The Role of Memory in the Tower of London Task

Page created by Herbert Bailey
 
CONTINUE READING
ME MORY, 1999, 7 (2), 209±231

 The Role of Memory in the Tower of London Task
L.H. Phillips, V. W ynn, K.J. Gilhooly, S. Della Sala, & R.H. Logie
                              University of Aberdeen, UK

   The Tower of London (TOL) task is widely used as a neuropsychological test of
   planning. Relatively little is known of the cognitive com ponents of the task, and in
   particular the role of memory in performance. The current studies on normal adults
   looked at the role of verbal and spatial working memory in the TOL. The effects of
   verbal and visuospatial dual-task manipulations on TOL performance were
   examined in an experiment with 36 participants. Both verbal and visuospatial
   executive secondary tasks caused poorer performance on the TOL; however,
   concurrent articulatory suppression enhanced performance. The results suggest that
   executive and spatial com ponents are important in the task, and raise questions
   about the role of preplanning in the TOL.

                                    INTRODUCTION
There is currently much debate about how to describe and assess executive
control processes of cognition (e.g. Rabbitt, 1997 ; Reitan & W olfson, 1994).
This interest has arisen largely from the study of patients with lesions to the
frontal lobes of the brain, who exhibit failures of executive control, such as
failures of planning . Several ``frontal lobe tests’ ’ have been developed, which
have face validity as measures of executive functions, and are proposed to be
selectively sensitive to frontal lobe lesions. How ever, it is dangerous to assume
that poor performance on any such ``frontal test’ ’ indicates poor executive
function, because there are always multiple possible reasons for low scores
(Phillips, 1997), and the construct validity of such tests is unkno wn (Kafer &
Hunter, 1997). Moreover, these frontal tests lack specificity, i.e. patients with
lesions in other areas of the brain also show poor performance (Anderson,
Bigler, & Blatter, 1995 ; Grafman, Jonas, & Salazar, 1990). Frontal lobe tests are
increasingly used to measure executive function in clinical and non-clinical
populations so it is important that the cognitive processes involved in the tests
are more accurately specified.

  Requests for reprints should be sent to Louise Phillips, Psychology Department, University of
Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB24 2UB, Scotland, UK. Email: louise.phillips@abdn.ac.uk
  These studies were funded by Economic and Social Research Council (UK), grant number
R000236119. Thanks to Jim Urquhart for writing the programs described in this paper and Geoff
Ward for advice in developing the Tower of London task.

                                Ó   1999 Psychology Press Ltd
210       PHILLIPS ET AL.

   One such test which has been widely used to assess executive function is the
Tower of London (TOL) task, a variant on the Tow er of Hanoi task (Shallice,
1982 , 1988), see Fig. 1. In the TOL, coloured disks must be m oved one-by-one
from an initial state to m atch a goal state. Instructions are given to plan the
whole sequence of m oves that m ust be carried out m entally, before executing the
sequence. The task has been studied in a variety of patient groups and also in
norm al populations (e.g. Allamanno et al., 1987 ; Joyce & Robbins, 1991 ; Kafer
& Hunter, 1997 ; Morris, Ahm ed, Syed, & Toone, 1993; Owen et al., 1990 ;
Owen, Doyon, Petrides, & Evans, 1996 ; W ard & Allport, 1997 ; W atts,
MacLeod, & M orris, 1988). The TOL is used as a m easure of planning ability:
i.e. poor performance is interpreted as inability to plan efficiently (see e.g.
Morice & Delahunty , 1996 ; Owen, 1997 ; Shallice, 1982 ; Temple, Carney, &
Mullarkey, 1996). Frontal lobe patients perform poorly on the TOL; for
example, Owen et al. (1990) found that m ove times were slower in frontal lobe
patients, and argued that this was due to inefficient preplanning necessitating
extra on-line planning during the execution phase. Accuracy of plans is also
assessed in term s of the num ber of moves m ade in excess of the m inimum
possible to solve the trials, and frontal patients make a higher number of excess
moves than other patient groups (Owen et al., 1990; Shallice, 1982). However,

FIG. 1. A n exam p le o f a T ower o f L on d on tr ial.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK               211

the term ``planning ’ ’ is used loosely in neuropsychological literature (Goel &
Grafman, 1995 ; McCarthy & W arrington, 1990), and little is known of the
com ponent cognitive processes involved in m aking and executing plans (Kafer
& Hunter, 1997; Owen et al., 1990 ; W ard & Allport, 1997). Kafer and Hunter
(1997), using factor analytic techniques, argue that the TOL has poor validity as
a measure of planning, and em phasise the need to gain further inform ation about
the cognitive processes involved in this test.
   The current version of the TOL task involved com puterised presentation and
responses made using a lightpen. In order to investigate TOL perform ance in a
non-clinical popul ation, a wider range of difficulty was introduced than on the
standard neuropsychological version, with five disks, as used by W ard and
Allport (1997), rather than three disks. The TOL trials differed in the m inimum
num ber of m oves to solution, and num ber of indirect or counterintuitive moves
(moves that do not put a disk in its final goal position, but are essential for
solution; W ard & Allport, 1997). Trials with indirect moves engender conflict
between the overall goal (m ove disks to their final positions) and the subgoal
(move disk away from its final position). Such goal±subgoal conflicts are known
to cause particular difficulties for frontal lobe patients (Goel & Grafm an, 1995 ;
M orris, M iotto, Feigenbaum , Bullock, & Polkey, 1997), and to increase time
spent planning on the TOL (W ard & Allport, 1997).

Working Memory in the TOL Task
The role of memory in planning is often highlighted (e.g. Cohen, 1996; Hayes-
Roth & Hayes-Roth, 1979; Owen et al., 1990). Cohen (1996) argues that
working m em ory is important in form ulating, retaining, and im plementing plans,
as well as revising them on-line. In the TOL, it seems plausible that the setting
up, maintenance, and execution of a m ultistage plan will make considerable
dem ands on working memory resources. A contrasting argument is proposed by
W ard and Allport (1997). They describe a study in which the memory load of
the TOL task was decreased by allowing on-screen m ovem ent of the disks
during planning. Decreasing the m em ory load in this way did not affect time
spent planning, which W ard and Allport argue is evidence that working memory
resources do not lim it perform ance on the task. How ever, allowing people to
plan by m oving disks is boun d to include som e increase in tim e to make a plan
due to the motor responses required, so perhaps this is balanced out by a
correspondin g decrease in the actual time spent thinking about a plan. Also,
W ard and Allport do not report the effect of their working m em ory m anipulation
on the number of excess moves made, the variable that is taken to indicate
efficiency of planning in neuropsychological studies.
   The presentation and response requirements of the TOL are visual and
spatial, but it is not clear whether the planning requirem ents load verbal or
visuospatial memory resources. Do people carry out the task by silently
212     PHILLIPS ET AL.

verbalising a plan of action (M orris et al., 1993), or by visualising a sequence of
movem ents (Joyce & Robbin s, 1991 ; Owen et al., 1990 ; W elsh, Cicerello,
Cuneo, & Brennan, 1995)? There is evidence from experimental m anipulations
and neuropsychological patients that verbal and visuospatial memory are
separable aspects of the memory system (for a review see Della Sala & Logie,
1993).
    M any authors have highlighted the role of visuospatial memory in the TOL;
and especially visuospatial W M (Joyce & Robbin s, 1991 ; Morice & Delahunty,
1996 ; Owen et al., 1996). Temple et al. (1996) suggest that it is difficult to
verbalise the processes involved in the TOL task, implying that visuospatial
rehearsal m ust take place instead. W elsh et al. (1995) exam ined Tower of Hanoi
performance in a non-clinical popul ation. Interview data were taken after each
trial, and participants reported that they ``m entally visualised the m ovement of
rings on the pegs’ ’ , which W elsh et al. interpret to indicate the use of visuo-
spatial rehearsal in the task.
    A contrasting argument is put forward by M orris et al. (1993), who m aintain
that active verbal rehearsal is involved in the TOL because patient and brain
activation studies suggest left rather than right hem isphere involvem ent. Patient
deficits tend to be found only after left frontal lobe lesions (Glosser &
Goodglass, 1990 ; Shallice, 1982 ; although Owen et al., 1990 found TOL deficits
after right frontal lesions too). Also, in brain activation studies of normal
individuals during TOL perform ance, left frontal lobe activation was significant
while right frontal activation was not (M orris et al., 1993 ; Owen et al., 1996).
Although it is an oversim plification to associate left hemisphere activity
exclusively with language, Morris et al. argue that the lateralisation during TOL
performance is great enoug h to suggest verbal planning and rehearsal.
Relationships have been reported between measures of verbal W M and the
TOL in schizophrenics (M orice & Delahu nty, 1996).

Dual Task Effects on TOL Performance
This experiment exam ines the role of different com ponents of working memory
in TOL perform ance. The experimental paradigm draws on the influential three-
com ponent model of working memory (W M ) of Baddeley and Hitch (1974). The
model com prises a ``central executive’ ’ responsible for such cognitive control
functions as planning (Baddeley, 1986), a verbal buffer, and a visuospatial
buffer. The focus of research based on this model has been on experimental
manipulations, and in particular the effects of a range of dual tasks on
performance on memory and reasoning tasks (Baddeley, 1996 ; Gilhooly, Logie,
W etherick, & W ynn, 1993; Logie, Gilhooly, & W ynn, 1994). The model has
proved useful in exploring the involvem ent of different com ponents of cognition
in tasks of reasoning (Logie et al., 1994), and has support from both
neuropsychological and cognitive evidence (e.g. Della Sala & Logie, 1993 ;
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                 213

Smith & Jonides, 1997). By using a range of dual tasks, the involvem ent of the
different com ponents of the W M m odel can be addressed. Two important
distinctions are m ade: verbal vs visuospatial processes, and executive vs non-
executive processes. In the current study, four different types of secondary task
(described in more detail later) are exam ined in com bination with the TOL task:
articulatory suppression to load the phonol ogical loop, random num ber
generation to load the phonological loop and central executive, pattern tapping
to load the visuospatial scratchpad, and random tapping to load the scratchpad
and central executive. By com paring the effects of these different secondary
tasks on TOL performance, conclusions can be draw n about the role of the
verbal buffer, visuospatial buffer, and executive com ponents of memory in the
TOL task.

    1. Articulatory suppression: Articulatory suppression, repeatedly saying a
phrase or sequence, loads the phonol ogical loop com ponent of working memory
by preventing silent verbal rehearsal (Baddeley, 1986). It has been widely used
to exam ine the role of verbal rehearsal in cognitive tasks, and is known to
interfere with verbal short-term m em ory (Baddeley, Lewis, & Vallar, 1984).
Articulatory suppression does not interfere with all tasks (e.g. accuracy of
conditional reasoning, Evans & Brooks, 1981) supporting the argument that
interference effects are caused by the presence of verbal rehearsal rather than
general resource dem ands. In relation to the TOL, Shallice (1982) found that
articulatory suppression did not interfere with accuracy of TOL perform ance,
and concluded that the task was not carried out using verbal rehearsal. How ever,
perform ance on the articulation task was not measured, so it is unclear how dual
task trade-off factors m ight have influenced perform ance. In the current study
the effects of articulatory suppression on TOL perform ance and vice versa are
examined in detail, to explore the role of verbal rehearsal in the task.
    2. Random number generation: In relation to the Baddeley and Hitch m odel
of working memory, articulating a sequence of numbers that is as random as
possible loads the central executive component of working memory (and the
phonol ogical loop). Producing random outpu t dem ands repeated inhibition of
stereotyped autom atic sequences (Baddeley, 1986), and thus places considerable
dem ands on the executive processes. Random num ber generation has been found
to interfere with executive and reasoning tasks, such as figural fluency, a non-
verbal ``frontal lobe’ ’ test (Phillips, 1997), and syllogistic reasoning (Gilhooly et
al., 1993). However, it is im portant to note that random number generation does
not interfere with perform ance on all cognitive tasks: for exam ple random
generation did not significantly disrupt a m ental size com parison task (Pearson
& Logie, 1998). If random num ber generation interferes with TOL perform ance,
this would indicate that executive processing is involved in the task. How ever,
executive function is unlikely to be a single entity but rather a fractionated
system of control processes (Rabbitt, 1997), and it is of interest to speculate
214     PHILLIPS ET AL.

whether control of verbal and visuospatial processing m ight activate rather
different executive resources. If this is the case, and the TOL m akes demands
predominantly on visuospatial executive processing, then there m ight be
relatively little interference between random number generation and TOL.
    3. Spatial pattern tapping: This secondary task requires participants to tap out
a sim ple repeated spatial pattern. It is proposed to load the visuospatial
scratchpad com ponent of working memory (Logie & M archetti, 1991 ) and has
been shown to interfere with tasks such as the ``manikin’ ’ test which demands
mental rotation (Farm er, Berman, & Fletcher, 1986). Pattern tapping does not
interfere with tasks better solved by verbal strategies, such as syllogistic
reasoning (Gilhooly et al., 1993), or verbal (AB) reasoning (Farm er et al., 1986).
If spatial rehearsal processes are essential in the TOL (Joyce & Robbins, 1991 ;
Owen et al., 1990; W elsh et al., 1995), then pattern tapping should interfere with
TOL perform ance.
    4. Random tapping : In order to investigate the role of non-verbal executive
processes in cognitive tasks, tapping a sequence of locations as randomly as
possible has been used (Baddeley, 1996). This task loads both central executive
resources, and the visuospatial scratchpad. There is evidence that random tapping
interferes significantly with performanc e on a range of verbal fluency tasks which
are often used to test executive function (Phillips, Crawford, & Chapm an, 1998).
Again, it can be noted that random tapping does not interfere with all tasks: e.g.
short-term m em ory for colours rem ains intact (Pearson, 1996). Agreem ent that
visuospatial working m em ory is involved in the TOL suggests that random
tapping should interfere quite substantially with TOL performance.

   By com paring perform ance across the secondary tasks, conclusions about the
involvement of different cognitive components in the TOL can be reached. For
example com paring the effects of articulatory suppression (repeating the
num bers from one to ten) and random number generation (trying to generate as
random a sequence as possible using the numbers from one to ten) will allow
inferences about the involvem ent of executive processes over and above the
shared com ponents of the secondary tasks such as retrieval of digit names,
verbal output, and maintaining the timing of outpu t at a regular pace. Also,
com paring the effects of pattern tapping and random tapping will allow
inferences about the executive com ponents of generating randomness over and
abov e shared com pone nts suc h as co-ordina ting m otor respo nses and
maintaining timing of taps.
   It is predicted that secondary tasks with a high executive load should affect
TOL perform ance. Fu rther, the pattern of interference with articulatory
suppression or spatial tapping will be determ ined by the extent to which TOL
performance is dependent on verbal memory (M orris et al., 1993 ) or visuospatial
memory (Owen, 1997). Possible trade-offs between prim ary and secondary tasks
will also be investigated.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                           215

                                           METHOD
Participants
The participants were 36 youn g adults, 12 male and 24 fem ale, with a mean age
of 20, and an age range of 18 to 25. They were recruited through posters or the
subject panel of the Aberdeen Psycholog y Departm ent, and m ost were
undergraduate students.

Materials
The TOL task was carried out on a PC equipped with a 17 ¢ ¢ monitor and
lightpen. All com puter programs were written in Visual Basic 3.0. A wooden
board, 280m m         ´
                   280mm , was used for the random tapping secondary task. This
had a 135m m      ´
                  135m m opening containing nine 25mm square sprung switches
arranged in three rows of three. The board was adjusted for the pattern tapping
task by fixing a cross-shaped piece of black cardboard over the centre switches,
leaving four switches arranged in a square. An open-fronted box was used to
obscure the tapping board from view during performance of the task, to ensure
there were no visual distractions from the secondary tasks. The switches were
attached to another com puter for recording the location and latency of keypress
responses. The intervals between responses for the verbal secondary tasks were
recorded using a directional m icrophone attached to the second com puter via a
voice key. The actual verbal responses for the random generation secondary task
were recorded on a tape recorder. A digital metronom e set to beat once per
second was used to help participants regulate the rate of responses.

Procedure
   Tower of London Task
   There were eight 5-disk Tower of London trials in each condition. The
com plexity of trials differed in terms of the number of moves required for
solution (ranging from 3±11), and the number of indirect moves (ranging from
0±6), i.e. m oves that were essential to the optimum solution, but did not place a
                                                                                1
disk into its final goal position (Goel & Grafman, 1995; W ard & Allport, 1997 ) .
The trials were arranged in approxim ately ascending order of difficulty. See Fig.

  1
    Ward and Allport (1997) report that the number of ``chunks of indirect moves’ ’ is a better
predictor of planning time in the TOL than the number of minimum moves or number of indirect
moves. However, a program that computed all minimum move paths for TOL trials revealed that for
most trials there is more than one minimum-move solution path, and these paths differ in terms of
Ward and Allport’ s classification of ``chunks of indirect moves’ ’ but usually not in the ``number of
indirect moves’ ’ (see Appendix), supporting the use of the less ambiguous latter measure.
216     PHILLIPS ET AL.

1 for an exam ple of the screen layout. In the Appendix, the num ber of moves
required for each of the trials is listed. It can be noted that these are matched
across sets (with the exception of trial 8, where the trials in set A and B could be
solved in 10 moves, and the trial in set C in 11 moves: despite this there was no
significant difference between the sets in terms of the average number of moves
taken to solution, because relatively few individuals solved these trials in 10
moves).
    The participant was seated in front of the com puter m onitor with a
practice TOL trial on screen. Instructions were given to m ove the coloured
disks on the bottom set of three pegs to exactly m atch the positions of
those on the first set. Participants were told that there were two parts to each
trial: first mentally planning the moves to make the bottom set of disks m atch
those of the goal set in the fewest possible moves, and second using the lightpen
to move the disks on the bottom set of pegs as quickly as possible. The need to
preplan before beginning to move the actual disks was emphasised. W hen
satisfied that the final state of the disks m atched the goal state, participants
selected a box marked ``accept’ ’ to m ove on to the next trial. The com puter
recorded for each trial: preplan tim es (from appearance of disks to first
movem ent with lightpen), tim e taken to make each m ove (com bined into a m ean
move time per trial), and num ber of moves m ade to solution (converted to an
excess m oves measure by subtracting the minimum moves in which trials could
be solved).
    All participants carried out the Tower of Londo n (TOL) task in a control (task
alone) condition and in two dual conditions ( either verbal random generation
and articulatory suppression or spatial random generation and spatial tapping).
The presentation order of the three task conditions was alternated between
participants, and the three sets of problems were used equally for each condition.
The secondary tasks were also given in single-task conditions, with no TOL
task, in order to exam ine the trade-off between primary and secondary tasks. For
all tasks (including control conditions), a m etronom e beat w as played
throughout.
    TOL perform ance was assessed (in all control and dual-task conditions) in
terms of plan times, move tim es, and num ber of excess m oves. Perform ance was
assessed initially across all eight trials. TOL measures were analysed by separate
repeated measures ANOVAs on plan tim es, m ove times, and excess moves,
com paring scores in control and dual-task conditions. Post-hoc com parisons
between the different conditions were made using the LSD test. Analyses were
also carried out to exam ine whether the type of TOL trial (num ber of moves to
solution, and num ber of indirect or goal m oves) was influential in determ ining
the effects of dual tasks. Trials were analysed in pairs, m atched on the m inimum
num ber of m oves to solution, and num ber of indirect moves required: three sets
of two trials were com pared. Overall performance levels on each pair of trials
indicated similarity in terms of planning times and excess m oves.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                217

   Trials with no indirect moves: trials 1 & 2 had no indirect m oves, and three/
   five minim um moves to solution.
   Trials with two indirect moves: trials 4 & 5 had seven moves to solution and
   mostly required two indirect moves (the exception being trial B4 with three
   indirect m oves)
   Trials with four indirect moves: trials 6 & 7 required nine m oves to solution,
   and had four indirect moves.

   Repeated m easures ANOVAs were carried out to examine the effects of dual-
task condition and trial type on TOL perform ance in both verbal and visuospatial
conditions. Dependent measures (planning time, and m oving time) were
averaged across the two trials at each level of difficulty. Num ber of excess
moves was calculated, but with only two trials at each difficulty level, the
resulting distribu tions had large numbers of zero values, du e to m any
participants m aking no excess moves on individual trials. Accuracy of
perform ance was therefore recoded as a dichotomous variable (0 = no excess
moves made across the two trials, 1 = one or more excess moves made across the
two trials), and com parisons of the effects of secondary tasks on accuracy m ade
by carrying out separate Cochran’ s Q tests at each level of difficulty.

   Secondary Tasks
   Articulatory suppression: The participant was told to say the digits 1 to 9
   aloud repeatedly in time to the m etronom e beat.
   Verbal random generation: The participant was instructed to say aloud the
   digits 1 to 9 in as random an order as possible, in time to the m etronom e beat.
   Spatial pattern tapping: The participant was instructed to tap the four
   switches of the tapping board continuo usly in a clockw ise direction in time to
   the metronome beat, with the non-preferred hand.
   Spatial random generation: Participants were asked to tap each of the nine
   switches in as random an order as possible with the non-preferred hand, at the
   rate of one per second to the beat of the metronome.

    It is im portant to analyse perform ance on the secondary tasks, in order to
investigate possible dual-task trade-offs between primary and secondary tasks.
Secondary task performance was assessed in terms of mean inter-response
interval and intra-individual variability in response times. The two executive
secondary tasks were also assessed in term s of Evans’ RNG index of the relative
frequency of digram com binations, on which higher scores indicate less efficient
randomisation (Evans, 1978). In order to standardise the number of responses
be ing analysed, secon da ry task pe rform anc e m easures were calculated
separately for both the first and last 100 taps or vocalisations during TOL
perform ance. The latter 100 responses would have occurred during more
218       PHILLIPS ET AL.

dem anding TOL trials with a large number of indirect moves. Performance on
secon dary tasks was examined by separate ANOVAs on each secondary task
com paring control responses with the first 100 responses during TOL and last
100 responses during TOL.

                                          RESULTS
TOL Task Performance
   Preplanning Time
   Counterintuitively, the time spent preplanning on the TOL was significantly
reduced when the task was performed with any of the four secondary tasks. (see
Fig. 2, first row). Indeed, under articulatory suppression, participants planned for
only one third of the tim e spent during control TOL performance. Verbal
secondary tasks caused considerably faster preplan tim es, F(2,34) = 38.2,
P < .001, but there was no difference between articulatory suppression and
random num ber generation in their effects on TOL preplan tim es. Visuospatial
secon dary tasks also caused faster planning times, F(2,34) = 5.73, P < .01, but
again there was no difference between pattern tapp ing and random tapping in
their effects on TOL preplan times.

   Disk Moving Time
   The considerably faster preplan tim es during secondary task perform ance
might suggest that participants would be slower at m oving disks, because extra
time would have to be spent planning during the execution of moves. However,
this was not the case (see Fig. 2). Analysis revealed that there was no effect of
visuospatial secondary tasks on TOL move times, F(2,34) = 2.29, n.s. The verbal
conditions did differ in their effects on tim e spent moving , F(2,34) = 18.8,
P < .0001 , and post-hoc tests revealed that articulatory suppression differed from
both control and random generation conditions, such that m oves were m ade
more quickly during articulatory suppression.

   Number of Excess Moves Made
    There was a significant effect of verbal secondary tasks on the number of
moves made in the TOL task, F(2,34) = 4.30, P < .05 (see Fig. 2, third row).
Post-hoc tests revealed that random num ber generation resulted in more moves
being m ade than either articulatory suppression or control conditions. There was
also an effect of visuospatial secondary tasks on the number of moves made in
the TOL task, F (2, 34) = 3.76, P < .05, with ra n d o m t a p p in g ca usin g
significa n t ly m o re m o ves t o b e m a d e t h an in sin gle-t a sk co n d itio n s. P a tt er n
ta p p in g d id n ot q u ite sign ifica n t ly affect T O L a ccu ra cy as co m p a red t o
FIG. 2. P r eplan tim es (top ), m o ve tim es (m id dle), and nu m b er of m o ves tak en (b otto m ) on the
T O L t r ia ls in co n tr o l a n d seco nd a r y t ask co nd it io n s. Secon d a r y ta sk a b b r evia t io n s:
R G = ra nd om gen era tion o f n u m b ers, A S = a r ticu lato ry su pp ression , R T = r a ndo m ta ppin g,
P T = pa ttern tap ping.

                                                                                                              219
220       PHILLIPS ET AL.

co n t ro l p erfo r m a nce (th e L SD t est revea led th e sign ifican ce o f d ifferen ce
b etween p a tt ern t a p p in g a n d con t ro l t o b e P = .06).

   Trial Type and the Effects of Dual Tasks on TOL
    The next set of analyses examined the effects of trial type (in terms of number
of indirect m oves) and secondary task on TOL perform ance. In relation to the
verbal secondary tasks, there were no differences between control, articulatory
suppression, and random generation conditions in term s of the accuracy of
performance, at any of the three levels of difficulty (Cochran’ s Q values of 4.67,
4.24, and 4.00 for zero, two, and four indirect-m ove trials respectively). Next,
analysis of the effects of visuospatial secondary tasks on TOL excess m oves was
considered. There was no effect of the secondary tasks on accuracy at any level
of difficulty (Cochran’ s Q values of .17, 3.17, and .15, for zero, two, and four
indirect-m ove trials respectively).
    There was a significant effect of dual-task condition on planning times, such
that both articulatory suppression and random generation caused considerably
less tim e to be spent preplanning , F (2, 34) = 5.21, P = .01. T h ere was a lso a n
effect o f t ria l d ifficulty, F (2, 34) = 4.78, P < .05, with t h e t ria ls req u irin g
fo u r in d irect m o ves resu ltin g in m u ch lo n ger p la n n in g t im es t h an th o se with
n o in d irect m o ves. F u rt h er, th e in tera ctio n b etween d u a l-t a sk co n d itio n a n d
tria l t yp e wa s sign ifica n t , F (4, 68) = 5.93, P < .001, a s sh o wn in F ig. 3. In

FIG. 3. T he effects o f du a l task s a nd t ria l typ e o n T O L plan ning tim e.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                           221

t h e co n tro l co nd itio n , t he m o re d em a n d in g t ria ls resu lted in co nsid era b ly
lo n ger p la n n in g t im es (in crea sin g fro m 7 seco n d s t o 19 seco n d s), h o wever, in
b o th du a l-t a sk co n d itio n s t h ere wa s n o effect o f t ria l d ifficu lty o n p la n n in g
t im e, with a p p ro xim a t ely 6 seco n d s p er t ria l sp en t p la n n in g. A sim ila r
p a tt ern wa s seen in rela t io n t o t h e sp a tia l seco n d a ry t a sk s. D u a l-t a sk
co n d itio n a ffect ed th e t im e sp en t p la n n in g, with m u ch less t im e sp en t
p la n n in g d urin g con cu rren t p a t t ern o r ran d om ta p p in g, F (2, 34) = 8.11,
P < .01. T h ere was a lso a n effect o f t rial t yp e, F (2, 34) = 9.78, P < .001,
with fou r-in d irect -m o ve t ria ls p la n n ed m o re slo wly t ha n t wo - o r n o -ind irect -
m o ve tria ls. T h e in t era ctio n b etween d u al-t a sk co n d itio n a nd t ria l t yp e wa s
a lso sign ifican t , F (4, 68) = 2.55, P < .05, a s sh o wn in F ig. 3. Sim ila r t o t h e
verb al t ask s, in t h e co n tro l co n d itio n p la n n in g t im e in crea sed d ra m a t ica lly
fro m n o -in d irect-m o ve t ria ls (7 seco n d s) t o fo u r-in d irect-m o ve t ria ls (22
seco n d s), wh ereas p la n n in g t im es d u rin g seco n d a ry ta sk s d id no t in crea se a s
t ria l t yp e differed, rem a in in g a t a rou n d 10 seco n d s.
     In relation to the tim e spent moving on the TOL trials, there was an effect of
du al-task con dition for verba l seconda ry tasks, F (2, 34) = 14.0, w it h
a rticu la t o ry su p p ressio n ca u sin g fa ster m o ve t im es th a n eith er ra nd o m
gen era t io n o r co n t ro l co n d itio ns. T h e effect o f t ria l t yp e on m o ve t im es wa s
a lso sign ifica n t, F (2, 34) = 5.12, P < .05, with fou r-in d irect -m o ve trials
b ein g p erfo rm ed m o re qu ick ly t h a n t wo -in d irect -m o ve tria ls. T h er e wa s n o
in t era ctio n bet ween d u a l-t a sk con d itio n a n d t ria l t yp e, F (4, 68) = 1.44.
T h ere wa s n o effect o f t h e sp a tial seco n da ry t ask s o n T O L m o ve t im es, F (2,
34) = 2.66. T ria l t yp e d id affect m o ve t im es, F (2, 34) = 5.65, P < .01, with
lo n gest m o ve t im es o n t h e t wo -in d irect -m o ve t ria ls (as in t h e verb al
co n d itio n s d escrib ed ea rlier). F in ally, t h ere was no in t er action between trial
t yp e an d d u a l-t ask co nd itio n in d eterm in ing m o ve t im es, F (4, 68 ) = .516.

Trade-off Between Primary and Secondary Tasks
It is extrem ely im portant, when using dual-task m ethods, to exam ine the
perform ance on both primary and secondary tasks in order to establish whether
perform ance trade-offs are occurring. W e therefore report the results from
analyses of inter-response intervals, variability, and Evans RNG scores for
secondary task data.

  Analysis of Inter-response-Intervals (IRIs) from
Secondary Tasks
    The data on IRIs are plotted in Fig. 4. Concurrently perform ing the Tower of
London caused slower IRIs com pared to control performance in: the articulatory
suppression task, F (2, 34) = 6.87, P < .01; ran d o m gen era t ion o f n u m b ers,
F (2, 34) = 4.19, P < .05; a n d p a tt ern t a p p in g, F (2, 34) = 6.99, P < .01.
T h ere wa s n o sign ifica n t effect o f co ncu rren t T O L perfo r m a nce on t h e IR Is
FIG. 4.    M ea n in ter-r espo n se-interva ls (to p gr a phs) fo r contr ol a nd fir st / last 100 r espo nses o f
the second ar y ta sk s d u rin g T O L ; a n d st an dar d d eviat io ns (b o ttom gr ap hs) of con tr ol an d fir st /
last 100 I R I s du rin g T O L . Seco nd ar y ta sk a b br evia tion s: R G = r a ndo m genera tion o f n um bers,
A S = ar ticula tory su p p ression , R T = ra nd om ta pp in g, P T = p attern ta ppin g.

222
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                             223

fo r ra n do m t ap p in g resp on ses, F (2, 34) = .67, n .s. T hese resu lts su ggest t h a t
seco n d a ry t a sk perfo rm a n ce wa s gen era lly slo wed wh ile co n cu rren tly
ca rryin g o u t t h e la t t er T o wer o f L o n d o n t ria ls, excep t in th e ca se o f
ra nd o m ta p p in g.

   Intra-individual Variability of IRIs
    A more sensitive index of dual-task costs is often the variability of IRIs in the
secondary task. This measures how accurately individuals can maintain a steady
rhythm of verbal or tapping responses. The variability of the control, and first
and last 100 IRIs during TOL for the four secondary tasks are plotted in Fig. 4,
second row. Control performance showed less variable IRIs than dual-task
perform ance in: articulatory suppression, F (2, 34) = 7.92, P < .01; ra nd o m
n u m b er gen er a t io n , F (2, 34 ) = 3.95, P < .05; p a t t er n t a p p in g, F (2,
34) = 10. 5, P < .001; a n d ra n d o m t ap p in g, F (2, 34) = 13.01 , P < .0001.
Seco n d a ry t a sk resp o n ses were co n sid era b ly m o re va ria b le d u rin g T O L
a cro ss all o f th e t a sk s.

   Randomness of Number Generation and Tapping
   The final analyses of secondary task perform ance com pared the randomness
of verbal output and keypresses using the Evans’ (1978) RNG index in control
and concurrent TOL perform ance conditions (see Fig. 5). Verbal random
num ber generation was no t significantly less random when carried out
concurrently w ith the TO L task, althou gh the com parison app roached
significance, F (2, 34) = 3.06, P = .06. In co n tra st, t ap p in g wa s sign ifica n t ly
less ra n d o m d u rin g T O L , F (2, 34) = 6.33, P < .01.

                                         DISCUSSION
Ca rrying out any of the secondary tasks caused participants to reduce
substantially the am ount of time spent preplanning on the TOL task. This
effect was consistent in both verbal and visuospatial, and high and low
executive-loading secondary tasks. In control con ditions, planning tim e
increased dramatically as the number of m oves and indirect moves required to
solve trials increased. In contrast, there was no increase in planning time in
corresponden ce to trial difficulty under any of the dual-task conditions. This
suggests that accurate preplanning for com plex TOL trials was not carried out
during secon dary task conditions. These results were contrary to expectations,
and suggest that either people ca nnot plan wh ile carryin g o u t an y o t h er
co gn itive a ctivit y; o r t ha t it is t o o effo rt fu l a n d a versive t o d o b o t h t h in gs a t
o n ce.
   The TOL is usually seen as a task measuring the ability to plan: so preventing
planning activity would be though t to cause poorer accuracy and slower
224        PHILLIPS ET AL.

FIG. 5. E van s’ m ea su r e o f r an do m n ess (R N G ind ices) in contr o l con dition s a nd dur ing fir st /
last 100 in ter-r esp on se interva ls con cur r ently with th e T O L . H igh er ind ex = less r a ndo m
pr o du ctio n.

execution of the task. How ever, none of the secondary tasks caused slower
execution times. This was particularly surprising in the case of the spatial
secon dary tasks, in which the motor com ponent of the non-preferred hand m ight
have interfered with TOL execution by the preferred hand. Further, articulatory
suppression caused people to execute the TOL move sequence m ore quickly
than in the control condition.
    Both random tapping and random number generation caused more m oves to
be m ade on the TOL, with the effect of pattern tapping on excess moves
approaching significance. This suggests involvem ent of executive com ponents
of working m emory in effective performance on the TOL, along with spatial
memory. It could be argued that the detrimental effects of random tapping and
random num ber generation on the num ber of excess moves m ade might be due
to low preplanning times. This would suggest that the locus of the executive
com ponent in the TOL is generating a mental plan of m oves. How ever, this
explanation can be questioned. The condition that caused the greatest reduction
in preplanning tim e, concurrent articulatory suppression, did not cause any
change in the number of excess movesÐ indeed it resulted in a trend towards
fewer excess m o ves. T h is su ggest s t h at t h e in crea ses in excess m o ves d u rin g
o th er seco n d a ry t ask co n ditio n s were n o t a d irect resu lt o f decrea sed t im e
sp en t p la nn in g. F ur t her, p la n n in g t im es wer e o n ly su b sta n t ia lly red u ced b y
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                           225

seco n d a ry t a sks d u rin g t h e m o st d em a n d ing T O L t ria ls, yet th ere wa s n o
evid en ce t h a t t h e effects o f execu t ive seco n d a ry t a sk s o n excess m o ves wer e
stro nger o n t h ese t ria ls. A ltern a t ively, execu tive seco n d a ry t a sk s m ay a ffect
excess m o ves by redu cin g o n -lin e p la n n in g d u rin g T O L execu t io n . T h is fit s
with t h e m o d el o f ``o p po r t un istic p la n n in g’ ’ p ro po sed b y H a yes-R o t h a n d
H a yes-R o th (1979), in wh ich p la n n in g is la rgely an a ctivit y ca rried o u t in
sm a ll b u rsts d u ring co gn itive a ctivit y, rat h er t h a n a sep a rat e p h a se in wh ich
en t ire p la n s a re m a d e o f p erfo rm an ce b efo re execu t io n .
    Although non-executive dual tasks dram atically decreased the am ount of
tim e participants spent preplanning their moves, this did not have a strong effect
on the accuracy of TOL perform ance. This concurs with findings (W ynn et al.,
1997 ) that varying initial preplanning tim es on the TOL task did not affect the
accuracy with which people performed. In one study, some participants were
instructed to plan as usual in the TOL task, while others were not told to plan.
Those who were not told to plan spent considerably less time preplanning than
those following ``plan’ ’ instructions, yet the two groups did not differ in term s
of the num ber of excess moves made. The use of the TOL as a measure of
planning can be questioned in light of the current evidence. Although som e elite
individuals may be a ble t o p rep la n a la rge n u m b er o f m o ves in th e T O L t a sk
if fo rced t o d o so b y t a sk in stru ctio n s (W a rd & A llp o rt , 1997), it seem s
u n lik ely th a t m o st in d ivid u als will d o so if given t h e u su a l in stru ctio n s used
in n eu ro p sych o lo gical a d m in ist rat io n o f t h e T O L : i.e. bein g t o ld t o p rep lan ,
b u t n o t p r even ted fro m o n -lin e p la n n in g d urin g t h e t ask . O n a verage,
p a rt icip a n t s a re lik ely to u se a stra t egy o f o n -line p la n n in g b ecau se it is less
d em a n d in g, yet effect ive in so lvin g t h e ta sk . P o o r p erform an ce o n t he T O L
b y a n y in d ivid u a l o r clin ica l gr o u p sh o u ld n o t b e in terp reted as d u e t o
d ysfu n ctio n al p la nn in g wit ho u t co nvin cin g su p p o rtin g evid en ce: a n u m b er
o f o t h er co gn itive d eficit s co u ld a lso ca u se p o o r p erfo rm a n ce.
    In general, concurrent articulatory suppression appears to have had very
beneficial effects on TOL performance: preplan tim es were a third that of the
control condition, move times were significantly faster, and the num ber of
moves made was fewer than in the control condition (although this difference
was not significant). This apparently counterintuitive result concurs with other
findings (e.g. Brandim onte & Gerbino, 1993 ; Hitch, Brandimonte, & W alker,
19 95) which sug gest that interfering with verbal articulation im proves
perform ance on some visuospatial tasks. Brandim onte and Gerbino (1993)
argue that articulatory suppression discourages the use of inefficient verbal
strategies on visuospatial tasks, and promotes the use of more appropriate
visuospatial strategies. In the current experim ent, random generation of numbers
did not improve perform ance, suggesting that only relatively non-demanding
secondary tasks can promote the use of visuospatial strategies. Another
possibility is that the rhythm created by articulation might have improved
perform ance by encoouraging faster m ovement. However, the advantage was
226       PHILLIPS ET AL.

not found for any of the other secondary tasks, which also required responses at
the same rate. The results suggest that performance on tasks involving spatial
planning can be im proved by chanting one to nine during performance, and it
would be of interest to look in future research at whether this effect extended to
other tasks such as route planning.
    Perhaps instructions requesting preplanning result in verbally rehearsed plans
being made (hence the very short plan tim es during articulatory suppression).
However, during execution, these verbal plans have to be translated into motor
movem ents, and evaluated on-line in relation to a visual display; so visuospatial
memory is likely to be more important during execution of the plans. During
execution, it might be difficult to translate a verbal plan into spatial movem ents,
and to relate the plan to any unexpected interm ediate visual states (for exam ple,
where a plan was inaccurate). Visuospatial on-line planning and evaluation is
therefore likely to be m ore effective.
    Although the purpose of this paper was not to test models of working memory
per se, t h e fin d in g t ha t a verb a l secon d ary t a sk can im pr o ve perfo r m a nce o n
a sp at ia l p la n n in g t a sk h a s so m e im p lica t io n s fo r different m o d els o f
wo rk in g m em o r y. In rela tio n t o t h e Ba dd eley a n d H itch m od el, t his result
ca n b e in t erp reted in t erm s o f p reven t in g verb a l reh ea rsa l in t he a rticu la t o ry
lo o p d u ring p la nn in g, wh ich wo u ld en co u ra ge t h e u se o f a n o p t im a l sp at ia l
stra t egy in vo lvin g t h e visu osp at ia l scrat ch pa d . A lso , m o d els o f work in g
m em o ry t h at p ro p o se in d ep en d en t lim ited -ca p acity verba l a n d visu o sp at ial
reso u rces (Sh a h & M iya k e, 1996), co u ld a cco m m o d a t e t h ese fin d in gs in a
sim ilar m a n n er. H o wever, th o se wh o p u t fo rward a sin gle lim ited -ca p acity
view o f wo rk in g m em o r y (e.g. Sa lth o u se, 1990; Sw an so n , 1996 ; T u rn er &
E n gle, 1989 ) m igh t h a ve d ifficulties in exp la in in g t h ese fin d in gs.
    Analysis of the secon dary task performance suggests that the TOL task was
prioritised to the detrim ent of the secondary tasks. Articulatory suppression,
random generation of num bers, and pattern tapping were all perform ed more
slowly during the latter TOL trials. This might have been due to fatigue,
although if so it is not clear why random tapping was not similarly im peded.
Slowed responses could have been due to general problem s in monitoring and
regulating speed of output, especially because in all of the secondary tasks
response latencies were m ore variable during TOL performance as com pared to
control conditions. The randomness of responses produced in both num ber and
tapping conditions was lower during TOL, although this was not significant for
num ber generation. The secondary task data suggest some degree of general,
perhaps attentional, interference with TOL.
    It is possible that the interference between the secondary tasks and TOL
could have been mostly at a peripheral level, for exam ple in terms of perceptual
or motor response interference rather than related to higher-level memory or
planning . However, findings from another experim ent suggest that this is not the
case. Using a similar method to that outlined here, the effects of dual tasks on a
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                 227

TOL m otor task were investigated. The TOL motor task has been used to
examine the contribution of sensori-motor factors in the TOL in a num ber of
neuropsychological studies (e.g. Owen et al., 1996), and consists of trials
matched in terms of start and goal states to TOL trials, but with each individual
move demonstrated by com puter, so that the participant merely has to copy the
moves made using a lightpen. This task therefore has the same perceptual and
motor demands as the TOL task itself, but without the planning and memory
com ponents. In contrast to the large effects of TOL on secondary task
perform ance reported here (see Fig. 4), carrying out the TOL motor task did not
significantly affect latency or variability of responses in any of the four
secondary tasks. This suggests that the effects of TOL on the secondary tasks is
largely due to the m em ory and planning dem ands of the task.
   It has been proposed that the TOL invok es visuospatial m em ory processes
(Joyce & Robbins, 1991 ; Owen et al., 1990 ; W elsh et al., 1995). A contrary
theory proposes that TOL taps active verbal processes because it is dependent on
the left frontal lobe function (M orris et al., 1993). The current results suggest
that visuospatial m em ory has a stronger role in TOL performance than verbal
memory. Pattern tapping, a task proposed to load visuospatial rehearsal
processes, caused greater interference with TOL perform ance than articulatory
suppression, a task proposed to load verbal rehearsal. It is perhaps worth noting
that the current version of the TOL task, with up to 11 move trials, is likely to be
more demanding of m em ory than the neuropsychological version which tends to
have a maxim um trial length of five m inimum m oves.
   The suggestion that the TOL is dependent on spatial, rather than verbal,
memory processes is not necessarily contrary to findings that: TOL deficits are
more com m on after left than right frontal lobe lesions (Glosser & Goodglass,
1990 ; Shallice, 1982), and brain activation during TOL in a norm al subjects is
highest in the left frontal lobe (M orris et al., 1993; Owen et al., 1996). An
alternative explanation for the neuropsycho logical findings is that the left frontal
lobes are involved in visuospatial memory. Suppor t for this idea can be found in
results reported by Owen et al. (1996). Owen et al. looked at brain activation in
norm al adults during the TOL and a task of visuospatial memory. Both the TOL
task and the visuospatial memory task activated the left frontal lobe, but
com parisons revealed greater left frontal lobe activation during performance of
the visuospatial m emory task as com pared to the TOL. This suggests that much
of the frontal involvem ent of the TOL task may be defined in terms of
visuospatial memory dem ands.
   In conclusion, these results indicate that the Tower of Londo n task does
require executive processing, because both verbal and spatial concurrent
executive tasks proved detrimental to perform ance. However, the nature of the
executive processing in the task may relate m ore to the execution and
monitoring of on-line planning than the ability to form effective preplans of
large sequences of moves. Further, visuospatial rather than verbal m em ory is
228       PHILLIPS ET AL.

involved in the task, as indicated by poorer performance during a concurrent
spatial task and enhanced performance during a concurrent verbal task.

                                                                 Manuscript received 14 January 1998
                                                                   M anuscript accepted 27 July 1998

                                         REFERENCES
Allamanno, N., Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Pasetti, C., & Spinnler, H. (1987). Problem solving
   ability in aging and dementia: Normative data on a non-verbal test. Ita lia n J our nal of
   N eur ological S ciences, 8, 111±120.
Anderson, C.V., Bigler, E.D., & Blatter, D.D . (1995). Frontal lobe lesions, diffuse damage, and
   neuropsychological functioning in traumatic brain-injured patients. J our nal of C linica l a nd
   E xper iment al N europsychology, 17, 900±908.
Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Q ua r terly J our na l of E x per imenta l
   P sychology, 49, 5±28.
Baddeley, A.D . (1986). W or king memory. O xfor d: O xfor d U niver sity P r ess.
Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1974). W orking memory. In G.A . Bower (Ed.), Recent a dva nces in
   lea rning a nd motiva tion (Vol. 8, pp. 647±667). N ew Y o r k : A cad em ic P r ess.
Baddeley, A.D., Lewis, V.J., & Vallar, C. (1984). Explaining the articulatory loop. Q uar terly
   J our nal of E xper imental P sychology, 36, 233±252.
Brandimonte, M .A., & Gerbino, W. (1993). W hen imagery fails: Effects of verbal recoding on
   accessibility of visual memories. In C. Cornoldi, R.H. Logie, M.A. Brandimonte, G. Kaufmann,
   & D. Reisberg (Eds.), S tr etching the ima gina tion: Representations of tr ansfor mations in menta l
   ima gery. (p p . 31±76). N ew Y or k: O xfo rd U niver sity P r ess.
Cohen, G. (1996). M emor y in the r ea l wor ld ( 2nd E dn.) . H ove, U K : P sycho logy P r ess.
Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. (1993). When working memory does not work: The role of working
   memory in neuropsychology. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), H andbook of N eur opsychology
   (Vo l. 8, p p. 9±71). A m st erd a m : E lsevier .
Evans, F.J. (1978). Monitoring attention deployment by random number generation: An index to
   measure subjective randomness. Bulletin of the P sychonomic S ociety, 12, 35±38.
Evans, J.St.B.T., & Brooks, P.G . (1981). Competing with reasoning: A test of the working memory
   hypothesis. Cur r ent P sychologica l R esear ch, 1, 139±147.
Farmer, E.W ., Berman, J.V.F., & Fletcher, Y.L. (1986). Evidence for a visuo-spatial scratch-pad in
   working memory. Q ua r terly J our na l of E x per imental P sychology, 38, 675±688.
Gilhooly, K.J., Logie, R.H., W etherick, N.E., & Wynn, V. (1993). Working memory and strategies
   in syllogistic reasoning tasks. M emor y & C ognition, 21, 115±124.
Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders in executive control functions among aphasic and
   other brain damaged patients. J our na l of C linica l a nd E x per imental N europsychology, 12, 485±
   501.
Goel, V., & Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated in `planning’ functions?
   Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. N eur opsychologia, 33, 623±642.
Grafman, J., Jonas, B., & Salazar, B. (1990). W isconsin Card Sorting Test performance based on
   location and size of neuroanatomical lesion in Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injury.
   P erceptua l and M otor S kills, 71, 1120±1122.
Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. C ognitive S cience, 3,
   275±310.
Hitch, G.J., Brandimonte, M.A., & Walker, P. (1995). Two types of representation in visual
   memory: Evidence from the effects of stimulus contrast on image combination. Memory &
   Cognition , 23 , 147±154.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                          229

Joyce, E.M., & Robbins, T.W . (1991). Frontal lobe function in Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff
    alcoholics: Planning and spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia , 29, 709±723.
Kafer, K.L., & Hunter, M . (1997). On testing the validity of planning/problem-solving tasks in a
    normal population. Journal of the International Neuropsychologica l Society, 3, 108±119.
Logie, R.H., Gilhooly, K.J., & W ynn, V. (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic
    problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 22, 395±410.
Logie, R.H., & M archetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial or central
    executive? In R.H. Logie & M . Denis (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (pp. 105±115).
    Amsterdam: Elsevier.
McCarthy, R.A., & W arrington, E.K. (1990). Cognitive neuropsycholog y. London: Academic Press.
Morice, R., & Delahunty, A. (1996). Frontal/executive impairments in schizophrenia. Schizo-
    phrenia Bulletin, 22 , 125±137.
Morris, R., Miotto, E.C., Feigenbaum, J.D., Bullock, P., & Polkey, C.E. (1997). The effect of goal±
    subgoal conflicts on planning ability after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in humans.
    Neuropsychologia , 35, 1147±1157.
Morris, R.G., Ahmed, S., Syed, G.M., & Toone, B.K. (1993). Neural correlates of planning ability:
    Frontal lobe activation during the Tower of London test. Neuropsychologia , 31 , 1367±1378.
Owen, A.M . (1997). Cognitive planning in humans: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and
    neuropharmacological perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology, 53 , 431±450.
Owen, A.M ., Downes, J.J., Sahakian, B.J., Polkey, C.E., & Robbins, T.W. (1990). Planning and spatial
    working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia , 28, 1021±1034.
Owen, A.M., Doyon, J., Petrides, M ., & Evans, A.C. (1996). Planning and spatial working memory: A
    positron emission tomography study in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 353±364.
Pearson, D.G . (1996). Working memory and strategy use during mental comparison tasks.
    Unpublished PhD, Aberdeen University, Aberdeen.
Pearson, D.G., & Logie, R.H. (1998). Strategy use and working memory during mental size
    comparisons. Manuscript in preparation.
Phillips, L. (1997). Do `frontal tests’ measure executive function? Issues of assessment and
    evidence from fluency tests. In P.M.A . Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive
    function (pp. 191±214). Hove: Psychology Press.
Phillips, L.H., Crawford, J.R., & Chapman, P. (1998). Cognitive control processes in verbal
    fluency . M anuscript submitted for publication.
Rabbit, P.M.A . (1997). Methodology of frontal and executive function. Hove, UK: Psychology
    Press.
Reitan, R.M ., & W olfson, D. (1994). A selective and critical review of neuropsychological deficits
    and the frontal lobes. Neuropsycholog y Review, 4, 161±198.
Salthouse, T.A. (1990). W orking memory as a processing resource in cognitive aging. Develop-
    mental Review, 10, 101±124.
Shah, P., & M iyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking
    and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental
    Psychology , 125 , 4±27.
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
    Society of London B, 298 , 199±209.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsycholog y to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University
    Press.
Sm ith, E.E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive
    Psychology , 33, 5±42.
Swanson, H.L. (1996). Individual and age-related differences in children’ s working memory.
    Memory & Cognition, 24, 70±82.
Temple, C.M ., Carney, R.A., & Mullarkey, S. (1996). Frontal lobe function and executive skills in
    children with Turner’ s syndrome. Developmenta l Neuropsycholog y, 12 , 343±363.
230      PHILLIPS ET AL.

Turner, M.L., & Engle, R.W . (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of
   Memory and Language, 28 , 27±154.
Ward, G., & Allport, A. (1997). Planning and problem-solving using the 5-disc Tower of London
   task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 49±78.
Watts, F.N., M acLeod, A.K., & Morris, L. (1988). Associations between phenomenal and objective
   aspects of concentration problems in depressed patients. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 241±
   250.
Welsh, M., Cicerello, A., Cuneo, R., & Brennan, M. (1995). Error and temporal patterns in Tower
   of Hanoi performance: Cognitive mechanisms and individual differences. Journal of General
   Psychology , 122 , 69±81.
Wynn, V., Phillips, L., Gilhooly, K., Logie, R., & Della Sala, S. (1997). Planning and the Tower of
   London: An analysis of ` think-aloud’ protocols. Paper presented at the British Psychological
   Society Cognitive Section XIV Annual Conference, Bristol, UK.
MEMORY AND THE TOWER OF LONDON TASK                              231

                                             APPENDIX
Information on the Tower of London Trials Used

           Minimum      Indirect     Solution
Trial       moves        moves        paths            Start array of disks      Goal array of disks

A1             3            0            1             m4 m3 m2 m1 r1            r2 l1 l2 m1 r1
A2             5            0            1             l5 l3 l2 l4 l1            m1 r1 m3 m2 r2
A3             5            1            1             r2 m2 m1 l1 r1            l4 l1 l3 l2 r1
A4             7            2            1             m1 m2 lr r1 m3            r3 r1 m2 m1 r2
A5             7            2            2             r1 r3 m1 l2 m2            l2 r1 l1 m1 r2
A6             9            4            5             r2 r1 l2 l1 m1            r1 m1 l1 r2 l2
A7             9            4            3             r1 l1 m2 r2 m1            r2 r4 r5 r3 r1
A8            10            5            1             l1 l2 l4 l5 l3            r3 l1 l1 r2 r1
B1             3            0            1             m2 m1 r1 l1 m3            l3 m1 l4 l1 l2
B2             5            0            1             r1 m1 m2 m3 m4            l2 r2 r1 l3 l1
B3             5            1            1             r3 m1 r2 r1 r4            l2 l1 l4 r1 l3
B4             7            3            2             m1 r3 m2 r1 r2            m1 r3 l1 l2 m2
B5             7            2            1             l2 r3 r1 r2 l1            m1 l1 l2 l3 l4
B6             9            4            1             r2 r1 r4 r5 r3            l1 r4 r2 r3 r1
B7             9            4            3             m4 m2 m1 m3 m5            m1 m2 m3 r1 l1
B8            10            5            1             m5 m1 m3 m4 m2            m1 l2 l1 r2 r1
C1             3            0            1             r2 r1 l2 l1 r3            m2 r1 m3 l1 m1
C2             5            0            1             l2 m3 m2 r1 m1            l2 l1 r1 l3 l4
C3             5            1            1             l1 m3 l2 m1 m2            m3 r1 m2 m1 m4
C4             7            2            1             r4 l1 r3 r2 r1            m2 m1 r3 r4 r1
C5             7            2            2             l3 l2 m1 l1 m2            m1 r2 r3 r4 r1
C6             9            4            4             m1 r1 m3 m2 l1            r1 m1 m3 r2 m2
C7             9            4            3             l2 l3 r1 l1 l4            l2 r2 l1 l3 r1
C8            11            6            4             r2 r4 r3 r5 r1            r3 r1 r5 r2 r4

Trial: letter represents set (A, B, or C), number indicates trial order.
Minimum moves: Minimum number of moves in which a trial can be solved.
Indirect moves: The number of moves in a minimum solution path which does not place a disk into
its target position.
Solution paths: The number of different solution paths with which the trial can be solved in the
minimum moves possible.
Minimum moves, indirect moves and solution path measures were established using a computer
program that calculates minimum move paths for TOL trials.
Positions: Disks are reported in the same order of colours for each trial. Positions refer to peg, then
height. The three pegs are referred to as l (left), m (middle), and r (right). The positions rise from 1 to
5, with 1 at the bottom and 5 at the top.
You can also read