versus expectation did not moderate the relationship between time interval
and the intention-behaviour correlation.

‘ Casual’ versus ‘steah ’ partner
Sheeran & Abraham (1994) showed that measures of condom use employed in most
studies of HIV-preventive behaviour do not specify the type of partner (e.g. ‘new ’,
‘casual’ or ‘steady’ partner) with whom a condom was used. Research suggests,
however, that intentions may be better predictors of condom use with ‘steady’ or
‘regular’ partners than condom use with ‘casual’ or ‘new’ partners. Morrison e t a/.
(1995) argued that the theory of reasoned action should better predict condom use
236                              Paschal Sheeran and Sheina Orbell
among steady partners than casual partners because the beliefs and attitudes of casual
partners are less well known to the actor, leading to ‘greater ambiguity in the
formation of, and follow-through on, intentions to use condoms’ (p. 654). Findings
appear to support this view. Morrison et af. (1995) and Galligan & Terry (1993) both
found that condom use was more predictable for steady partners than for casual
partners.

The present   stub
In summary, the present study uses meta-analysis to determine the strength of the
relationship between intentions and condom use among heterosexual and gay
respondents. The effects of six potential moderators of this relationship are also
examined : (i) sexual orientation, (ii) gender, (iii) age, (iv) time interval, (v) intention
versus expectation and (vi) type of partner.

                                               Method
Sample of studies
Several methods were used to generate the sample of studies: (u) computerized searches of social
scientific and medical databases (PsychLit, PsychINFO, Social Science Citation Index (BIDS), Medline,
Index Medicus, AIDSline, Dissertation Abstracts Online and the Conference Papers Index) from the
first report of HIV/AIDS (January 1981) to the time of writing (May 1997), (b) reference lists in each
article identified above were evaluated for inclusion, and (c) the authors of published articles were
contacted and requests were made for unpublished studies and studies in press.
   There were several inclusion criteria for the review:
1. Studies had to include a measure of intention and a measure of self-reported condom use. Studies
   which did not disaggregate condom use from other measures of HIV-preventive behaviour, such as
   abstinence or non-penetrative sex, were excluded. While these studies are informative, as DiClemente
   (1992) points out, composite measures of HIV-preventive behaviour mean that the effects of
   predictors as they specifically relate to condom use cannot be isolated. Studies which did not
   disaggregate condom use from other measures of contraception were also excluded for this reason.
2. A bivariate statistical relationship between intention and condom use had to be retrievable from
   studies. Where studies did not include relevant statistics, the authors of the study were contacted and
   requests were made for bivariate associations. Almost all authors provided these data (Boldero,
   Moore & Rosenthal, 1992; Morrison, 1993; Morrison et ul., 1995; Reinecke etul., 1995; Rye, 1995;
   White, Terry & Hogg, 1994).
3. Studies had to measure intention at time Toand measure condom use behaviour at some later time
   T,. Studies which reported contemporaneous measures of intentions and behaviour were excluded
   because cross-sectional designs do not permit causal inferences (Basen-Engquist, 1992; Basen-
   Engquist & Parcel, 1992; Brown, DiClemente & Park, 1992; Cochran, Mays, Ciarletta, Caruso &
   Mallon, 1992; Hernandez & DiClemente, 1992; Jemmott & Jemmott, 1991; Macey & Boldero,
   1992; Schaalma, Kok & Peters, 1993; Trefie, Juggemann & Ross, 1992).
   Using these inclusion criteria, a total of 28 tests of the association between intention and condom use
could be used in the review. Of these, just two hypotheses came from samples of gay men. The remainder
involved exclusively or predominantly heterosexual samples. The 18 studies which yielded the 28 effect
sizes are preceded by an asterisk in the reference list. These 18 studies include 2 unpublished papers
(yielding three hypotheses: Morrison, 1993; Rye, 1995).
   Study characteristics were coded independently by the authors. Reliabilities were uniformly high,
ranging from 94 to 100 per cent. Disagreements were jointly resolved. Table 1 presents the
characteristics and effect sizes obtained from each study.
1. Studies of the relationship between behavioural intentions and heterosexual condom use

                                                                                   Time
                                                                                  interval                            Respondent
Author(s)                Sample              Intention versus expectation         (weeks)           Type of partner       sex       N     r

m, Sheeran,      Random sample of       Intention, 1 item (‘In future, I intend     52       ‘New ’ partner            Women        81   .15
s & Spears        heterosexual            to use a condom if I have sex with                                            Men         41   .33
                  adolescents (16-19      someone new ’; 5-point scale,
                  years)                  ‘strongly agree ’ to ‘strongly
                                         disagree’)
 , Moore &       Heterosexual           Intention“, 1 item (‘Strength of             6       Not specified             Women        95   .31
thal (1992)       undergraduates         intention’, 5-point scale, ‘very                                               Men         49   .40
                                         determined not to use a condom’ to
                                         ‘very determined to use a condom’)
 ell, Millward   Random sample of       Expectation, 2 items, [‘7-point scale       52       Not specified
e-Shaw (1994)     heterosexual           (‘I do not expect to have sex’ to ‘I
                                                                                                                                               I
                  adolescents (16-20     definitely will do this’ for two                                                                      P
                  years)                 instances of condom use) (always use                                                                  a.
                                         condoms/use condoms when not
                                         certain about the other person’s
                                         sexual history) ’1
                                                                                                                                               2:
Aiken &          Heterosexual college   Expectation, 4 items (e.g. ‘How              6       Not specified             Women        81   .69   2
(1996)            students               likely is it that you will use a
                                         condom the next time you have
                                         intercourse? ’, response options not
                                         reported), alpha = .77
van              Gay men attending      Intention, 1 item (‘Do you intend           26       I.   Casual ’ partner      Men        244   .21
ven, Kok          Municipal Health       to use a condom when you have
rt (1993)         Clinic in Amsterdam    anal intercourse with a casual
                  (mean age = 41.2       partner in the future?’; 5-point
                  years)                 scale; ‘certainty not’ to ‘yes,
                                         certainly ’)
1984)            Heterosexual           Expectation, 1 item (‘unlikely-likely        4       Not specified              Men        44    .55
                  undergraduates         I will always use condoms’; number                                                                    P;,
                                                                                                                                               CJ
                                         of points on the scale not specified)                                                                 4
1. (cont.)                                                                                                                                N
                                                                                                                                          w
                                                                                                                                          00

                                                                                 Time
                                                                                interval                         Respondent
 uthor(s)             Sample               Intention versus expectation         (weeks)       Type of partner        sex      N      1

Fisher &      Convenience sample      Intention, 1 item (‘If I have insertive      8       Not specified            Men       29d   .59
995)”          of gay men              anal intercourse in the next two
               recruited from gay      months, I intend to always use latex
               organizations           condoms’; 5-point scale, ‘very
                                       likely’ to ‘very unlikely’)
                                      Intention, 1 item (‘If I have receptive      8       Not specified
                                       anal intercourse in the next two
                                       months, I intend to always use latex
                                       condoms ’; 5-point scale, ‘very
                                       likely’ to ‘very unlikely’)
              Heterosexual            Intention, 1 item (‘If I have sex            8       Not specified
               undergraduates          during the next two months, I
                                       intend to always use latex
                                       condoms’; 5-point scale, ‘very
                                       likely’ to ‘very unlikely’)
              Heterosexual 9th        Intention, 1 item (‘If I have sex            4       Notspecified
               grade high school       during the next two months, I                                               Men        29    .ll   0
               pupils (adolescents)    intend to always use latex                                                                         PP
                                       condoms ’; 5-point scale, ‘very
                                       likely’ to ‘very unlikely’)
n & Terry     Heterosexual            Expectation, 1 item (‘Over the next         12       ‘Regular’ partnersc     Mixed      50    .63
               undergraduates          three months I will definitely use         12       ‘Casual/new’            Mixed      27    .38
                                       condoms with regular (casual/new)                   partners
                                       partners’; 7-point scale, ‘very
                                       unlikely’ to ‘very likely’)
, Kashima,    Predominantly           Intention, 1 item (‘Whether they             8       Notspecified            Mixed      144   .49
  McCamish,    heterosexual            intended to use a condom during
ins &          convenience sample      their next sexual encounter’; 7-point
 in (1992)     obtained through        scale, ‘definitely not intend’ to
               student groups and      ‘definitely intend ’)
               social networks
s, Terry,        Heterosexual             Intention, 1 item (‘Whether they          8   Not specified    Women     91      .60
 ins, Kashima     undergraduates           intended to perform their sexual                               Men      70      .53
cCamish (1994)                             activities with themselves or their
                                           partner using a condom on their
                                           next sexual encounter’; 7-point
                                           scale, ‘definitely do not intend’ to
                                           ‘definitely intend ’)
on (1993)        Heterosexual teenagers Expectation, 1 item (‘How likely are       12   Casual partner   Women”    43      .18
                  at sexually             you to use condoms with your                  Steady partner   Women    140      .49
                  transmitted diseases    steady/casual partner(s) over the             Casual partner    Men      32      .32
                  (STD) clinics and       next 3 months?’, ‘very unlikely’ to           Steady partner    Men      77      .45
                  juvenile detention      ‘very likely’)
                  centres
on, Rogers       Heterosexual adult     Expectation, 1 item (‘How likely are       12   Casual partner   Women     38      .28
 ore & Baker      STD clinic attenders   you to use condoms with your                   Steady partner   Women    163      .31
)                 (mean age = 27.7       steady/casual partner(s) over the              Casual partner    Men      52      .26
                  years)                 next 3 months?’; ‘very unlikely’ to            Steady partner    Men     105      .49
                                         ‘very likely’)
ke, Schmidt      Random household       Intention, 3 items (‘I insist on using     52   ‘New’ partners   Mixed    172    .22 1.24,
ten (1996)        survey of adolescents a condom with new sexual partners                                               .20, .22]’
                  (predominantly         even if my partner does not want
                  heterosexual)          to’; 3-point scale, ‘yes, true’, ‘don’t
                                         know’, ‘no, false’)
995)             Predominantly          Intention, no details                      8    Not specified    Women    56    .55 [.50,
                  heterosexual                                                                                            .60]’
                  undergraduates
son &            Predominantly          Expectation, 2 items (e.g. ‘How            12   Not specified    Mixed    85       .66
ott (1996)        heterosexual           likely is it that you will use
                  undergraduates         condoms if you decide to have sex
                                         in the next 3 months?’; 5-point
                                         scale, ‘very unlikely ’ to ‘very
                                         likely’), alpha = .72
1. (cont.)

                                                                                             Time
                                                                                            interval                            Respondent
Author(s)                    Sample                    Intention versus expectation         (weeks)      Type of partner            sex          N          f
                                               ~        ~~          ~             ~~            ~

 , Li, Black,       Heterosexual young             Expectation, 1 item (‘How likely is it      24         Not specified            Mixed         24       .05
 o, Galbraith,       people aged 9-15               that you will use a condom the next
lman & Kaljee        years in public                time you have sex?’; %point scale,
                     housing                        ‘likely’, ‘uncertain’, ‘unlikely’)
                     developments
r Velde,            Heterosexual STD               Intention, 1 item (Intend to use            16      ‘Private’ partners*         Mixed        100       .35         \

kaas &               clinic attenders (age          condoms with private/prostitution          16        ‘Prostitution ’           Mixed        147       .42         3
er Pligt             > 17 years)                    contacts; 5-point scale, ‘definitely                     partners                                                 2
                                                    no’ to ‘definitely yes’)                                                                                          a\a
Terry &             Heterosexual                   Intention, 3 items (e.g. ‘I intend to        4         Not specified            Mixed        164       .80         P
(1994)               university students            use a condom every time I have sex                                                                                5
                                                    during the next month’; 7-point                                                                                   s
                                                    scale, extremely unlikely’ to
                                                             ’                                                                                                        2.
                                                    ‘extremely likely’), alpha = .96                                                                                  3
                                                                                                                                                                      0
 0 e t ul. (1992) employed two measures of intention to use a condom: a ‘prior intention’ measure and a measure of ‘intention in action’. The latter measure refers
 ndents’ perceptions of their intentions immediately prior to intercourse. Bccause intention in action was measured at the same time as condom use, only the prior
n measure is included here.
 es a mixed sex sample. Data were not disaggregated for men and women.
independent samples were studied.
ndents having insertive or receptive anal intercourse are not independent. In order to compute the overall intention-condom use effect size, the average-weighted
ion for the two measures was employed and the largest N in the analysis (cf. Gerrard, Gibbons & Bushman, 1996).
 dents with ‘casual’and ‘steady’ partners are not independent. In order to compute the overall intention-condom use effect size, the average weighted correlation
two measures was employed and the largest N in the analysis (cf. Gerrard et uJ., 1996).
measures of condom use were employed.
  easures of intention were employed.
e’ partners refer to respondents who only had private partners. Samples for private and prostitution partners are therefore independent.
Intentions and condom use                                      241

Meta-anahtic strateg))
The effect size estimate employed here was a weighted average of the sample correlations, r+. r+
describes the direction and strength of the relationship between two variables with a range of - 1.0 to
+ 1.O. Computing the weighted average effect size requires a transformation of the correlation from
each relevant hypothesis into Fisher’s Z. The following formula is then employed :

                                    Average Z value = J(N, x r z o
                                                            JN,
where rzt = the Fisher’s Z transformation of the correlation from each study i, N , = number of persons
in study i.
   In this way correlations based on larger samples receive greater weight than those from smaller
samples. The average Z value is then backtransformed to give r+ (see Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hunter,
Schmidt & Jackson, 1982).
   Homogeneity analyses were conducted using the chi-square statistic (Hunter et a/., 1982) to determine
whether variation among the correlations was greater than chance. The degrees of freedom for the chi-
square test is A- 1, where k is the number of independent correlations. If chi-square is non-significant,
then the correlations are homogeneous and the average weighted effect size, r+s can be said to represent
the population effect size.
   Transformations of other statistics (e.g. t , contingency tables) to statistic r and computation of
weighted average effect size and homogeneity statistics were conducted using Schwarzer’s (1988) Meta
computer program.

Multiple samples and multiple measures. Where studies included more than one sample and reported
separate statistical tests for each sample, then the correlation from each sample was used as the unit of
analysis. Where studies included more than one measure of condom use (e.g. condom use with ‘casual’
versus ‘steady’ partners) (Morrison, 1993; Morrison et a/., 1995), then the weighted average correlation
was computed within each independent sample of that study. The largest N in that sample was then
employed in computing the overall effect size (cf. Gerrard, Gibbons & Bushman, 1996). These
procedures retain the richness of the data without violating the independence assumption which
underlies the validity of meta-analytic procedures.

                                               Results
The overall intention-condom use relationship
The sample size-weighted average correlation between intention and condom use
was r+ = .44 (95 per cent confidence interval = .41-.47, A = 28, N = 2532). In order
to ensure that this statistic was not biased by the preponderance of published studies,
the effect sizes for published versus unpublished studies were compared. The average
correlations for published studies (r+ = .44,k = 25, N = 2259) and unpublished
studies (r+ = .44, A = 3, N = 273) were identical. To determine the robustness of the
average correlation obtained here, we estimated the number of unpublished studies
containing null results which would be required to invalidate this study’s conclusion
that intention and condom use are significantly related ( p < .05). The ‘Fail-safe N’
(Rosenthal, 1984) was 217. Since there are unlikely to be so many unpublished studies
with null results which we were unable to locate, the r+ obtained can confidently be
viewed as significantly different from zero. While the average correlation is robust,
the homogeneity statistic shows considerable variation in the correlations reported
in previous studies (x2(21) = 161.65, p < .OOl). This heterogeneity encourages a
search for moderators.
242                          Paschal Sheeran and Sheina Orbell

Effects of moderator variables on the intention-condom use relationship
The first moderator variable we had hoped to examine was sexual orientation.
Unfortunately, since there were only two hypotheses involving gay men with a
combined sample size of N = 273, meaningful comparison of the effect sizes from
gay versus heterosexual samples was not possible. This view is supported by findings
showing that the Fail-safe N for the average correlation between intentions and
behaviour for gay men was 8. This value is considerably less than Rosenthal’s (1984)
                                                         +
guidelines for regarding a correlation as ‘robust’ (5k 10, or 20 studies in the present
case). More studies of gay men are required in order to determine whether sexual
orientation moderates the intention-condom use relationship.
   We adopted two strategies to examine the effects of other moderators (Hunter &
Schmidt, 1990). First, correlations between r+ and each of the moderator variables,
gender, sample age, time interval and intention versus expectation were computed
(see Table 2). Second, we treated each moderator as a categorical variable. We
computed the effect size for each level of the moderator and used Fisher’s Z test for
the comparison of independent correlations to test the significance of the difference
between effect sizes. Table 3 presents the separate effect sizes obtained for men and
women, adolescents and older samples, shorter and longer time intervals, and
behavioural intention and behavioural expectation (analyses for condom use with a
‘steady’ versus a ‘casual’ partner were more complex and are described later).

Gender. We hypothesized that there would be a stronger correlation between
intention and condom use for (heterosexual) men than for (heterosexual) women.
However, the average effect sizes for men (r+ = .45) and women (r+ = .44)did not
differ significantly (Z = 0.22, n.s.) and gender and effect size were not associated
( r = .02, n.s.). Thus, men and women do not appear to differ in their capacity to
implement their intentions to use condoms.

Sample age. There was a significant correlation between sample age and the strength
of the intention-condom use relationship ( r = - .69, p < .OOl). Consistent with our
hypothesis, the effect size for adolescents (r+= .25) was significantly smaller than the
effect size for older samples (r+ = .50, Z = 6.48, p < .OOl). Adolescents were less
able to implement their intentions to use condoms than undergraduate and adult
samples.

Time interval. The correlation between the logarithmic transformation of time
interval and strength of the intention-condom use relationship was also significant
(r = - .59, p < .OOl). Longer delays between the assessment of intention and the
assessment of condom use were associated with attenuation of the intention-
behaviour correlation. Dividing time interval at the sample median (Mdn = 10 weeks),
the average correlation for ‘short’ intervals (r+ = .59) was significantly bigger than
the correlation for ‘long’ intervals (r+ = .33; Z = 8.28, p < .OOl).
    It should be noted that sample age and time interval were negatively correlated
( r = - .58, p < .OOl), indicating that studies of adolescent samples have generally
employed longer time intervals while studies of undergraduates and adults have
Intentions and condom use                                    243

Table 2. Correlations between intention-condom use effect size and moderator
variables

                                         1          2               3          4             5

1.   Gender'                            1.oo      - .08        - .06         - .08          .02
2.   Sample ageb                                   1.oo        - .58*           .13         .69*
3.   T i m e interval'                                          1   .oo         .02       - .59*
4.   BI vs. BEd                                                                1.oo         .05
5.   Intention-condom use r+                                                               1.oo
* p < .001.
  Gender was coded men = 0, women = 1. Analyses for gender d o not include data for gay men. For
correlations involving gender, N = 1310. N = 2532 for all other correlations.
* Sample age was coded adolescents = 0, other = 1.
  Time interval was computed as a logarithmic transformation (base 10 log) of the delay in assessment
between intention and condom use (in weeks). As Cohen & Cohen (1983) and Randall & Wolff (1994)
point out, a logarithmic transformation of time is more Likely than an untransformed variable to be
linearly related to the dependent variable.
  Behavioural expectation (BE) was coded = 0, behavioural intention (BI) was coded = 1.

Table 3. Intention-condom use effect sizes obtained for each moderator variable

              Moderator            k'            Nb           r+c         95% CId Chi-square'

     Women                           9           825          .44         .38-.50       41.76*
     Men                             8           485          .45         .37-.52       11.55
     Adolescents                     9           661          .25         .17-.32       15.41
     O l d e r samples              19          1871          .50         .46-.53      119.23*
     ' Short' time interval         14          1040          .59         .54-.62       91.77*
     'Long' time interval           14          1492          .33         .2&.37        42.92*
     Intention                      18          1700          .44         .40-.48      111.12*
     Expectation                    10           832          .43         .37-.48       50.68*

* p < .001.
 Number of correlations.
 Sample size upon which sample-weighted average correlation is based.
 Sample-weighted average correlation between intentions and condom use.
 95 % confidence interval.
 Chi-square test for homogeneity of sample correlations.

employed shorter intervals. To ensure that the effects of time interval were
independent of the effects of sample age, we compared the correlations between time
interval and r+ within the adolescent and older samples. There remained a significant
difference between longer and shorter time intervals for adolescents (r+ = .16 and
.36, respectively, Z = 2.72, p < .Ol) and for older samples (r+ = .37 and .60
respectively, Z = 6.57, p < .OOl).
   In order to ensure that the effects of sample age were independent of the effects of
time interval, the effect sizes for sample age were compared within each level of time
interval. There was a significant difference between the effect sizes for adolescents
244                         Paschal Sbeeran and Sbeina Orbell
(r+ = .25) and older samples (r+ = .37) for time intervals greater than 10 weeks (Z =
2.51, p < .Ol). There was also a significant difference between adolescents and older
samples for shorter time intervals (r+ = .18 and .60, respectively, Z = 3 . 8 4 , ~< .Ol).
Longer time intervals and younger age, therefore, both attenuate the strength of the
correlation between behavioural intention and condom use.
Bebavioural intention versus behavioural expectation. The correlation between intention
versus expectation and r+ was not significant ( r = .05, n.s.) and when the effect sizes
for intention (r+ = .44)and expectation (r+ = .43) were compared, the difference was
not significant (Z = 0.29, n.s.). Measures of behavioural intention versus behavioural
expectation appeared to have similar average correlations with condom use.
‘ Casual’ versus ‘steah’ partner. Galligan & Terry (1993), Morrison (1993) and
Morrison et al. (1995) present intention-condom use correlations separately for
‘casual’ and ‘steady’ partners (see Table 1). We therefore computed separate effect
sizes for the two types of partner. Consistent with our hypothesis, the correlation
between intention and condom use with ‘steady’ partners seemed to be stronger
(r+ = .45, k = 5, N = 535, 95 % CI = .38-.51,      x2  = 9.42, n.s.) than the correlation
with ‘casual’ partners (r+ = .27, k = 5, N = 192,95% CI = .13-.40, xa = 0.91, n.s.).
We cannot statistically compare these effect sizes because these ‘casual ’ and ‘steady ’
samples are not independent and because the Ns differ for the two correlations.
   In a second analysis we combined the effect sizes for ‘steady’ partners from
Morrison (1993), Morrison et al. (1995) and Galligan & Terry (1993) and compared
the result with the combined effect sizes for ‘casual’/‘new’ partners from Abraham
et al. (1996), de Wit et al. (1993) and Reinecke e t al. (1996). The average correlation
between intention and condom use with ‘steady’ partners (r+ = .45) was significantly
bigger than the average correlation for ‘casual’ partners (r+ = .21, k = 4, N = 538,
95% CI = .13-.29,    x2  = 0.99, n.s., Z = 4.44,p < .OOl).

                                      Discussion
A sample size-weighted average correlation coefficient of .44was obtained between
intentions and condom use. Because condom use requires the cooperation of a sexual
partner, we had anticipated that the intention-behaviour effect size here would be
lower than that obtained in previous reviews. Contrary to expectations, r+ = .44is
very similar to the average correlations obtained by Randall & Wolff (1994) (r+ =
.45) and Sheppard e t al. (1988) (r+ = .53) in their meta-analyses of the theory of
reasoned action. Condom use is not, it seems, less predictable from intentions than
are other behaviours.
   Literature searches revealed just two longitudinal studies of intentions and
condom use among gay men. This small sample precluded meaningful comparison
of effect sizes for gay versus heterosexual samples. It is, perhaps, worrying that
despite the large number of longitudinal studies of gay men (see Flowers e t al., 1997,
for review), the most proximate predictor of condom use-intentions              to use
one-has rarely been measured. Clearly, this is a serious omission in studies to date,
which needs to be rectified in future research.
   Our expectation was that gender would influence the intention-condom use
Intentions and condom use                              245
relationship. Previous research showed that men have greater power in heterosexual
relationships (Holland e t al., 1990a, b), which suggested that men might be better
able to implement their intentions to use condoms than women. We found that the
strength of the intention-condom use correlation did not differ for men and women,
however. Future research will need to examine whether variables characterized by
substantive gender differences in previous research such as sex roles, experiences of
sexual coercion, or condom use self-efficacy might directly influence the enactment
of intentions to use condoms.
     We hypothesized that the intention-condom use relationship would be weaker
among adolescent samples compared to older samples based on research suggesting
that intentions to use condoms are very unstable among adolescents (Reinecke e t al.,
1996; Stanton e t al., 1996). This hypothesis was supported both by the overall
analyses and by analyses which controlled for the confounding effect for time
interval. The effect of sample age upon the intention-behaviour correlation does not
appear to have been examined in previous research. However, despite the significant
effect obtained here, it remains unclear why age moderated the relationship between
intentions and condom use. We have suggested that intention stability (Bagozzi &
Yi, 1989; Doll & Ajzen, 1992) or lack of direct experience with the behaviour
(Kashima e t al., 1993) might be responsible. Future research should directly address
these hypotheses taking account of the need to increase adolescents’ capacity to enact
their intentions to practise safer sex.
     A previous meta-analysis of the effects of time interval on the intention-behaviour
correlation (Randall & Wolf€, 1994) concluded that the strength of the intention-
behaviour relationship does not diminish as the delay between the assessment of
intention and behaviour increases. We argued that there were insufficient data in
Randall & Wolff s (1994) meta-analysis to appropriately address this hypothesis and
that a stronger test would be afforded by determining the effects of time interval in
the context of a single behaviour. Time interval had a significant negative
 relationship with the intention-condom use correlation in the present study, and this
effect remained significant even when sample age was controlled.
     This finding supports the position repeatedly stressed by Fishbein and Ajzen (e.g.
 Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Ajzen & Madden, 1986) that the measure of
 intention should be as close as possible to the performance of the behaviour. This is
 not to suggest that intentions are necessarily very poor predictors of behaviour over
 longer time periods. Cohen (1992) suggests that a weighted average correlation of .10
 should be characterized as ‘small’, a value of .30 as ‘medium’, and a value of .50 as
 ‘large ’. Our findings therefore indicate that the average correlation between
 intention and behaviour over longer time intervals is ‘medium’ rather than small
 (r+ = .32), while the average correlation over shorter time intervals is ‘large’ (r+ = .56).
     We also examined whether a ‘measureof behavioural intention versus behavioural
 expectation influenced the intention-condom use correlation. Contrary to Sheppard
 e t al.’s (1988) meta-analysis, but consistent with Randall & Wolff s (1994) findings,
 we found no difference between the average correlations between expectations and
 condom use versus intentions and condom use. Our data indicate that the type of
 measure of behavioural intention does not influence the predictive validity of that
 measure.
246                        Paschal Sheeran and Sheina Orbell
   The final moderator of intention-condom use consistency examined here was
condom use with ‘casual’l‘new ’ partners versus condom use with ‘steady’ partners.
We hypothesized that the intention-condom use correlation would be stronger for
‘steady’ sexual partners than for ‘casual’ partners because the beliefs and attitudes of
these partners are better known to actors and communication about contraceptive
behaviour is more likely (Morrison et al., 1995). Although relatively few studies
specified the type of partner with whom a condom was used, our predictions were
supported. Condom use with a ‘steady’ partner was better predicted by intention
than was condom use with a ‘casual’ partner. This finding underlines the need to
specify type of sexual partner in future psychosocial studies of condom use (Sheeran
& Abraham, 1994). Specifying the type of partner with whom a condom is used
would contribute to research in this area by enabling researchers to identify the
unique determinants of condom use in different types of relationship. This would
enable more careful targeting of psychological variables in AIDS education
campaigns.
   Possible criticisms of our meta-analysis should be addressed. The present research
is based upon a relatively small number of hypotheses (k = 28) compared to previous
reviews (Randall & WoH, 1994; Sheppard et aL, 1988). Mullen (1984) points out that
the validity of meta-analysis does not depend upon the number of studies included
in a review, but depends upon the extent to which the studies which have been
included are representative of the population of studies on that topic. Since the
present study involved an exhaustive literature search (including unpublished
research), we believe that the findings obtained here are valid. Moreover, since the
present study focused upon a single behaviour, inferences about the effects of
moderator variables can be made with confidence.
   Our meta-analysis also has the difficulty that the measurement of condom use relies
upon self-reports. Randall & Wolff (1994) have shown that there are stronger
intention-behaviour correlations when self-report measures of behaviour are
employed compared to more objective behaviour measures. This is a difficulty for
research on sexual behaviour and for other behaviours which are sensitive, private
or illegal. Catania, Gibson, Chitwood & Coates (1990) point out that there is no
‘gold standard’ for the measurement of condom use and that the employment of self-
reports of behaviour is unavoidable. This does not represent a serious problem for
our research, however, because test-retest reliability analyses and validation of self-
reports against reports of sexual partners indicate that self-report measures of
condom use do have satisfactory reliability and validity (Blake, Sharp & Temoshok,
1992; Catania et al., 1990; Sheeran & Abraham, 1994).
   In conclusion, this review finds that there is a medium to strong correlation
between intentions to use condoms and condom use. The weighted average
correlation obtained here does not differ substantively from the correlations found in
previous meta-analyses of intention-behaviour relationships. While gender and
measures of intention versus expectation did not moderate the intention-condom use
relationship, shorter time intervals, older samples and condom use with a ‘steady’
rather than a ‘casual’ sexual partner were each associated with stronger correlations
between intention and behaviour. Future research will need to examine variables
such as intention stability and condom communication which may mediate the effects
Intentions and condom use                                    247
of time interval, age and types of sexual partner on the intention-condom use
relationship.
                                      Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Jennifer Boldero, Diane Morrison, Jost Reinecke, Barbara J. Rye,
Catherine A. Sanderson and Katy White for their cooperation in providing additional data. We are
particularly grateful to Jennifer Boldero, Diane Morrison and Barbara J. Rye for providing unpublished
data. We would also like to thank Christine Galloway, Olivia Rickerby and Janette Watson of the
Department of Information Studies, University of Sheffield for conducting the computerized literature
search. We thank Penny Ditchburn for production of the tables.

                                            References
* Studies included in meta-analysis are preceded by an asterisk.
Abraham, S. C. S. & Sheeran, P. (1993). In search of a psychology of safer sex promotion: Beyond
  beliefs and texts. Health Education Research, 8, 245-254.
Abraham, S. C. S. & Sheeran, P. (1994). Modelling and modifying young heterosexuals’ HIV-
  preventive behaviour : Theories, findings and implications. Patient Education and Counseling, 23,
  173-186.
* Abraham, S. C. S., Sheeran, P., Abrams, D. & Spears, R. (1996). Psychosocial determinants of
     condom use consistency among sexually active Scottish teenagers. Pychology and Health, 11,
     641455.
Ajzen, I. (1985). A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds), Action Control. New
  York: Springer-Verlag.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
  50, 1-33.
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, N J:
  Prentice-Hall.
Ajzen, I. & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions and
  perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453474.
Bagozzi, R. P. & Yi, Y. (1989). Degree of intention formation as a moderator of the attitudebehavior
  relationship. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52, 266-279.
Bajos, N., Wadsworth, J., Ducot, B., Johnson, A. M., Le Pont, F., Wellings, K., Spira, A., Field, J.
  & the ACSF Group (1995). Sexual behaviour and HIV epidemiology: Comparative analysis in France
  and Britain. A I D S , 9, 735-743.
Bandura, A. (1992). A social cognitive approach to the exercise of control over AIDS infection. In
  R. J. DiClemente (Ed.), Adolescents and A I D S : A generation in Jeopar4. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Basen-Engquist, K. (1992). Psychosocial predictors of ‘safer sex’ behaviors in young adults. A I D S
  Education and Prevention, 4, 12G134.
Basen-Engquist, K. & Parcel, G. S. (1992). Attitudes, norms and self-efficacy: A model of adolescents’
  HIV-related sexual risk behaviour. Health Education Quarterly, 19,263-277.
Biglan, A., Noell, J., Ochs, L., Smolkowski, K. & Metzler, C. (1995). Does sexual coercion play a role
  in the high-risk sexual behavior of adolescent and young adult women? Journul of Behavioral Medicine,
  18, 549-568.
Blake, S. M., Sharp, E. S. & Temoshok, L. (1992). Methodological considerations in developing
  measures of HIV risk-relevant behaviors and attitudes : An empirical illustration. Psychology and
  Health, 6, 265-280.
* Boldero, J.. Moore, S. & Rosenthal, D. (1992). Intention, context and safe sex: Australian
     adolescents’ responses to AIDS. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1374-1 398.
* Breakwell, G. M., Millward, L. J. & Fife-Shaw, C. (1994). Commitment to ‘safer’ sex as a predictor
     of condom use amongst 16-20 year olds. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 189-217.
Brown, L. K., DiClemente, R. J. & Park, T. (1992). Predictors of condom use in sexually active
  adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 13,651-657.
* Bryan, A. D., Aiken, L. S. & West, S. G. (1996). Increasing condom use: Evaluation of a theory-
248                              Paschal Sheeran and Sbeina Orbell
      based intervention to prevent sexually transmitted diseases in young women. Health Psychology, 15,
      371-382.
Catania, J. A., Gibson, D. R., Chitwood, D. D. & Coates, T. J. (1990). Methodological problems in
   AIDS behavioral research : Influences on measurement error and participation bias in studies of
   sexual behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 339-362.
Catania, J. A., Stone, V., Binson, D. & Dolcini, D. D. (1995). Changes in condom use among
   heterosexuals in Wave 3 of the AMEN survey. Journal of Sex Research, 32, 193-200.
Cochran, S. D., Mays, V. M., Ciarletta, J., Caruso, C. & Mallon, D. (1992). Efficacy of the theory of
   reasoned action in predicting AIDS-related risk reduction among gay men. Journal of Applied Social
   Psychology, 22, 1481-1501.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied Multa$le RegressionlCorrelation Anabsis for the Behavioral Sciences.
   Hillsdale, N J : Erlbaum.
Cote, J. A., McCullough, J. & Reilly, M. (1985). Effects of unexpected situations on behavior-intention
  differences: A garbology analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 18tF194.
DeVroome, E. M. M., Paalman, M. E. M., Dinglestad, A. A. M., Kolker, L. & Sandfort, T. G. M.
  (1994). Increase in safe sex among the young and non-monogamous: Knowledge, attitudes and
  behavior regarding safe sex and condom use in The Netherlands from 1987 to 1993. Patient Edwation
  and Counseling, 24, 279-288.
* de Wit, J. B. F., van Griensven, G. J. P., Kok, G. & Sandfort, T. G. M. (1993). Why do homosexual
     men relapse to safe sex? Predictors of resumption of unprotected anogenital intercourse with casual
     partners. A I D S , 7 , 1113-1 118.
DiClemente, R. J. (1992). Psychosocial predictors of condom use among adolescents. In
  R. J. DiClemente (Ed.), Adolescents and A I D S : A Generation in Jeoparh. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Doll, J. & Ajzen, I. (1992). Accessibility and stability of predictors in the theory of planned behavior.
  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 754-765.
Dunne, M. P., Donald, M., Lucke, J., Nilsson, R., Ballard, R. & Raphael, B. (1994). Age-related
  increase in sexual behaviours and decrease in condom use among adolescents in Australia. International
  Journal of S T D and A I D S , 5, 41-47.
* Fisher, W. A. (1984). Predicting contraceptive behavior among university men: The role of emotions
     and behavioral intentions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 104-123.
* Fisher, W. A., Fisher, J. D. & Rye, B. (1995). Understanding and promoting AIDS preventive
     behavior: Insights form the theory of reasoned action. Health Psychology, 14, 255-264.
Flowers, P., Sheeran, P., Smith, J. A. & Beail, N. (1997). The role of psychosocial factors in HIV risk-
  reduction among gay and bisexual men: A quantitative review. Psychology and Health, 12, 197-230.
Gagnon, J. & Simon, W. (1974). Sexual Conduct: The Social Sources of Human Sexualig. London:
  Hutchinson.
* Galligan, R. F. & Terry, D. (1993). Romantic ideals, fear of negative implications and the practice
     of safe sex. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1685-1711.
* Gallois, C., Kashima, Y., Terry, D., McCamish, M., Timmins, P. & Chauvin, A. (1992). Safe and
     unsafe intentions and behavior: The effects of norms and attitudes. Journal of Applied Social
     psycho lo^, 22, 1521-1545.
* Gallois, C., Terry, D., Timmins, P., Kashima, Y. & McCamish, M. (1994). Safe sexual intentions and
     behavior among heterosexuals and homosexual men : Testing the theory of reasoned action.
     Psychology and Health, 10, 1-16.
Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X. & Bushman, B. J. (1996). The relation between perceived vulnerability to
  HIV and precautionary behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 390-409.
Hedges, L. V. & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.
Hernandez, J. T. & DiClemente, R. J. (1992). Self-control and ego identity development as predictors
  of unprotected sex among late adolescent males. Journal of Adolescence, 15, 437-477.
Holland, J., Ramazaglou, G. & Scott, S. (1990~).Managing risk and experiencing danger: Tensions
  between government AIDS health education policy and young women’s sexuality. Gender and
  Education, 2, 125-146.
Holland, J., Ramazaglou, C., Scott, S., Sharp, S. & Thomson, R. (1990b). Sex, gender and power:
  Young women’s sexuality in the shadow of AIDS. Sociology of Health and Illness, 12, 336350.
Intentions and condom use                                      249
Hospers, H. J . & Kok, G. (1995). Determinants of safe and risk-taking sexual behavior among gay
   men: A review. A I D S Education and Prevention, 7 , 74-94.
Hunter, J. E. & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of Meta-analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research
   Findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Hunter, J. E., Schmidt, F. L. & Jackson, G. B. (1982). Meta-analysis: Cumulating Research Findings Across
   Studies. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Jemmott, L. S . & Jemmott, J. B. (1991). Applying the theory of reasoned action to AIDS risk
   behaviour : Condom use among black women. Nursing Research, 40,228-234.
Kasen, S., Vaughan, R. D. & Walter, H. J. (1992). Self-efficacy for AIDS-preventive behaviors among
   tenth grade students. Health Education Quarterly, 19, 187-202.
Kashima, Y., Gallois, C. & McCamish, M. (1993). The theory of reasoned action and cooperative
   behaviour: It takes two to use a condom. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 227-239.
Liska, A. E. (1984). A critical examination of the causal structure of the Fishbein-Ajzen model. Social
   Psychology Quarterb, 47, 61-74.
Macey, L. P. & Boldero, J . M. (1992). The prediction of condom use by adult males and females.
   Annual Meeting of Australian Social Psychologists, Auckland, New Zealand, April.
* Morrison, D. M. (1993). Condom use with steady and casual partners: Results from a longitudinal
      study of high-risk adolescents and adults. N I C H D Conference on Behavioral Research on the Role of
      Condoms in Reproductive Health, Rochille, M D .
* Morrison, D. M., Rogers Gillmore, M. & Baker, S. A. (1995). Determinants of condom use among
      high-risk adults: A test of the theory of reasoned action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25,
      651-676.
Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC Meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum.
Nicolson, P. (1993). Deconstructing sexology : Understanding the pathologisation of female sexuality.
   ]ournal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 11, 191-201.
Randall, D. M. & Wolff, J. A. (1994). The time interval in the intention-behaviour relationship: Meta-
   analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 405-418.
* Reinecke, J., Schmidt, P. & Ajzen, I. (1996). Application of the theory of planned behavior to
      adolescents’ condom use: A panel study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 749-772.
Reiss, I. L. & Leik, R. K. (1989). Evaluating strategies to avoid AIDS: Numbers of partners vs. use
   of condoms. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 181-196.
Robertson, B. (1985). Sexual behaviour and risk of exposure to HIV among 18-25-year-olds in
    Scotland: Assessing change 1988-1993. A I D S , 9, 285-292.
Rose, S. & Frieze, I. H. (1989). Young singles’ script for a first date. Gender and Socieg, 3, 258-268.
Rosenthal, R. R. (1984). Meta-analytic Procedures for Social Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
* Rye, B. J . (1995). Unpublished data. Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario,
       London, Ontario, Canada.
 * Sanderson, C. A. & Jemmott, J. B. (1996). Moderation and mediation of HIV-prevention
       interventions : Relationship status, intentions and condom use among college students. Journal of
       Applied Social Psychology, 26, 20762099.
 Schaalma, H., Kok, G. & Peters, L. (1993). Determinants of consistent condom use by adolescents: The
    impact of experience of sexual intercourse. Health Education Research, 8, 255-269.
 Schwarzer, R. (1988). Meta: Programs for Seconday Data Anabsis. Berlin: Free University of Berlin.
 Sheeran, P. & Abraham, S. C. S. (1994). Measurement of condom use in 72 studies of HIV-preventive
    behaviour : A critical review. Patient Education and Counseling, 24, 199-216.
 Stall, R. D., Coates, T. J. & Hoff, C. (1988). Behavioral risk reduction for HIV infection among gay
    and bisexual men. American Psychologist, 43, 878-885.
 Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J. & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-
    analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. Journal of
    Consumer Research, 15, 325-343.
 * Stanton, B. F., Li, X., Black, M. M., Ricardo, I., Galbraith, J., Feigelman, S. & Kaljee, L. (1996).
       Longitudinal stability and predictability of sexual perceptions, intentions, and behaviors among
       early adolescent African-Americans. Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, 1G19.
 Terry, D. J. & O’Leary, J. E. (1995). The theory of planned behaviour: The effects of perceived
    behavioural control and self-efficacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 199-220.
You can also read