Examining the Hierarchical Influences of the Big-Five Dimensions and Anxiety Sensitivity on Anxiety Symptoms in Children

Page created by Calvin Warner
 
CONTINUE READING
Examining the Hierarchical Influences of the Big-Five Dimensions and Anxiety Sensitivity on Anxiety Symptoms in Children
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
                                                                                                                                             published: 04 June 2019
                                                                                                                                      doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01185

                                            Examining the Hierarchical
                                            Influences of the Big-Five
                                            Dimensions and Anxiety Sensitivity
                                            on Anxiety Symptoms in Children
                                            Erika Wauthia 1,2,3* , Laurent Lefebvre 1,3 , Kathy Huet 1,3,4 , Wivine Blekic 1,2,3 ,
                                            Khira El Bouragui 1,3,5 and Mandy Rossignol 1,3
                                            1
                                              Department of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Mons,
                                            Mons, Belgium, 2 National Fund for Human Research (FRESH), National Fund for Scientific Research, Brussels, Belgium,
                                            3
                                              Interdisciplinary Research Center in Psychophysiology and Cognitive Electrophysiology, Mons, Belgium, 4 Laboratory
                                            of Phonetics, Research Institute for Language Sciences and Technology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, University
                                            of Mons, Mons, Belgium, 5 Laboratory C2S, University of Reims Champagne-Ardenne, Reims, France

                                            Anxiety sensitivity (AS), namely the fear of anxiety symptoms, has been described as
                                            a precursor of sub-threshold anxiety levels. Sexton et al. (2003) posited that increased
                                            AS would arise from an elevated neuroticism and that both would act as vulnerability
                                            factors for panic disorder (PD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and generalized
                          Edited by:        anxiety disorder (GAD) symptoms. Accordingly, this study aimed to (1) evaluate the
                       Sarah Whittle,       applicability of this model to a pediatric population and (2) examine the influences of
The University of Melbourne, Australia
                                            the other Big-Five personality dimensions on the four lower-order dimensions of AS
                     Reviewed by:
                       Ayhan Bilgiç,        (cognitive, physical, control, and physical) and on social phobia (SP), separation anxiety
 Necmettin Erbakan University, Turkey       disorder (SAD) and depression symptoms. 200 children (104 girls) aged between 8
                       Nebi Sümer,
           Sabancı University, Turkey
                                            and 12 years old (mean age = 132.52 months, SD = 14.5) completed the Childhood
                  *Correspondence:
                                            Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Silverman et al., 1991), the Big Five Questionnaire for Children
                        Erika Wauthia       (Barbaranelli et al., 2003), and the Revised’s Children Anxiety and Depression Scale
          erika.wauthia@umons.ac.be         (Chorpita et al., 2000). Regression analyses confirmed that AS and neuroticism together
                  Specialty section:
                                            significantly predicted the presence of PD, OCD, and GAD symptoms but also SP,
        This article was submitted to       SAD, and depression symptoms. Moreover, neuroticism interacted with extraversion,
   Personality and Social Psychology,
                                            conscientiousness and agreeableness to significantly predict SP, GAD, and depression.
               a section of the journal
               Frontiers in Psychology      Surprisingly, the global AS score was only predicted by agreeableness, while AS
         Received: 07 January 2019          dimensions also specifically related to openness. Finally, AS dimensions did not predict
           Accepted: 06 May 2019            the presence of specific anxiety symptoms. To conclude, the predicting model of anxiety
          Published: 04 June 2019
                                            symptoms in children sets neuroticism and AS on the same level, with an unexpected
                             Citation:
       Wauthia E, Lefebvre L, Huet K,
                                            influence of agreeableness on AS, raising the importance of other trait-like factors in the
           Blekic W, El Bouragui K and      definition of such models. Moreover, AS should be considered as a unitary construct
        Rossignol M (2019) Examining
                                            when predicting the presence of anxiety symptoms in children. Future interventions must
     the Hierarchical Influences of the
     Big-Five Dimensions and Anxiety        consider these associations to help children detect and recognize the symptoms of their
     Sensitivity on Anxiety Symptoms        anxiety and help them to interpret them correctly.
 in Children. Front. Psychol. 10:1185.
      doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01185         Keywords: Big-Five personality, neuroticism, anxiety sensitivity, anxiety symptoms, children, vulnerability factors

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                                     1                                              June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Examining the Hierarchical Influences of the Big-Five Dimensions and Anxiety Sensitivity on Anxiety Symptoms in Children
Wauthia et al.                                                                                            Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

INTRODUCTION                                                                Physical Concerns (fear of physical symptoms of anxiety; i.e., “it
                                                                            scares me when my heart beats rapidly”); Cognitive Concerns
While a large number of youths report excessive anxiety-related             (fear of loss of cognitive control; i.e., “when I’m nervous, I worry
emotional and behavioral responses (e.g., avoidance) and anxiety-           that I may be mentally ill”) and Social Concerns (fear of publicly
related cognitive patterns (American Psychiatric Association                observable anxiety symptoms; i.e., “it embarrasses me when my
[APA], 2013), only up to 20% of them meet the criteria for                  stomach growls”). Recently, Stassart and Etienne (2014) proposed
an anxiety disorder diagnosis (Costello et al., 2004). Moreover,            to add a fourth lower-order factor relating to Losing Control
subthreshold anxiety levels interfere with children’s general well-         Concerns (i.e., “Other kids can usually tell when I feel shaky”).
being, developmental social skills and social life (Pine et al.,               The CASI has proven to be a reliable and valid questionnaire
1998; Kendall et al., 2001; Kendall and Treadwell, 2007; Mazzone            for measuring AS in both clinical and non-clinical pediatric
et al., 2007; Kessler et al., 2012). Notably, they are associated           populations (Silverman et al., 1991; Rabian et al., 1999). Using
with impaired memory and cognitive performances (Bulbena and                this instrument, studies revealed that AS is typically higher in
Berrios, 1993; Daleiden, 1998) and consequently have precarious             children meeting diagnosis criteria of anxiety disorders and in
impacts on the academic adjustment of children (Fergusson and               highly anxious children as compared to healthy control children
Woodward, 2002; Wittchen and Fehm, 2003; Mazzone et al.,                    (Rabian et al., 1993; Vasey et al., 1995; Joiner et al., 2002;
2007; Beesdo et al., 2009; Thapar et al., 2012). Subthreshold               McLaughlin et al., 2007). However, AS scores do not distinguish
anxiety levels also have a high comorbidity with other mental               between children reporting anxiety disorders and children with
disorders, such as depressive moods, and are associated with                externalizing disorders, probably due to comorbid diagnoses
an increased prevalence of mental disorders in adulthood                    (Rabian et al., 1999).
(Fergusson et al., 2005; Bittner et al., 2007; Copeland et al., 2009;          As already mentioned, it has been shown that AS determines
Wolitzky-Taylor et al., 2014) and with an enhanced suicide risk             the development and severity of a large variety of fears and
(Balázs et al., 2017). Indeed, the presence of sub-threshold panic          anxiety disorders (Taylor et al., 1992) and seems particularly
disorder (PD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and social              prominent in those based on somatic signals, as social anxiety
phobia (SP) predict the full onset of these disorders later on              or PDs. Notably, Taylor et al. (1992) compared the levels of
(Wolitzky-Taylor et al., 2014). Therefore, a major concern is               AS across DSM-III anxiety disorders and revealed that AS was
identifying children that are at high risk of developing anxiety            elevated in all diagnostic groups, in comparison with normal
symptoms and preventing deleterious consequences. This can be               controls, except for simple phobia. Similarly, Kearney et al. (1997)
done by gaining a better understanding of the etiological factors           found higher levels of AS in children and adolescents with
of sub-threshold anxiety levels.                                            clinical levels of PD. Eley et al. (2004) reported similar results
   Recently, increasing attention has been given to anxiety                 in adults, which showed an enhanced ability to perceive internal
sensitivity (AS) that constitutes a stable, trait-like cognitive            physiological cues and a tendency to fear them. Higher levels of
vulnerability factor for anxiety disorders (Olatunji and Wolitzky-          AS were also observed in youths with SP and associated with the
Taylor, 2009). AS – also known as the fear of anxiety –                     fact that they interpret their enhanced physiological arousal as a
refers to the tendency to interpret anxiety symptoms (including             visible cue of anxiety and a potential source of embarrassment in
somatic sensations) as signals of social, psychological or physical         social situations (Eley et al., 2004; Anderson and Hope, 2009).
catastrophe susceptible to cause illness, embarrassment or                     Deacon and Abramowitz (2006) examined the relationships
additional anxiety (Reiss et al., 1986). For example, an individual         between the three dimensions of AS and the type of anxiety
with elevated AS levels might be more likely to interpret a racing          disorder in adults. They reported higher AS scores in anxious
heart as a precursor to a heart attack or will fear to sweat in             patients than in healthy undergraduate students and higher
public because of his concerns about negative social evaluations.           scores in patients with PD than in patients with SP and
If AS is a dispositional variable, it describes a specific tendency         generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Their analyses confirmed
to fearfully respond to one’s own anxiety symptoms (Olatunji                that the physical concerns’ dimension was related to PD
and Wolitzky-Taylor, 2009); which is thus distinguishable from              supporting previous results (Zinbarg et al., 1997, 2001; Hayward
trait anxiety which refers to a tendency to respond with fear to            et al., 2000; Blais et al., 2001) and the idea that fear of physical
a wide range of stressors (Spielberger, 1989). In other words,              sensations can contribute to the panic-related psychopathology.
unlike individuals with high levels of AS, individuals with high            Secondly, they outlined a near-exclusive relationship between
trait anxiety will not respond to anxious symptoms unless they              the social concerns’ dimension and SP. Finally, the cognitive
identify them as a threat (McNally, 1989).                                  concerns dimension can account for elevated AS in patients with
   Anxiety sensitivity has been conceptualized as an higher-order           OCD (Cox et al., 1999; Sexton et al., 2003) although conflicting
factor hierarchically organized by several lower-order factors              results have been highlighted (Deacon and Abramowitz, 2006).
(Cox et al., 1996; Zinbarg et al., 1997; Taylor and Cox, 1998;                 Prospective studies indicated that AS constitutes a predictor of
Muris, 2002). In children and adolescents, AS is measured with              the future occurrence of anxiety symptoms and anxiety disorders
the Childhood Anxiety Sensitivity Index (CASI, Silverman et al.,            (Schmidt et al., 1997, 1999; Hayward et al., 2000; Schmidt
1991) that is an age-downward modification of the Anxiety                   et al., 2006). Conducting longitudinal studies with a follow-up
Sensitivity Index (ASI; Peterson and Reiss, 1992; Zinbarg et al.,           assessment 1 year after the baseline, Weems et al. (2007) and
1999; Taylor et al., 2007) initially developed for adults. This             Schmidt et al. (2010) found that AS predicted the development
inventory investigates three distinct dimensions of AS, namely              of various symptoms of anxiety disorder in early adolescence and

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                           2                                        June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                           Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

could be considered as a cognitive risk factor. The predictive role        Nowadays, while neuroticism and extraversion are the only
of AS was also reported among children and adolescents for PD              significant predictors of AS (Borger et al., 1996), studies
(Lau et al., 1996; Reiss et al., 2001), OCD (Calamari et al., 2001),       investigating its relationships with other personality dimensions
and PTSD symptoms (Kılıç et al., 2008).                                    such as openness – that includes intellectual curiosity, the need
    Cognitive models of anxiety disorders offer a theoretical              for variety and non-dogmatic attitudes–and agreeableness that
framework for the relationship between AS and the subsequent               involves trust, altruism and sympathy, are scarce (Bienvenu and
development of anxiety. Notably, Wells and Leahy (1998)                    Stein, 2003; Bienvenu et al., 2004), especially in children.
proposed that, if anxiety symptoms are initially caused by                    Literature highlighted that these personality dimensions also
biological factors, their persistence are the consequence of a             appear to be associated with anxiety disorders, as demonstrated
misinterpretation of internal/external events. Indeed, events such         in adults (Clark et al., 1994; Brown, 2007; Kotov et al., 2010;
as bodily sensations caused by a postural change in blood                  Naragon-Gainey et al., 2014). Notably, Bienvenu et al. (2001)
pressure, tiredness, excitement or stress are taken as a sign of an        evidenced higher scores of neuroticism in adults who reported
immediate catastrophe such as dying, suffocating, having a heart           a history of SP, agoraphobia or PD. Individuals with a history of
attack/seizure, fainting, collapsing, losing one’s mind or losing          SP and agoraphobia also presented lower scores of extraversion,
control (Wells, 2013). Once misinterpretations developed, a shift          supporting previous results (Solyom et al., 1986; Watson et al.,
in selective attention occurs and anxious individuals become self-         1988; Trull and Sher, 1994; Brown et al., 1998; Naragon-Gainey
focused on negative feelings and symptoms. In order to advert              et al., 2014; Newby et al., 2017). Higher scores of neuroticism
those, engage themselves in safety/coping behaviors that will              have been found in patients with OCDs and post-traumatic stress
maintain and exacerbate symptoms through four mechanisms                   disorders (PTSD) (Trull and Sher, 1994; Samuels et al., 2000),
(Wells and Leahy, 1998). First, anxiety persists because the non-          but PTSD patients also showed lower scores of extraversion,
occurrence of the catastrophe is attributed to the used behavior.          agreeableness and conscientiousness (Trull and Sher, 1994). In
Second, the use of certain safety behaviors will intensify or              children, longitudinal studies also revealed that high levels of
prolong unwanted symptoms (e.g., focusing on heart rate will               neuroticism and lower levels of extraversion were predictors of
consequently increase it). Third, some safety behaviors increase           higher levels of anxiety (Ehrler et al., 1999; Muris et al., 2004;
self-focused attention -as the person focuses attention inward             Vreeke and Muris, 2012). Interestingly, Naragon-Gainey et al.
to monitor and gauge the effectiveness of those behaviors- that            (2014) suggested that the development of social anxiety disorder
amplifies awareness of their symptoms. Fourth, safety behaviors            may result from the interaction between low levels of extraversion
can interfere with the adaptation of the anxious individuals in            and increased AS level.
everyday situations. According to this model, AS would amplify                Therefore, neuroticism seems related to both constructs
preexisting anxiety levels to the extent that individuals with             of AS and anxiety disorders. Brown et al. (1998) defined a
high levels of AS may misinterpret physical sensations as danger           model in which neuroticism would be a higher-order factor
signals and as a result experience elevated levels of anxiety              that has a direct causal influence on various types of anxiety
(Olatunji and Wolitzky-Taylor, 2009).                                      disorders. According to this model, neuroticism would operate
    Anxiety sensitivity’s levels can be modulated by the                   with another factor, positive affectivity (PA) which contributes
dimensions of the Five-Factor Model of Personality (Big                    to the development of depressive affects. Taylor (1998) refined
Five) (McRae and Costa, 1996) and notably by neuroticism.                  this model by postulating the existence of a hierarchical model
Also known as emotional instability or negative affectivity                in which fears are the product of various etiological factors
(Watson and Clark, 1984), neuroticism refers to the tendency to            with different levels of specificity. Some factors are disorder-
experience negative emotions and the poor ability to cope with             common factors (i.e., neuroticism) and others are disorder-
stress. Despite some conceptual differences, reviews suggested             specific factors such as AS. Extending this model, Sexton et al.
that neuroticism and trait anxiety are highly similar constructs           (2003) combined the influence of AS and neuroticism on the
(Zinbarg and Barlow, 1996; Barlow, 2000; Sexton et al., 2003);             development of anxiety disorders. In their model, neuroticism
they arise from genetic influences and from early childhood                acts as a general vulnerability factor that directly prompts the
learning and they comprise cognitive and psychological features            onset of anxious symptoms, namely worry, panic symptoms,
or tendencies to readily perceive threat, and to be readily aroused        health anxiety and OCD symptoms. In parallel, neuroticism
(Craske, 1999; Sexton et al., 2003).                                       mediates the development of anxiety sensibility which in turn
    Studies revealed that elevated levels of AS were positively            intensifies the four categories of anxious symptoms. To sum
correlated to an increased neuroticism (Cox et al., 1999).                 up, neuroticism may constitute a general vulnerability while AS
This is consistent with previous studies using the Big Three               would be a specific etiological variable in the occurrence of
model of personality (Tellegen, 1985) showing that AS was                  specific anxiety complaints. Norton et al. (2005) replicated this
positively correlated to negative emotionality (NE) (Arrindell,            hierarchical model in patients suffering from clinical levels of
1993; Lilienfeld, 1997). AS appears to be negatively associated            anxiety disorders and obtained highly consistent results.
to extraversion, which refers to one’s quantity and intensity                 Accordingly, the first aim of this study was to test the
of interpersonal interactions and positive emotions, and with              applicability of Sexton et al.’s (2003) hierarchical influences of
conscientiousness that is characterized by a disciplined striving          neuroticism and AS on anxiety symptoms in children between
after goals and a structured adherence to principles (Costa and            8 and 12 years of age. We hypothesized that high levels of
McCrae, 1990; Borger et al., 1996; Brandes and Bienvenu, 2006).            neuroticism would directly predict increased AS levels and

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                          3                                        June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                          Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

increased PD, OCD, and GAD symptoms and that high AS levels              guardian gave written informed consent in accordance with the
would also be directly associated to these symptoms.                     Declaration of Helsinki.
    The second aim of this study is the examination of a second,
more elaborated, model derived from the first one. As already            Measures
done by Norton et al. (2005) and Norton and Price (2007), we             Childhood Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Silverman et al., 1991;
tested paths for SP, SAD, and depressive symptoms which are              Stassart et al., 2013) - The CASI is a self-report inventory
highly prominent symptoms in children (Costello et al., 2005;            including 18 items scored on a three-point Likert scale ranging
Beesdo et al., 2009). Furthermore, we added the influence of other       from 1 “not at all” to 3 “very much.” On each item, children have
Big-Five personality traits since some associations had already          to rate the extent to which the experience of anxiety will have
been shown with extraversion in adults (Bienvenu and Stein,              negative consequences. Example items are “It scares me when my
2003; Bienvenu et al., 2004) while other personality dimensions          heart beats fast,” “It scares me when I feel nervous,” and “It scares
such as conscientiousness, openness and agreeableness had                me when I feel shaky.” Test-retest reliability has been reported
not been investigated. We expected that extraversion as the              at 0.79 in clinical and at 0.76 for non-clinical samples (Bilgiç
positive corollary of neuroticism would have direct and indirect         et al., 2013). The French version of CASI has a satisfactory validity
significant effects on all anxiety symptoms and on AS levels. Due        and good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87
to the fact that OCD may be conceptualized as a maladaptive              (Vanasse et al., 2010; Stassart and Etienne, 2014). The CASI allows
version of conscientiousness and that some conscientiousness-            the calculation of four lower-order dimensions: cognitive, social,
related traits converged with OCD in adults (Samuels et al.,             physical and fear of losing control (Stassart and Etienne, 2014).
2000), we also postulated that conscientiousness would have              Cronbach’s alphas of the separate dimensions of the AS ranged
direct effects on OCD symptoms in school-aged children.                  from 0.33 to 0.82 (Stassart and Etienne, 2014).
    The third objective of this study was to examine this extended           Big Five Questionnaire for Children (Barbaranelli et al., 2003;
hierarchical model, decomposing AS into its four lower-order             Rossier et al., 2007 for the French version) – The Big Five
factors and to investigate associations between these factors,           Questionnaire for Children (BFQC) is a self-reported measure
personality dimensions and anxiety symptoms. Since PD is                 consisting of 65-items used for assessing the basic personality
defined by the occurrence of panic attacks whose physical                dimensions of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness,
manifestations are significant (e.g., palpitations, sweating, and        emotional instability or neuroticism and openness in youths.
shaking) (Craske et al., 2010), we hypothesized that PD                  Each of the five dimensions is evaluated by 13 items. These items
symptoms would be related to higher scores on the physical               are rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “Almost
concerns dimension. Given that SP is defined as the excessive            never true” to 5 “Almost always true.”
and persistent fear of (one or more) social or performance                   Revised Children Anxiety and Depression Scale (Chorpita et al.,
situations involving exposure to others (Furmark, 2002; Schneier,        2000; Bouvard et al., 2015 for the French version) – The Revised
2006), we expect that SP symptoms would be associated to                 Children Anxiety and Depression Scale (RCADS) is a 47-items
higher scores on the social concerns dimension. The prominent            self-reported questionnaire used to assess DSM-IV anxiety and
presence of worries in GAD (Chorpita et al., 1997; Weems                 depression symptoms in children and adolescents from 7 to
et al., 2000) allowed us to posit that GAD symptoms would                18 years old. Children have to rate how often each item applies
be associated with cognitive concerns and (4) the rigidity of            to them; each item is scored on a three-point Likert scale from 0
thoughts characterizing OCD symptoms led us to hypothesize               “never” to 3 “always.” Separated scores are obtained for SAD, SP,
that OCD symptoms would be associated with cognitive and                 GAD, PD, OCD, and depression.
losing control concerns.
                                                                         Data Analyses
                                                                         Data Screening and Outlier Analysis
MATERIALS AND METHODS                                                    All measures were initially assessed for multivariate outliers and
                                                                         univariates outliers. First, the data were assessed for multivariate
Participants                                                             outliers by entering all measures into a multiple regression
Ten primary schools in the Mons region (Belgium) were                    analysis and computing Mahalanobis distance, as suggested in the
contacted and agreed to participate in this study. Inventories           paper of Sexton et al. (2003). A chi-square cut-off of p < 0.001
were distributed to parents who had to sign an agreement before          was used as the criteria for multivariate outliers (Tabachnick
their child’s participation. 250 parents responded positively            and Fidell, 1996). Four multivariate outliers were identified
and 200 Caucasian children (104 girls) aged from 8 to 12                 and removed from the data set, yielding a working n of 196.
(mean age = 132.55 months; SD = 14.5) completed all the                  Univariate outliers were then identified by taking the inter-
inventories. All children were free from learning disorders,             quartile range 1.5 times and declaring all data points as outliers
neurologic or psychiatric conditions as assessed with the Child          that are either this distance above the upper quartile or this
Behavior Checklist (Achenbach and Edelbrock, 1983; Fombonne              distance below the lower quartile (Hoaglin and Iglewicz, 1987).
et al., 1988). The study was carried out in accordance with the          Sixty-one data points were identified as univariate outliers that
recommendations of the Ethic Board of the Faculty of Psychology          were consequently Windsorized, replacing the outlying data with
and Education of the University of Mons. The protocol was                non-outlying values (Hoaglin and Iglewicz, 1987). We conducted
approved by the committee. All participants and their legal              the Harman’s one-factor test (Harman, 1970; Podsakoff et al.,

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                        4                                         June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                                        Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

2003) to detect the presence of common method variance. The                             neuroticism and high levels of AS had significant direct effects
result of the unrotated factor analysis indicated one single factor                     on the presence of PD symptoms [F(2,169) = 25.20; p =< 0.001;
that accounted for 13.87% of the variance, that allowed to                              R2 = 0.297; β = 0.260]. Results also showed that neuroticism
conclude that the common method only have a limited effect on                           and AS had significant direct effects on OCD symptoms
the relationships between measures of different constructs.                             [F(2,169) = 18.58; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.182; β = 0.187]. Finally,
                                                                                        we also found that neuroticism (β = 0.491) and AS (β = 0.343)
Sample Characteristics                                                                  had a significant effect on GAD symptoms [F(2,169) = 25.30;
A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was computed                               p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.233]. First hypothesized model is summarized
to explore potential differences between sex groups on the                              in Supplementary Table 2.
dependent variables used in this study. The MANOVA revealed a                              We also used backward regression-based path analyses to test
significant multivariate sex difference for the openness dimension                      the second hypothesized hierarchical model. Figure 2 presents
of the BFQ-C [F(1,127) = 5.79; p = 0.018]. Means obtained                               this model with standardized path coefficients. Examination
by our sample to the five dimensions of the BFQ-C were                                  of the effects of the five personality dimensions on AS levels
compared to those obtained by Olivier and Herve (2015) in their                         revealed that only agreeableness had a significant effect on AS
validation study. Results showed that our data were similar to                          [F(5,128) = 3.51; p =< 0.001. R2 = 0.125; β = −0.305]. Then, we
those obtained by a group of French-speaking children of the                            evaluated the effects of personality dimensions and AS on anxiety
same age group. To assess hierarchical relationships between                            symptoms. First, we found that high levels of conscientiousness
variables, we conducted regression-based path analyses using                            (β = 0.207), neuroticism (β = 0.472) and AS (β = 0.310) had
multiple backward regression analyses on full-saturated models.                         significant direct effects on the presence of SP symptoms [F(6,
All statistical analyses were computed with SPSS 21. The alpha                          128) = 11,64; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.364]. Second, results showed
level of significance was set at 0.05 throughout analyses.                              that high levels of neuroticism (β = 0.285) and AS (β = 0.336)
                                                                                        had direct significant effect on the presence of PD symptoms
                                                                                        [F(6,128) = 6.38; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.239]. Third, analyses revealed
RESULTS                                                                                 that low levels of extraversion (β = −0.235) and high levels
                                                                                        of neuroticism (β = 0.355) and AS (β = 0.305) had significant
Descriptive Statistics                                                                  direct effects on GAD symptoms [F(6,128) = 7.80; p =< 0.001;
Mean, standard deviations of the main variables are shown in                            R2 = 0.277]. Fourth, we found that low levels of agreeableness
Supplementary Table 1.                                                                  and high levels of conscientiousness (β = 0.350), neuroticism
                                                                                        (β = 0.198), and AS (β = 0.252) had significant direct effects
Path Analyses                                                                           on OCD symptoms [F(6,128) = 5.78; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.221].
                                                                                        Analyses revealed a significant direct effect of high levels of
The first hypothesized hierarchical model was analyzed using
                                                                                        neuroticism (β = 0.411) and AS (β = 0.271) on the presence
regression-based path analyses (Kline, 1998; Sexton et al., 2003).
                                                                                        of SAD symptoms [F(6,128) = 9.07; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.309].
Figure 1 presents this hypothesized model with standardized
                                                                                        Finally, it appears that high levels of neuroticism (β = 0.408),
path coefficients. An examination of the effects indicated that
                                                                                        AS (β = 0.328) and low levels of agreeableness (β = −0.067)
neuroticism did not have a significant direct effect on AS
                                                                                        predict depression symptoms [F(6, 195) = 17.09; p =< 0.001;
[F(1,169) = 3.77; p = 0.054; R2 = 0.022]. Then, we examined
                                                                                        R2 = 0.336]. Second hypothesized model is summarized in
effects of the vulnerability variables on the presence of PD, OCD
                                                                                        Supplementary Table 3.
and GAD symptoms. Analyses have shown that high levels of
                                                                                           We used backward regression-based path analyses to test
                                                                                        the third hypothesized hierarchical model. Figure 3 presents
                                                                                        this model with standardized path coefficients. Examination
                                                                                        of the effects of the five personality dimensions on the four
                                                                                        AS dimensions revealed that low levels of agreeableness had
                                                                                        significant direct effects on the cognitive [F(5,123) = 4.24;
                                                                                        p = 0.001; R2 = 0.147; β = −0.382], physical [F(5,128) = 3.47;
                                                                                        p = 0.006; R2 = 0.124; β = −0.316] and control [F(5,
                                                                                        128) = 3.58; p = 0.005; R2 = 0.127; β = −0.284].Results showed
                                                                                        conscientiousness (β = 0.245) and openness (β = −322) also had
                                                                                        significant direct effects on the losing control dimension [F(5,
                                                                                        128) = 3.58; p = 0.005; R2 = 0.127].
                                                                                           Finally, in this model, we investigated the effects of the
                                                                                        five dimensions of the personality and of the four dimensions
                                                                                        of AS on specific anxiety symptoms. Analyses revealed that
                                                                                        high levels of conscientiousness (β = 0.205) and neuroticism
  FIGURE 1 | Summary of the first hypothesized model with standardized path
                                                                                        (β = 0.462) had significant direct significant effects on SP
  coefficients (PD, panic disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; OCD,
  obsessive-compulsive disorder) (∗ for p-values < 0.05; ∗∗ for p-values < 0.01).
                                                                                        symptoms [F(9,128) = 7.69; p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.368]. Neuroticism
                                                                                        had a direct effect on PD symptoms [F(9,128) = 7.69; p =< 0.001;

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                                       5                                        June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                                           Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

  FIGURE 2 | Summary of the second hypothesized model with standardized path coefficients (SP, social phobia; PD, panic disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety
  disorder; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SAD, separation anxiety disorder) (∗ for p-values < 0.05; ∗∗ for p-values < 0.01).

  FIGURE 3 | Summary of the third hypothesized model with standardized path coefficients (SP, social phobia; PD, panic disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder;
  OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SAD, separation anxiety disorder) (∗ for p-values < 0.05; ∗∗ for p-values < 0.01).

R2 = 0.368; β = 0.315]. Neuroticism (β = 0.345) and extraversion                      disorders and anxiety symptoms are supposed to arise from
(β = −0.235) had effect on GAD symptoms [F(9,128) = 5.81;                             hierarchical influences of trait-like factors as personality traits
p =< 0.001; R2 = 0.305]. Analyses revealed that high levels of                        and AS. Consequently, this study intends to explore the isolate
conscientiousness (β = 0.330) and neuroticism had significant                         and cumulate influences of those general vulnerability factors in
effects on OCD symptoms [F(9,128) = 4.20; p =< 0.001;                                 the development of anxiety symptoms in children between 8 and
R2 = 0.241]. We observed that high levels of neuroticism                              12 years of age.
(β = 0.390) and high levels of social concerns (β = 0.229) had a                         To meet this purpose, we first investigated the applicability
significant effect on SAD symptoms [F(9,128) = 6.97; p =< 0.001;                      of the Sexton et al.’s (2003) model in pediatric populations.
R2 = 0.345]. Finally, we observed that depressive symptoms                            This model suggests that neuroticism is a global vulnerability
were predicted by high levels of neuroticism [F(9,178) = 10.75;                       factor that directly influences the development of specific
p = 0.000; β = 0.400]. Third hypothesized model is summarized                         anxiety symptoms and influences the development of AS. While
in Supplementary Table 4.                                                             this model relies on a strong empirical background in adult
                                                                                      populations (Arrindell, 1993; Borger et al., 1996; Cox et al.,
                                                                                      1996; Lilienfeld, 1997; Taylor and Cox, 1998; Sexton et al., 2003),
DISCUSSION                                                                            we failed to demonstrate a significant influence of neuroticism
                                                                                      on AS levels in this study. With regard to this result, we
Previous studies conducted in adults (Brown et al., 1998; Taylor,                     hypothesize that, in children, AS may also be considered as a
1998; Barlow, 2000; Sexton et al., 2003) suggested that anxiety                       lower-order vulnerability factor in the same way as neuroticism.

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                                     6                                              June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                           Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

Consequently, in children, these factors should be equally treated.        with the level of AS. However, while PA had frequently been
Another explanation would be that in our sample neuroticism                associated to extraversion (Watson et al., 1988; Watson, 2002;
will be better represented by its lower-order factor, trait anxiety.       McNiel and Fleeson, 2006), we found here that the agreeableness
Our data suggest a strong correlation between these two variables          score was the significant predictor of the total AS level in
and numerous evidences in the adult literature suggest that                children. Consequently, in our sample it appears that PA relates
neuroticism and trait anxiety are highly similar constructs                more to this personality dimension and was also enhanced in
(Zinbarg and Barlow, 1996; Craske, 1999; Barlow, 2000; Sexton              previous research (DeNeve and Cooper, 1998). Recent evidence
et al., 2003). Therefore, we may hypothesize that the associations         suggests that low levels of agreeableness are associated with
between AS and trait anxiety is already strong during childhood            low emotional and behavioral regulation abilities due to poorer
and may broaden to the larger trait of neuroticism with age.               child cooperation, persistence, self-control and expressed affects
Consequently, it would be interesting to replicate this study by           (Ahadi and Rothbart, 1994; Caspi and Silva, 1995; Caspi,
adding the impact of trait anxiety in the different predicting             1998; Laursen et al., 2002). Accordingly, diminished emotional
models. Finally, it is also important to note that the absence of          regulation abilities could lead to a greater focus on anxiety
association between AS and neuroticism may also be attributable            symptoms. It would thus be possible that children with low levels
to our limited sample size and/or to the removal of a significant          of agreeableness are less able to regulate their emotions, which
number of outlying values, underlining the need of further                 lead them to be more focused on, and sensitive to their symptoms.
studies on a larger and more representative population.                    However, further studies need to replicate this result.
    Our analyses revealed that increased neuroticism scores                    In this second model, analyses confirmed that higher levels
are significant predictors of PD, GAD and OCD symptoms;                    of neuroticism and of AS are associated with increased anxiety
confirming previous results obtained in adults by Bienvenu and             symptoms, as we hypothesized with regards to previous literature
Stein (2003) and Sexton et al. (2003). As predicted, AS levels also        (Rabian et al., 1993; Joiner et al., 2002; McLaughlin et al., 2007).
appeared to be a predictor of those anxiety symptoms in youths,            However, they also showed that other frequently encountered
confirming previous research in this domain (Lau et al., 1996;             anxiety symptoms (SP, SAD and depression) in children are
Calamari et al., 2001; Reiss et al., 2001). Relationships between          predicted by increased neuroticism and AS. Those results are in
AS and PD is not surprising given the vast amount of literature            line with studies conducted in adults (Bienvenu et al., 2001; Eley
enhancing such effects (Kearney et al., 1997; Eley et al., 2009).          et al., 2004; Anderson and Hope, 2009) and they confirm the idea
Associations between AS and OCD are also understandable since,             that neuroticism and AS must be considered as direct cognitive
as proposed by Cox et al. (1999), AS may be represented in OCD             risk-factors of anxiety disorders.
as fear of loss of cognitive control symptoms that may stem from               Our analyses of this model also showed that other personality
the experience of unwanted, intrusive thoughts, coupled with an            dimensions appeared to act with neuroticism to have a significant
inflated sense of responsibility for those thoughts. Accordingly,          role in the predictive model of anxiety symptoms. Notably,
those intrusive thoughts and their negative consequences may               high neuroticism and low levels of extraversion predict GAD
be progressively feared. Finally, while AS had no influence on             symptoms, supporting studies demonstrating that GAD is
GAD symptoms in Sexton et al. (2003) study, we found here                  characterized by high-levels of self-focus attention (Mor and
a significant effect. However, the model of Sexton et al. (2003)           Winquist, 2002). Self-focus attention reflects to an awareness
proposed Intolerance of Uncertainty (IU) as the vulnerability              of self-reference or self-generated information (Ingram, 1990),
factor of excessive worry and GAD in adults, as previously                 which is conceptually opposed to extraversion. Furthermore,
shown by Koerner and Dugas (2008). Intolerance of uncertainty              high levels of neuroticism and conscientiousness predicted SP
(IU) refers to the tendency to react negatively on an emotional,           symptoms. These data are in accordance with the previous
cognitive and behavioral level to uncertain situations and events          suggestions of Bienvenu et al. (2004) according to which children
(Dugas et al., 2004). While studies conducted on IU in children            with high conscientiousness would show high levels of self-
are still scarce (Comer et al., 2009), we may hypothesize that             discipline and achievement striving. Those characteristics would
the significant effects of IU on GAD symptoms would appear                 predispose children to develop high representations of social
at a later age, with the increase of mentalization abilities of            situations and to avoid those situations consequently.
children, and that the significant effect of AS would in turn                  Surprisingly, analyses revealed that conscientiousness
decrease. However, as we choose here to focus on paths between             considered alone did not predict the occurrence of SP symptoms.
personality, AS and symptoms, further studies must address this            The same result was observed for extraversion in the model
question adding the child form of the Intolerance of Uncertainty           predicting GAD, revealing the importance of the connection of
Scale (Comer et al., 2009).                                                these personality traits with neuroticism. Finally, high levels of
    The second aim of this study was to examine the applicability          neuroticism and AS are coupled with low levels of agreeableness
of an extended model that comprised all five personality                   in the predicting model of depressive symptoms, raising the
dimensions of the Big Five model of personality (McCrae and                importance of emotion regulation difficulties in depression.
Costa, 1999) and other symptoms frequently encountered in                      The last model tested in our study aimed to evaluate
the young population such as SP, SAD and depression. Our                   the impact of personality traits on AS dimensions and the
results showed that AS is significantly predicted by low levels            impact of these dimensions on the investigated symptoms. We
of agreeableness. Previous results in adults (Brown et al., 1998;          first observed that associations between personality traits and
Norton et al., 2005) showed that positive affectivity (PA), which          anxiety symptoms confirmed the observations made in the
is a common-disorder factor of neuroticism, was associated                 second model presented in this study. Second, we found that

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                          7                                        June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                          Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

low levels of agreeableness significantly predicted the presence          thought to promote the presence of anxiety symptoms. Since
of physical, cognitive and control concerns about anxiety                 our second and extended hierarchical model seems to be the
symptoms, confirming the major role of agreeableness in the               one that best suits the data collected in our sample of children
development of concerns about anxiety symptoms. Nevertheless,             aged between 8 and 12, we can conclude that both high levels of
we found that the social dimension of the AS was not predicted            neuroticism and AS acts as vulnerability factors in the onset of
by any of the personality dimensions, suggesting it may depend            the vast majority of anxiety symptoms encountered in pediatric
on other vulnerability factors. Surprisingly, other personality           population. However, contrary to our expectations, we failed
dimensions appeared to have a significant role in the predicting          to demonstrate a hierarchical relationship between these two
model of those dimensions. Notably, fear of losing control                constructs. Our analyses suggest that, in children, AS would
concerns were associated to low levels of openness and high levels        depend more on low levels of agreeableness which underlines the
of conscientiousness. Interestingly, while correlated positively,         importance of PA. Our study also allows to extend the model
the four distinct dimensions of AS did not separately influence           of Sexton et al. (2003) by showing that neuroticism and AS act
anxiety symptoms, highlighting the importance to consider AS              in collaboration with conscientiousness and extraversion in the
as a unitary concept in the development of a predicting and               manifestations of anxiety symptoms. Finally, our data suggests
hierarchical model of anxiety symptoms. However, these results            the importance of considering AS as a unitary construct in the
may be directly connected to the factor structure of the CASI             predicting model of anxiety symptoms.
used in our study. Indeed, we used a four-factors structure                  Altogether, our results suggest that AS and other trait-like
(social, physical, cognitive, and control) as suggested by the            factors may act as risk factors in the development of later anxiety
research of Stassart and Etienne (2014) conducted on French-              disorder. However, to confirm this and to support previous data
speaking children. However, the factor structure of AS scales             demonstrating this (Weems et al., 2007; Schmidt et al., 2010),
set off numerous debates (Stassart and Etienne, 2014). Indeed,            it seems crucial to conduct a longitudinal study focusing on
while the majority of authors agreed to distinguish the fear              these aspects. If such results can be obtained in future work,
of physiological symptoms from mental, social and control                 researchers’ attention should be drawn on the construct of AS in
concerns, their subdivision into three dimensions is subtler, with        association with certain personality dimensions; notably in the
social and control concerns sometimes considered as a unified             development of interventions that would help children detect
dimension. Further studies should therefore be conducted to               and recognize their symptoms of anxiety and help them to
investigate the precise factorial structure of the CAS.                   interpret them correctly.
    It is also important to consider the results obtained in this
study, since participants came from a community sample of
children reporting no neurological or developmental disorders             AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
and reporting non-clinical levels of anxiety. This study should
be replicated in children suffering from clinical anxiety disorders       EW and MR had the initial ideas. EW collected and wrote the
to confirm the role of personality traits and AS in specific              drafts and the final manuscript. EW and KH analyzed the data.
fears and anxieties in children. Indeed, the replication of the           MR reviewed the several drafts of the manuscript. KEB, WB, and
hierarchical model developed by Sexton et al. (2003) in clinical          LL revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version
samples allowed us to draw some consequent conclusions                    of the manuscript.
about the model.
    A limitation of this study is that we decided to use
self-reported inventories for anxiety similar to that used for            FUNDING
personality. We may question the validity of self-reported
variables in children because of their limited cognitive abilities        This research was funded by a National Fund for Human
and their possible lack of engagement toward those methods.               Research (FRESH/FNRS) Credit (No: 6200918F) granted to EW.
However, Measelle et al. (2005) found that children as early as
5 years of age, are able to describe themselves reliably on a self-
reported Big Five scale and that their ratings were increasingly          ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
consistent with evaluations by parents and teachers. These data
confirm that young children already have a coherent, stable and           The authors would like to thank the master-degree students of
valid perception of themselves and confirming previous studies            the University of Mons (Belgium) who helped to collect the data.
enhancing the validity to investigate personality dimensions in           They would also like to thank Michelle Udoh (University of
this population which has also long been debated (Tackett et al.,         Leicester, United Kingdom) for her useful comments on the last
2008, 2012; Soto and John, 2014). Future studies should also              version of this text.
multiply the number of informants to assess children’s anxiety
and depression levels.
    In conclusion, this study aimed to draw a hierarchical model          SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
of the associations between personality traits, AS and anxiety
symptoms. In Sexton et al. (2003) model developed on adults,              The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online
AS was considered as a specific vulnerability factor that depends         at:     https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.
on a subordinate vulnerability factor, neuroticism, and both are          01185/full#supplementary-material

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                         8                                        June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                                                        Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

REFERENCES                                                                                     Bulbena, A., and Berrios, G. (1993). Cognitive function in the affective disorders: a
                                                                                                  prospective study. Psychopathology 26, 6–12. doi: 10.1159/000284794
Achenbach, T. M., and Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior                   Calamari, J. E., Hale, L. R., Heffelfinger, S. K., Janeck, A. S., Lau, J. J., Weerts,
    Checklist And Revised Child Behavior Profile. Vermont VT: Department of                       M. A., et al. (2001). Relations between anxiety sensitivity and panic symptoms
    Psychiatry of the University of Vermont.                                                      in nonreferred children and adolescents. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 32,
Ahadi, S. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (1994). “Temperament, development, and the                      117–136. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(01)00026-x
    big five,” in The Developing Structure of Temperament and Personality From                 Caspi, A. (1998). From child to adult: personality continuities across the life course.
    Infancy to Adulthood, eds C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, and R. P. Martin                  Zeitschrift fur Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 19, 1–1.
    (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc).                                         Caspi, A., and Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict
American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical                         personality traits in young adulthood: longitudinal evidence from a birth
    Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5 R ). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric                    cohort. Child Dev. 66, 486–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00885.x
    Publishing, Inc.                                                                           Chorpita, B. F., Tracey, S. A., Brown, T. A., Collica, T. J., and Barlow, D. H. (1997).
Anderson, E. R., and Hope, D. A. (2009). The relationship among social phobia,                    Assessment of worry in children and adolescents: an adaptation of the Penn
    objective and perceived physiological reactivity, and anxiety sensitivity in an               State Worry Questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 569–581. doi: 10.1016/s0005-
    adolescent population. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.             7967(96)00116-7
    03.011                                                                                     Chorpita, B. F., Yim, L., Moffitt, C., Umemoto, L. A., and Francis, S. E. (2000).
Arrindell, W. A. (1993). The fear of fear concept: evidence in favour of                          Assessment of symptoms of DSM-IV anxiety and depression in children: a
    multidimensionality. Behav. Res. Ther. 31, 507–518. doi: 10.1016/0005-                        revised child anxiety and depression scale. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 835–855. doi:
    7967(93)90133-f                                                                               10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00130-8
Balázs, J., Miklósi, M., Keresztény, Á, Hoven, C. W., Carli, V., Wasserman, C., et al.         Clark, L. A., Watson, D., and Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and
    (2017). Adolescent subthreshold-depression and anxiety: Psychopathology,                      the mood and anxiety disorders. J. Abnor. Psychol. 103, 103–116. doi: 10.1037/
    functional impairment and increased suicide risk J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry                 /0021-843x.103.1.103
    54, 670–677. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12016                                                       Comer, J. S., Roy, A. K., Furr, J. M., Gotimer, K., Beidas, R. S., Dugas, M. J.,
Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Rabasca, A., and Pastorelli, C. (2003). A                       et al. (2009). The intolerance of uncertainty scale for children: a psychometric
    questionnaire for measuring the big five in late childhood. Pers. Individ. Diff.              evaluation. Psychol. Assess. 21, 402–411. doi: 10.1037/a0016719
    34, 645–664. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00051-x                                            Copeland, W. E., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., and Angold, A. (2009). Childhood
Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from                  and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders.
    the perspective of emotion theory. Am. Psychol. 55:1247. doi: 10.1037//0003-                  Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 764–772. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85
    066x.55.11.1247                                                                            Costa, P. T. Jr., and McCrae, R. R. (1990). Personality disorders and the five-factor
Beesdo, K., Knappe, S., and Pine, D. S. (2009). Anxiety and anxiety disorders in                  model of personality. J. Pers. Disord. 4, 362–371.
    children and adolescents: developmental issues and implications for DSM-V.                 Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., and Angold, A. (2004). Developmental epidemiology of
    Psychiatric Clin. 32, 483–524. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2009.06.002                                 anxiety disorders. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 14, 631–648.
Bienvenu, O. J., Nestadt, G., Samuels, J. F., Costa, P. T., Howard, W. T., and Eaton,          Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., and Angold, A. (2005). The developmental
    W. W. (2001). Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders and the five-factor               epidemiology of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and
    model of personality. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 189, 154–161. doi: 10.1097/00005053-               comorbidity. Child Adoles. Psychiatr. Clin. 14, 631–648. doi: 10.1016/j.
    200103000-00003                                                                               chc.2005.06.003
Bienvenu, O. J., Samuels, J. F., Costa, P. T., Reti, I. M., Eaton, W. W., and                  Cox, B. J., Borger, S. C., Taylor, S., Fuentes, K., and Ross, L. M. (1999). Anxiety
    Nestadt, G. (2004). Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model                sensitivity and the five-factor model of personality. Behav. Res Ther. 37,
    of personality: a higher-and lower-order personality trait investigation in a                 633–641. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00174-0
    community sample. Depress. Anxiety 20, 92–97. doi: 10.1002/da.20026                        Cox, B. J., Parker, J. D., and Swinson, R. P. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity: confirmatory
Bienvenu, O. J., and Stein, M. B. (2003). Personality and anxiety disorders: a review.            evidence for a multidimensional construct. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 591–598. doi:
    J. Pers. Disord. 17, 139–151. doi: 10.1521/pedi.17.2.139.23991                                10.1016/0005-7967(96)00006-x
Bilgiç, A., Türkoğlu, S., Özcan, Ö, Tufan, A. E., Yılmaz, S., and Yüksel, T.                  Craske, M. G. (1999). Anxiety Disorders: Psychological Approaches to Theory and
    (2013). Relationship between anxiety, anxiety sensitivity and conduct disorder                Treatment. Colorado, CO: Westview Press.
    symptoms in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity                  Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Epstein, A., Wittchen, H. U., Pine, D. S., Lewis-
    disorder (ADHD). Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 22, 523–532. doi: 10.1007/                    Fernández, R., et al. (2010). Panic disorder: a review of DSM-IV panic disorder
    s00787-013-0392-z                                                                             and proposals for DSM-V. Depress Anxiety 27, 93–112. doi: 10.1002/da.20654
Bittner, A., Egger, H. L., Erkanli, A., Costello, E. J., Foley, D. L., and Angold,             Daleiden, E. L. (1998). Childhood anxiety and memory functioning: a comparison
    A. (2007). What do childhood anxiety disorders predict? J. Child Psychol.                     of systemic and processing accounts. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 68, 216–235. doi:
    Psychiatry 48, 1174–1183. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01812.x                               10.1006/jecp.1997.2429
Blais, M. A., Otto, M. W., Zucker, B. G., McNally, R. J., Schmidt, N. B., Fava,                Deacon, B., and Abramowitz, J. (2006). Anxiety sensitivity and its dimensions
    M., et al. (2001). The anxiety sensitivity index: item analysis and suggestions               across the anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 20, 837–857. doi: 10.1016/j.
    for refinement. J. Pers. Assess. 77, 272–294. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa                       janxdis.2006.01.003
    7702_10                                                                                    DeNeve, K. M., and Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: a meta-analysis
Borger, S., Cox, B., Fuentes, K., and Ross, L. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity and the five-          of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 124:197. doi:
    factor model of personality. Paper presented at the Poster presented at the annual            10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197
    meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy. Atlanta, GA                Dugas, M. J., Buhr, K., and Ladouceur, R. (2004). “The Role of Intolerance of
Bouvard, M., Denis, A., and Roulin, J.-L. (2015). The French version of the revised               Uncertainty in Etiology and Maintenance,” in Generalized Anxiety Disorder:
    child anxiety and depression scale (RCADS) in a nonclinical sample. Swiss J.                  Advances in Research and Practice, eds R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, and D. S.
    Psychol. 74:119. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000158                                               Mennin (New York, NY: Guilford Press).
Brandes, M., and Bienvenu, O. J. (2006). Personality and anxiety disorders. Curr.              Ehrler, D. J., Evans, J. G., and McGhee, R. L. (1999). Extending Big-Five theory
    Psychiatry Rep. 8, 263–269.                                                                   into childhood: a preliminary investigation into the relationship between Big-
Brown, T. A. (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among                           Five personality traits and behavior problems in children. Psychol. Schools 36,
    dimensions of temperament and DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorder                                451–458. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6807(199911)36:63.0.co;2-e
    constructs. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116:313.            doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.116.            Eley, T. C., Lau, J. Y. F., and Gregory, A. M. (2009). Unravelling Gene-Environment
    2.313                                                                                         Interplay for Children and Adolescent Depression. ACAMH Occasional Papers
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., and Barlow, D. H. (1998). Structural relationships                 No. 28, 19–27.
    among dimensions of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and dimensions                   Eley, T. C., Stirling, L., Ehlers, A., Gregory, A. M., and Clark, D. M. (2004). Heart-
    of negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic arousal. J. Abnorm. Psychol.               beat perception, panic/somatic symptoms and anxiety sensitivity in children.
    107:179. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.2.179                                                    Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 439–448. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(03)00152-9

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                                              9                                                  June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
Wauthia et al.                                                                                                                        Moderators of Anxiety Symptoms in Children

Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Ridder, E. M., and Beautrais, A. L. (2005).                 McNally, R. J. (1989). Is anxiety sensitivity distinguishable from trait anxiety?
    Subthreshold depression in adolescence and mental health outcomes in                         reply to Lilienfeld, Jacob, and Turner (1989). J Abnorm. Psychol. 98, 193–194.
    adulthood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 66–72.                                                  doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.98.2.193
Fergusson, D. M., and Woodward, L. J. (2002). Mental health, educational, and                 McNiel, J. M., and Fleeson, W. (2006). The causal effects of extraversion on positive
    social role outcomes of adolescents with depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59,               affect and neuroticism on negative affect: manipulating state extraversion and
    225–231.                                                                                     state neuroticism in an experimental approach. J. Res. Pers. 40, 529–550. doi:
Fombonne, E., Chedan, F., Carradec, A., and Achard, S. (1988). Le Child Behavior                 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.05.003
    Checklist: un instrument pour la recherche en psychiatrie de l’enfant. Psychiatr.         McRae, R., and Costa, P. (1996). “Toward a new generation of personality theories:
    Psychobiol. 3, 409–418.                                                                      Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model,” in The Five-Factor Model of
Furmark, T. (2002). Social phobia: overview of community surveys. Acta Psychiatr.                Personality: Theoretical Perspectives, ed. J. S. Wiggins (New York, NY: Guilford
    Scand. 105, 84–93. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.1r103.x                                     Press.).
Harman, H. H. (1970). Modern Factor Analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago                  Measelle, J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., and Cowan, C. P. (2005).
    Press.                                                                                       Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self-reports on the big five
Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Kraemer, H. C., and Taylor, C. B. (2000). Predictors of              dimensions? A longitudinal study from ages 5 to 7. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89:90.
    panic attacks in adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 207–214.            doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.90
Hoaglin, D. C., and Iglewicz, B. (1987). Fine-tuning some resistant rules for                 Mor, N., and Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: a
    outlier labeling. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 82, 1147–1149. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1987.             meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 128:638. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
    10478551                                                                                  Muris, P. (2002). An expanded childhood anxiety sensitivity index: its
Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: review and a                 factor structure, reliability, and validity in a non-clinical adolescent
    conceptual model. Psychol. Bull. 107:156. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.107.2.156                  sample. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 299–311.                doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(00)
Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, N. B., Schmidt, K. L., Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Perez,             00112-1
    M., et al. (2002). Anxiety sensitivity as a specific and unique marker of anxious         Muris, P., de Jong, P. J., and Engelen, S. (2004). Relationships between neuroticism,
    symptoms in youth psychiatric inpatients. J. Abnormal Child Psychol. 30,                     attentional control, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical children.
    167–175.                                                                                     Pers. Individ. Diff. 37, 789–797. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.007
Kearney, C. A., Albano, A. M., Eisen, A. R., Allan, W. D., and Barlow, D. H. (1997).          Naragon-Gainey, K., Rutter, L. A., and Brown, T. A. (2014). The interaction of
    The phenomenology of panic disorder in youngsters: an empirical study of                     extraversion and anxiety sensitivity on social anxiety: evidence of specificity
    a clinical sample. J. Anxiety Disord. 11, 49–62. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(96)                 relative to depression. Behav. Ther. 45, 418–429. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.
    00034-5                                                                                      01.004
Kendall, P. C., Brady, E. U., and Verduin, T. L. (2001). Comorbidity in childhood             Newby, J., Pitura, V. A., Penney, A. M., Klein, R. G., Flett, G. L., and Hewitt,
    anxiety disorders and treatment outcome. J. Am. Acad. Child Adoles. Psychiatry               P. L. (2017). Neuroticism and perfectionism as predictors of social anxiety. Pers.
    40, 787–794.                                                                                 Individ. Diff. 106, 263–267. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.057
Kendall, P. C., and Treadwell, K. R. (2007). The role of self-statements as a mediator        Norton, P. J., and Price, E. C. (2007). A meta-analytic review of adult
    in treatment for youth with anxiety disorders. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75:380.            cognitive-behavioral treatment outcome across the anxiety disorders.
    doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.75.3.380                                                              J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 195, 521–531.               doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253843.
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., and Wittchen,                   70149.9a
    H. U. (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk               Norton, P. J., Sexton, K. A., Walker, J. R., and Ron Norton, G. (2005). Hierarchical
    of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr.               model of vulnerabilities for anxiety: Replication and extension with a clinical
    Res. 21, 169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359                                                      sample. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 34, 50–63. doi: 10.1080/16506070410005401
Kılıç, E. Z., Kılıç, C., and Yılmaz, S. (2008). Is anxiety sensitivity a predictor of         Olatunji, B. O., and Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2009). Anxiety Sensitivity and the
    PTSD in children and adolescents? J. Psychoso. Res. 65, 81–86. doi: 10.1016/j.               Anxiety Disorders: A Meta-Analytic Review and Synthesis. Washington, D.C:
    jpsychores.2008.02.013                                                                       American Psychological Association.
Kline, R. B. (1998). Software review: software programs for structural equation               Olivier, M., and Herve, M. (2015). The Big five questionnaire for children (BFQ-C):
    modeling: AMOS, EQS, and LISREL. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 16, 343–364. doi:                    a french validation on 8-to 14-year-old children. Pers. Individ. Diff. 87, 55–58.
    10.1177/073428299801600407                                                                   doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.030
Koerner, N., and Dugas, M. J. (2008). An investigation of appraisals in individuals           Peterson, R. A., and Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety Sensitivity Index. Lawrence: Erlbaum
    vulnerable to excessive worry: the role of intolerance of uncertainty. Cogn. Ther.           Associates.
    Res. 32, 619–638. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9125-2                                          Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., and Ma, Y. (1998). The risk for early-
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., and Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” Personality              adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and
    Traits to Anxiety, Depressive, and Substance Use Disorders: A Meta-Analysis.                 depressive disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 56–64.
    Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.                                      Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common
Lau, J. J., Calamari, J. E., and Waraczynski, M. (1996). Panic attack                            method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and
    symptomatology and anxiety sensitivity in adolescents. J. Anxiety Disord. 10,                recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88:879. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.
    355–364. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(96)00016-3                                                   5.879
Laursen, B., Pulkkinen, L., and Adams, R. (2002). The antecedents and correlates              Rabian, B., Embry, L., and MacIntyre, D. (1999). Behavioral validation of the
    of agreeableness in adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 38:591. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.                childhood anxiety sensitivity index in children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 28,
    38.4.591                                                                                     105–112. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2801_9
Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). The relation of anxiety sensitivity to higher and lower             Rabian, B., Peterson, R. A., Richters, J., and Jensen, P. S. (1993). Anxiety sensitivity
    order personality dimensions: implications for the etiology of panic attacks.                among anxious children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 22, 441–446. doi: 10.1207/
    J. Abnorm. Psychol. 106:539. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.106.4.539                                s15374424jccp2204_4
Mazzone, L., Ducci, F., Scoto, M. C., Passaniti, E., D’Arrigo, V. G., and Vitiello, B.        Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., and McNally, R. J. (1986). Anxiety
    (2007). The role of anxiety symptoms in school performance in a community                    sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav. Res. Ther.
    sample of children and adolescents. BMC Public Health 7:347.                                 24, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality.              Reiss, S., Silverman, W. K., and Weems, C. F. (2001). “Anxiety sensitivity,” in The
    Handbook Pers. Theory Res. 2, 139–153.                                                       Developmental Psychopathology of Anxiety, eds M. W. Vasey and M. R. Dadds
McLaughlin, E. N., Stewart, S. H., and Taylor, S. (2007). Childhood anxiety                      (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 92–111.
    sensitivity index factors predict unique variance in DSM-IV anxiety                       Rossier, J., Quartier, V., Enescu, R., and Iselin, A. (2007). Validation of the french
    disorder symptoms. Cogn. Behav. Therapy 36, 210–219. doi: 10.1080/                           version of the hierarchical personality inventory for children (HiPIC). Eur. J.
    16506070701499988                                                                            Psychol. Assess. 23, 125–132. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.2.125

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org                                            10                                                   June 2019 | Volume 10 | Article 1185
You can also read