A game of thrones: Neural plasticity in mammalian social hierarchies

Page created by Neil Alvarez
 
CONTINUE READING
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2014
                                Vol. 9, No. 2, 108–117, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.882862

A game of thrones: Neural plasticity in mammalian social
                     hierarchies

       Skyler J. Mooney, Diana E. Peragine, Georgia A. Hathaway, and Melissa M. Holmes

       Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga ON, Canada L5L 1C6

       Social status is a key regulator of health and reproduction in mammals, including humans. Despite this, relatively
       little is known about how social status influences the mammalian brain. Furthermore, the extent to which status is
       an independent construct, i.e., not simply acting as a psychosocial stressor, is yet to be determined. Research to
       date reveals several promising mechanisms and/or systems associated with social status, including monoamine
       systems, hypothalamic neuroendocrine axes, and the hippocampus, though whether these differences are the cause
       or effect of status is often unclear. We review these candidates and propose how best to approach this research
       question in the future.

Keywords: Dominance; Neurogenesis; Plasticity; Social defeat; Social status.

Animals living in groups rarely form a truly egali-                   and, in turn, how status influences the brain is parti-
tarian society. Rather, individuals have differing sta-               cularly challenging, given the diversity and variabil-
tus or rank with the group. This translates to varying                ity of social hierarchies seen across species. For
access to resources where dominant individuals with                   example, the canonical view in behavioral neu-
high status have preferential access over individuals                 roscience is that it is stressful to be socially subor-
of low status, though the resources in question vary                  dinate. Thus, low social rank is a psychosocial
across species (e.g., mates versus food versus terri-                 stressor, translating to physiological stress (e.g., ele-
tories, etc.). These social hierarchies can be rigid                  vated glucocorticoid levels) in subordinate animals.
and stable or remarkably fluid, also depending on                      While this is often true in circumstances with large
species and context. Import antly, social status                      resource inequality or with winner/loser paradigms,
affects physiological and behavioral function in                      it is not the case for many naturally occurring hier-
diverse mammals, including humans, with some-                         archies such as those seen in wolves, lemurs, mar-
times profound effects on direct reproductive fitness                  mosets, and macaques (reviewed in Creel (2001)). In
and overall health.                                                   addition to affecting stress physiology by influen-
   While great strides have been made in under-                       cing the function of the hypothalamic–pituitary–
standing the hormonal and behavioral correlates of                    adrenal (HPA) axis, social status also directly regu-
social status in mammals (e.g., alterations in stress                 lates reproduction. In many species, subordinates
and reproductive function), much less is known                        exhibit dampened activity of the hypothalamic–
about how the central nervous system differs                          pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axis, resulting in lower
between dominant and subordinate animals.                             levels of circulating gonadal steroid hormones
Understanding how the brain controls social status                    (e.g., testosterone) than their dominant counterparts

    Correspondence should be addressed to: Melissa M. Holmes, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga
ON, Canada L5L 1C6. E-mail: melissa.holmes@utoronto.ca
    S.J. Mooney and D.E. Peragine have contributed equally to this manuscript.
    This work was funded by a NSERC Discovery Grant to MMH, Ontario Graduate Scholarships to SJM and DEP, and a NSERC CGS award
to GAH.

                                                     © 2014 Taylor & Francis
SOCIAL STATUS AND NEURAL PLASTICITY           109

(e.g., Carlson et al., 2004; Creel, Creel, Mills, &         dominance relationship is established (Filipenko,
Monfort, 1997; Holmes, Goldman, Goldman,                    Alekseyenko, Beilina, Kamynina, & Kudryavtseva,
Seney, & Forger, 2009).                                     2001; Filipenko, Beilina, Alekseyenko, Dolgov, &
    Here, we explore what is really known about             Kudryavtseva, 2002a, 2002b). In the food competition
how status affects the mammalian brain and attempt          paradigm, social hierarchies are established by com-
to tease apart direct effects of status from poten-         petition for a limited food supply, in either a single
tially indirect effects of stress and/or reproduction.      food hole (Hoshaw, Evans, Mueller, Valentino, &
Are these distinctions even important? We believe           Lucki, 2006) or worker–parasite context (Dhingra,
the answer is an unequivocal “yes”. If we are to            Raju, & Meti, 1997). Despite their popularity, these
truly understand the “social brain”, we need to             paradigms often fail to mirror the natural ecology of
know how the brain navigates and responds to the            the species and consistently result in increased stress
social hierarchies that shape an organism’s day-to-         in the loser or subordinate, as indicated by circulating
day life. However, there is a striking paucity of this      glucocorticoids (e.g., Berton et al., 1998; Keeney
specific issue in the mammalian literature. Status           et al., 2006; Marini et al., 2006; Mitra, Sundlass,
and psychosocial stressor are often used as syno-           Parker, Schatzberg, & Lyons, 2006). Furthermore,
nyms, and this is simply inaccurate in many cases.          much of this work does not directly compare subordi-
Similarly and relatedly, dominance and aggression           nate and dominant animals and, instead, compares
are often used interchangeably and that is also often       subordinates/losers to unmanipulated controls. Thus,
incorrect. The relationships between status, stress,        the social defeat approach is actually studying psy-
and aggression are highly species-specific and con-          chosocial stress and not social status per se.
text-dependent. Based on the current status of the          Unfortunately, this distinction is sometimes lost
field, we are certainly not in a position to make any        when interpreting results. Interpretation is further
sweeping generalizations about how status affects           complicated by differences in the paradigms them-
the brain. Nor are we exhaustively reviewing every          selves, control groups (e.g., single- vs. social-housed),
paper that suggests status-induced plasticity.              length of social exposure, etc., that make it difficult to
Instead, our goal for this Short Review is to               know if effects are due to some other variable (e.g.,
highlight the importance of this research                   environmental complexity). As such, we caution how
question and shed light on how best to tackle it in         these studies are interpreted in the context of social
the future.                                                 status and categorize them as “indirect” evidence.
                                                               Social defeat is accompanied by a depressive-like
                                                            behavioral profile. Therefore, much of the work
  INDIRECT EVIDENCE FOR STATUS                              investigating associated neural plasticity has focused
  EFFECTS ON NEURAL PLASTICITY                              on monoamine systems. For example, serotonin (5-
                                                            HT) 1a receptor numbers and responsivity are
The majority of work investigating social status in         decreased in subordinate rats and tree shrews
traditional laboratory rodents focuses on social defeat,    (Berton et al., 1998; Flügge, 1995; Flügge, Kramer,
including resident–intruder, winner–loser, and food         Rensing, & Fuchs, 1998; but see Berton et al.
competition paradigms. In the resident–intruder             (1999)). Interestingly, pharmacological manipulation
model, a resident/dominant animal selected for size,        of these receptors affects the submissiveness of
aggression, and/or prior winning experience is              rodents and primates during experimental models of
exposed in its home territory to an unfamiliar animal,      social stress (Cooper, McIntyre, & Huhman, 2008;
prompting attack and defeat of the intruder/subordi-        Raleigh, McGuire, Brammer, Pollack, & Yuwiler,
nate (Berton, Aguerre, Sarrieau, Mormede, &                 1991), suggesting a possible bidirectional role.
Chaouloff, 1998, 1999; Gardner, Thrivikraman,               Social defeat has numerous other effects on this sys-
Lightman, Plotsky, & Lowry, 2005; Paul et al.,              tem, such as increases in the activation of serotoner-
2011). This most popular and widely applied defeat          gic neurons (Gardner et al., 2005; Paul et al., 2011),
paradigm is also the most artificial, with dominance         serotonin transporter activity (Filipenko et al., 2002a,
contests “rigged” by the size, experience, and territori-   2002b; Zhang et al., 2012) and 5-HT2a receptor bind-
ality of the resident. By contrast, winner–loser and        ing (Berton et al., 1998). These effects extend to the
food competition paradigms pit “matched” individuals        dopaminergic system where defeat decreases dopa-
against one another. In the winner–loser model, two         mine transporter binding in the brains of mice, rats,
mice share a sensory contact cage, with the partition       and tree shrews (Filipenko et al., 2001; Isovich,
lifted daily to allow regular dominance bouts. After        Engelmann, Landgraf, & Fuchs, 2001; Isovich,
repeated encounters, a victor emerges, and a clear          Mijnster, Flügge, & Fuchs, 2000).
110    MOONEY ET AL.

    Given its robust effects on circulating glucocorti-      Fuchs & Flügge, 2002; Gould et al., 1997; Isovich
coids, it is not surprising that varying periods of social   et al., 2000; Thomas et al., 2007), it can be difficult to
defeat or subordination also alter corticotropin-            determine direction of causality. For example, in
releasing factor receptor expression in the brains of        female bonnet macaque (Macaca radiata) groups,
mice, rats, and tree shrews (Fekete et al., 2009; Fuchs      stress-induced decreases in dominance rank are corre-
& Flügge, 1995; Funk, Li, & Le, 2006; Keeney et al.,         lated with decreased hippocampal neurogenesis
2006; Kozicz, Bordewin, Czéh, Fuchs, & Roubos,               (Perera et al., 2011). Fluoxetine, a selective serotonin
2008; Marini et al., 2006; Panksepp, Burgdorf,               reuptake inhibitor, reverses the effects on rank but not
Beinfeld, Kroes, & Moskal, 2007; Reber et al.,               if animals are irradiated, suggesting that neurogenesis
2007). This is coupled with both short- and long-            is necessary for maintaining dominance (Perera et al.,
term changes in α 2-adrenoceptor, glucocorticoid             2011). However, this pattern is also seen with depres-
receptor and mineralocorticoid receptor expression           sion-scores instead of dominance rank, thus we cannot
(Buwalda et al., 2001; Flügge, 1996; Flügge, Jöhren,         conclude whether it is social rank or depressive beha-
& Fuchs, 1992; Jöhren, Flügge, & Fuchs, 1994;                vior that is directly related to changes in neurogenesis.
Marini et al., 2006; Sutanto et al., 1992). These altera-
tions in stress physiology appear to trigger morpholo-
gical and functional plasticity in the hippocampus.            DIRECT EVIDENCE FOR STATUS
Repeated social defeat and/or subordination results           EFFECTS ON NEURAL PLASTICITY?
in morphological blunting of the apical dendrite in
hippocampal pyramidal neurons of rats and tree               A more direct approach for studying social status
shrews, with corresponding effects on cell excitability      effects on the brain is the study of longer-term, natural
(Kole, Costoli, Koolhaas, & Fuchs, 2004; Magariños,          hierarchies that are formed by colonial or highly
McEwen, Flügge, & Fuchs, 1996). Acute and chronic            social species. Colony housing and caging that more
defeat and/or subordination inhibits both cell prolif-       closely approximate the natural habitat of an animal,
eration and survival in the hippocampus of mice, rats,       such as the visible burrow system (VBS) in laboratory
tree shrews, and monkeys, although effects vary con-         rodents (see Blanchard et al. (1995) for review), may
siderably across experiments (Czéh et al., 2001, 2002,       offer insights into the effects of status that are due to
2006; Ferragud et al., 2010; Gould, McEwen,                  multiple elements of the animal’s natural social
Tanapat, Galea, & Fuchs, 1997; Gould, Tanapat,               ecology.
McEwen, Flügge, & Fuchs, 1998; Hoshaw et al.,                    At first glance, rodents housed in the VBS appear
2006; Mitra et al., 2006; Thomas, Hotsenpiller, &            to exhibit similar changes in the serotonergic and
Peterson, 2007; Van Bokhoven et al., 2011; Yap               dopaminergic systems compared to rodents in social
et al., 2006). Decreases in cell proliferation and survi-    defeat paradigms. For example, mice in the VBS have
val are associated with a decrease in hippocampal            reduced 5-HT1a receptor (McKittrick, Blanchard,
volume (Czéh et al., 2001; Lucassen et al., 2001;            Blanchard, McEwen, & Sakai, 1995) and serotonin
Ohl, Michaelis, Vollmann-Honsdorf, Kirschbaum, &             transporter (McKittrick et al., 2000) binding.
Fuchs, 2000) that is mimicked with cortisol treatment        However, this is true for both dominants and subordi-
(Ohl et al., 2000), suggesting that defeat-induced           nates compared to controls, suggesting that the
effects on the hippocampus are directly due to stress.       decreased 5-HT1a binding in social defeat paradigms
While stress-induced structural changes also occur in        is due to stress and not status. Similarly, subordinate
the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), plasticity related      mice in the VBS have increased 5-HT2 receptor bind-
to defeat and the acquisition of subordinate behavior        ing; dominants exhibited the same pattern though it
does not appear to occur in this region (Markham,            failed to reach statistical significance (McKittrick
Luckett, & Huhman, 2012). Instead, defeat-induced            et al., 1995). As with social defeat, subordinate rats
changes within the mPFC manifest at a functional             in the VBS show decreased dopamine transporter
level, with mPFC inactivation enhancing, and stimu-          binding but only compared to controls, not dominants
lation impairing the acquisition of conditioned defeat       (Lucas et al., 2004). This pattern is also seen with D2
(Markham et al., 2012). Correspondingly, subordinate         receptor binding (Lucas et al., 2004).
mice have lower excitatory synaptic strength in mPFC             Albeit still indirect, the best evidence for social
pyramidal cells than dominants, and dominance beha-          status regulating the serotonergic system comes from
vior can, in turn, be altered by modulating synaptic         rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) living in natural
strength in this region (Wang et al., 2011).                 matrilineal hierarchies where subordinate females
    Precisely because social defeat is associated with a     show increased binding of both the 5-HT1a receptor
depressive-like behavioral profile (Flügge et al., 1998;      in the hypothalamus and of the serotonin transporter
SOCIAL STATUS AND NEURAL PLASTICITY           111

in the orbital prefrontal cortex relative to dominant       for sex might prove difficult because the formation of
females (Embree et al., 2013). For the dopaminergic         stable hierarchies appears to be sex-specific in certain
system, dominant animals show increased D2 receptor         species (Tamashiro et al., 2004). However, when sex
availability and subordinates show decreased dopa-          comparisons can be made, valid status-specific effects
mine transporter availability in cynomolgus monkeys         are more likely to be detected.
(Macaca fascicularis) (Morgan et al., 2002; Nader               In our lab, we use the naked mole-rat
et al., 2012).                                              (Heterocephalus glaber) to study the effects of social
   Importantly, social status in more natural para-         status on brain plasticity. These eusocial rodents exhi-
digms also triggers changes in the HPA axis as well         bit the strictest reproductive and social hierarchies
as hippocampal plasticity. For example, subordinate         among mammals: breeding is restricted to a single
mice in the VBS show enhanced CRF mRNA relative             dominant female, called the queen, and her 1–3 male
to controls in areas that mediate fear/anxiety responses    consorts in colonies that can include hundreds of
such as the central amygdala (Albeck et al., 1997) and      subordinate animals. We have found that social status
the oval nucleus of the bed nucleus of the stria termi-     is more important than sex for determining morphol-
nalis (BNST) (Choi et al., 2006), and reduced hippo-        ogy of several reproductively relevant brain regions
campal glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor        where breeders have larger volumes of the paraven-
mRNA (Chao, Blanchard, Blanchard, McEwen, &                 tricular nucleus (PVN), BNST, and medial amygdala
Sakai, 1993). However, while these effects are not          (MeA) than subordinates (Holmes, Seney, Goldman,
significant in dominant animals, the pattern is often        & Forger, 2011; Holmes et al., 2007). Furthermore,
similar, again suggesting a role for stress and not         subordinates have a higher percentage of androgen
social status. Indeed, as in social defeat, both domi-      receptor-immunoreactive neurons throughout the
nant and subordinate animals housed in the VBS              social decision making network (Holmes, Goldman,
show reductions in apical dendritic arborization within     & Forger, 2008; Holmes et al., 2013) and increased
the hippocampus (McKittrick et al., 2000). Therefore,       numbers of oxytocin-immunoreactive neurons in the
the best example of a stress-independent effect of          PVN (Mooney & Holmes, 2013). However, several
social status on the hippocampus is the increased           variables differ between dominant and subordinate
survival of newborn neurons in dominant rats in the         naked mole-rats including age (breeders are almost
VBS compared to subordinates despite no status dif-         always the oldest animals in the colony), stress, gona-
ferences in glucocorticoids (Kozorovitskiy & Gould,         dal steroid hormones, and behavioral profile. While
2004). However, in this case, we need to be concerned       age does not appear to be a key player when included
about reproduction-induced effects. The VBS is a            as a statistical variable, our efforts to tease apart the
mixed-sex environment, permitting dominant males            effects of stress and reproduction have yielded inter-
to have reproductive experience during the experi-          esting results. By removing subordinates from their
mental paradigm and, indeed, both sexual experience         colony and controlling their reproductive activity (via
and circulating testosterone can increase neurogenesis      gonadal manipulation and/or opposite- versus same-
in adult male rats (Leuner, Glasper, & Gould, 2010;         sex partner housing), we can begin to identify the
Spritzer & Galea, 2007).                                    social and endocrine cues associated with transitions
   This highlights another important point, which is        in social status. For example, removing subordinates
that sex differences and social status have not been        from the colony—a release from subordination—is
sufficiently explored. Social defeat paradigms are           sufficient to trigger plasticity in the PVN (Holmes
almost entirely male-biased, and females are typically      et al., 2011; Mooney & Holmes, 2013), but not the
included in the VBS only as stimuli to facilitate hier-     BNST or MeA, suggesting regional specificity for the
archies in experimental males (Albeck et al., 1997;         underlying mechanisms. Thus far, a role for gonadal
Chao et al., 1993; Choi et al., 2006; Kozorovitskiy &       steroid hormones seems limited given that dominant
Gould, 2004; Lucas et al., 2004; McKittrick et al.,         animals maintain their status and associated neural
1995, 2000). When females are examined, some                morphology following gonadectomy (Goldman,
social-rank correlates such as D2 receptor availability     Forger, & Goldman, 2006; Holmes et al., 2011) and
appear similar in both sexes (Morgan et al., 2002;          that gonads are not required for the transition to a
Nader et al., 2012). However, if we consider that           breeder-like number of oxytocinergic neurons
5-HT1a binding in subordinate female monkeys                (Mooney & Holmes, 2013).
(Embree et al., 2013) differs from that of subordinate          We are also beginning to uncover how social status
male rats and tree shrews (Berton et al., 1998; Flügge,     affects neurogenesis in naked mole-rats. We find that
1995; Flügge et al., 1998), it is difficult to tease apart   breeders have reduced doublecortin-immunoreactive
the effects of species, paradigm, and sex. Accounting       cells in the dentate gyrus, piriform cortex, and
112   MOONEY ET AL.

basolateral amygdala, compared to subordinates, and        activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, occipi-
these effects are more pronounced in females               tal/parietal cortex, ventral striatum, and parahippo-
(Peragine, Simpson, Mooney, & Holmes, 2013).               campal cortex when viewing an individual who is
Interestingly, subordinates that were removed from         superior at a given task. The amygdala and mPFC
their colony and pair-housed were comparable to in-        also show increased activation, but only in unstable
colony subordinates, indicating that release from the      hierarchies.
social hierarchy is not sufficient for breeder-like neu-        In a larger social context, SES encompasses many
rogenesis profiles. These results contrast prior reports    interdependent and diverse factors such as income,
on neurogenesis and social rank in noncooperative          access to health and education, and parental nurtur-
breeders (Ferragud et al., 2010; Hoshaw, Evans,            ance (Hackman, Farah, & Meaney, 2010). Not unlike
Mueller, & Lucki, 2004; Kozorovitskiy & Gould,             animals subjected to repeated social defeat, people of
2004). We are now investigating the underlying neu-        low SES are more likely to experience chronic stress,
roendocrine mechanisms of status-induced changes in        and are therefore at a greater risk of cardiovascular
neurogenesis and, importantly, how they might relate       and metabolic diseases as well as depression and
to status differences in behavior.                         anxiety (McEwen & Gianaros, 2010). Certain compo-
                                                           nents of SES appear to mediate brain structure in
                                                           children. Lawson, Duda, Avants, Wu, and Farah
 SOCIAL HIERARCHIES AND NEURAL                             (2013) found that greater parental education is corre-
      PLASTICITY IN HUMANS                                 lated with greater cortical thickness in the right ante-
                                                           rior cingulate gyrus and left superior frontal gyrus of
Similar to the research on nonhuman animals, emer-         the child. Additionally, lower parental education is
ging evidence suggests that social status in small-        associated with increased amygdala volume and a
group settings (e.g., classrooms and the workplace)        greater income-to-needs ratio correlates with greater
and socioeconomic status (SES) has measurable influ-        hippocampal volume (Noble, Houston, Kan, &
ences on human brain structure and function. In the        Sowell, 2012). Lower childhood SES leads to
context of small groups, bullying might be an analog       decreased adult hippocampal volume (Staff et al.,
to the social defeat paradigms employed in laboratory      2012), which might be consistent with the decreases
animals, with the bully as the dominant individual and     in hippocampal neurogenesis observed in rodents in
the victim as the subordinate (Björkqvist, 2001).          social defeat paradigms and the VBS. However, as
Victims subjected to ongoing incidences of bullying        with the rodent literature, because SES alters stress
experience chronic stress and are more likely to exhi-     profiles in humans, we need to question whether mor-
bit depression, low self-esteem, and other problems        phological differences in the brain are due to stress
(Hawker & Boulton, 2000). These psychosocial pro-          rather than status, per se.
blems appear to mirror the depressive behavior of              SES also mediates activation of several brain areas,
subordinate rodents in social defeat paradigms.            including the left fusiform gyrus and left inferior frontal
Youths with conduct disorders (essentially bullies)        gyrus, areas important for phonological awareness,
show greater activation of the amygdala and ventral        word rhyming, and overall language function (Noble,
striatum and decreased amygdala–prefrontal coupling        Wolmetz, Ochs, Farah, & McCandliss, 2006; Raizada,
compared to healthy controls in response to pictures       Richards, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2008). Similarly, low-SES
of intentionally inflicted pain (Decety, Michalska,         children exhibit decreased prefrontal activity during a
Akitsuki, & Lahey, 2009). However, as bullies tend         novelty task (Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry, &
to be unpopular and disadvantaged (Lagerspetz,             Knight, 2009) and lower-SES adolescents exhibit
Björkqvist, Berts, & King, 1982), the extent to            decreased left frontal activity, which is also associated
which this is a study of social status is questionable.    with the individual’s risk for depression (Tomarken,
In actuality, dominant children use coercive or pro-       Dichter, Garber, & Simien, 2004). The effects of status
social strategies instead of aggression to compete with    on the prefrontal cortex in humans might align with
their peers (Hawley, 1999). Attaining dominance may        rodent studies that demonstrate a role for this region in
therefore be the result of strategic social interactions   the acquisition of conditioned defeat (Markham et al.,
and not of agonistic behavior per se, a possibility that   2012) and the expression of dominance behaviors
has been largely overlooked in both human and non-         (Wang et al., 2011).
human animal literature. Thus, the best evidence for           Interestingly, perceived social status also plays an
status influences in small-group hierarchies comes          important role in neural function and overall health
from a recent report by Zink et al. (2008). In both        (Sapolsky, 2004). For example, lower subjective
stable and unstable hierarchies, people show greater       social status covaries with less gray matter volume
SOCIAL STATUS AND NEURAL PLASTICITY                  113

in the perigenual area of the anterior cingulate cortex   or will not be involved as mechanisms underlying
(Gianaros et al., 2007), and perceiving one’s parents     status-induced plasticity. The hormones and receptors
as low in status is associated with higher amygdala       in these axes have broad, complex, and dynamic
reactivity to threatening facial expressions (Gianaros    effects on brain and behavior beyond the simple defi-
et al., 2008). It is possible that differences in amyg-   nitions of “stress” or “sex.”
dala reactivity might be due to altered activity of the       In regard to human research, creative experimental
HPA axis, as indicated by social defeat studies in        design is critical for answering questions about status.
nonhuman animals (Albeck et al., 1997).                   As with nonhuman animals, it is important to study
                                                          more natural (i.e., preexisting) small-group hierarchies.
                                                          For example, members of a team share a common goal
   WHERE DO WE GO FROM HERE?                              and similar environment but have differing ranks.
                                                          Furthermore, while short-term manipulations of social
In order to understand how social status regulates        status and/or SES are somewhat simple in the labora-
reproductive fitness and health, we need to know           tory setting, long-term changes, though challenging,
whether it is a direct mediator of neural plasticity      need to be investigated. One possibility here would be
beyond its associations with stress and reproduction.     to capitalize on lotteries that have a predetermined
While we believe in the strength of our work with         number of participants or situations where people
naked mole-rats, the best way to test social status       could be assessed pre- and post-promotion, graduation,
effects on mammalian neural plasticity is a compre-       job loss, etc. These are complicated and elaborate para-
hensive, comparative approach. Naked mole-rats have       digms that would likely be heavily confounded but they
many advantages for the study of status but they have     might shed light on how status influences the human
many species-specific adaptations, not the least of        brain in a natural context.
which is their extreme sociality, which might not             In this Short Review, we have focused on a subset
generalize to other mammals. Thus, the single biggest     of the literature, highlighting those areas that have
step forward we can make is to expand social defeat       received the most attention to date and where conver-
paradigms to directly compare dominants and subor-        gence of evidence is strongest. It is clear that we
dinates or winners and losers. Even though these          cannot yet conclude whether social status is a distinct
animals are often intentionally mismatched in age,        construct or the result of numerous other variables that
size, and/or experience, these are powerful compari-      vary in importance between sex and species. While it
sons to make given the built-in control for social        is likely that plasticity in the serotonergic system and
environment; the pairs experience the same events         hippocampus will continue to provide insight into the
but from different status perspectives. Second, we        neural mechanisms of social status, there are numer-
need the work with less traditional species, including    ous other candidates, including the neuropeptides
those that live in natural hierarchies and/or in which    oxytocin and vasopressin, which hold promise. With
status is not linked to circulating glucocorticoids, to   a greater appreciation for the importance of social
include more neural end points. Of course, we under-      status effects on reproductive fitness and health, and
stand this is not easy when working with unique           with a comprehensive and systematic effort, we can
species as we face many of the same challenges in         make great strides in this area in the coming years.
our own efforts. Finally, we need to consider different
subtypes of dominants and subordinates. For example,                       Original manuscript received 5 November 2013
naked mole-rat subordinates can be further classified                         Revised manuscript accepted 8 January 2014
into subcastes with distinct behavioral and physiolo-                               First published online 30 January 2014
gical profiles (Lacey & Sherman, 1991). Furthermore,
a subset of subordinate animals in the VBS shows a
blunted stress response and these subordinate animals
                                                                             REFERENCES
are classified as nonresponders (Lucas et al., 2004;
McKittrick et al., 1995). These animals show altered      Albeck, D. S., McKittrick, C. R., Blanchard, D. C.,
serotonergic and dopaminergic responses compared to          Blanchard, R. J., Nikulina, J., McEwen, B. S., & Sakai,
normal stress-responding subordinates, highlighting          R. R. (1997). Chronic social stress alters levels of corti-
the complex nature of social subordination and inter-        cotropin-releasing factor and arginine vasopressin
actions with the stress response. Collectively, this         mRNA in rat brain. The Journal of Neuroscience,
                                                             17(12), 4895–4903.
approach will identify those brain changes that are       Berton, O., Aguerre, S., Sarrieau, A., Mormede, P., &
more directly attributable to social status. Of course,      Chaouloff, F. (1998). Differential effects of social stress
this is not to say that the HPA and HPG axes cannot          on central serotonergic activity and emotional reactivity
114    MOONEY ET AL.

   in Lewis and spontaneously hypertensive rats.                       aggressive conduct disorder: A functional MRI investi-
   Neuroscience, 82(1), 147–159.                                       gation. Biological Psychology, 80(2), 203–211.
Berton, O., Durand, M., Aguerre, S., Mormede, P., &                Dhingra, N. K., Raju, T. R., & Meti, B. L. (1997). Selective
   Chaouloff, F. (1999). Behavioral, neuroendocrine and                reduction of monoamine oxidase A and B in the frontal
   serotonergic consequences of single social defeat and               cortex of subordinate rats. Brain Research, 758(1–2),
   repeated fluoxetine pretreatment in the Lewis rat strain.            237–240.
   Neuroscience, 92(1), 327–341.                                   Embree, M., Michopoulos, V., Votaw, J. R., Voll, R. J., Mun,
Björkqvist, K. (2001). Social defeat as a stressor in humans.          J., Stehouwer, J. S., . . . Sánchez, M. M. (2013). The
   Physiology & Behavior, 73(3), 435–442.                              relation of developmental changes in brain serotonin
Blanchard, D. C., Spencer, R. L., Weiss, S. M., Blanchard,             transporter (5HTT) and 5HT1A receptor binding to emo-
   R. J., McEwen, B., & Sakai, R. R. (1995). Visible burrow            tional behavior in female rhesus monkeys: Effects of
   system as a model of chronic social stress: Behavioral and          social status and 5HTT genotype. Neuroscience, 228,
   neuroendocrine correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology,                83–100.
   20(2), 117–134.                                                 Fekete, E. M., Zhao, Y., Li, C., Sabino, V., Vale, W. W., &
Buwalda, B., Felszeghy, K., Horváth, K. M., Nyakas, C., de             Zorrilla, E. P. (2009). Social defeat stress activates medial
   Boer, S. F., Bohus, B., & Koolhaas, J. M. (2001).                   amygdala cells that express type 2 corticotropin-releasing
   Temporal and spatial dynamics of corticosteroid receptor            factor receptor mRNA. Neuroscience, 162(1), 5–13.
   down-regulation in rat brain following social defeat.           Ferragud, A., Haro, A., Sylvain, A., Velazquez-Sanchez, C.,
   Physiology & Behavior, 72(3), 349–354.                              Hernandez-Rabaza, V., & Canales, J. J. (2010). Enhanced
Carlson, A. A., Young, A. J., Russell, A. F., Bennett, N. C.,          habit-based learning and decreased neurogenesis in the
   McNeilly, A. S., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2004). Hormonal               adult hippocampus in a murine model of chronic social
   correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta).           stress. Behavioural Brain Research, 210(1), 134–139.
   Hormones and Behavior, 46(2), 141–150.                          Filipenko, M. L., Alekseyenko, O. V., Beilina, A. G.,
Chao, H. M., Blanchard, D. C., Blanchard, R. J., McEwen,               Kamynina, T. P., & Kudryavtseva, N. N. (2001).
   B. S., & Sakai, R. R. (1993). The effect of social stress           Increase of tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine transpor-
   on hippocampal gene expression. Molecular and                       ter mRNA levels in ventral tegmental area of male mice
   Cellular Neuroscience, 4(6), 543–548.                               under influence of repeated aggression experience.
Choi, D. C., Nguyen, M., Tamashiro, K. L., Ma, L. Y.,                  Molecular Brain Research, 96(1), 77–81.
   Sakai, R. R., & Herman, J. P. (2006). Chronic social            Filipenko, M. L., Beilina, A. G., Alekseyenko, O. V.,
   stress in the visible burrow system modulates stress-               Dolgov, V. V., & Kudryavtseva, N. N. (2002a).
   related gene expression in the bed nucleus of the stria             Increase in expression of brain serotonin transporter
   terminalis. Physiology & Behavior, 89(3), 301–310.                  and monoamine oxidase a genes induced by repeated
Cooper, M. A., McIntyre, K. E., & Huhman, K. L.                        experience of social defeats in male mice. Biochemistry
   (2008). Activation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors in the                   (Moscow), 67(4), 451–455.
   dorsal raphe nucleus reduces the behavioral conse-              Filipenko, M. L., Beilina, A. G., Alekseyenko, O. V., Dolgov,
   quences of social defeat. Psychoneuroendocrinology,                 V. V., & Kudryavtseva, N. N. (2002b). Repeated experi-
   33(9), 1236–1247.                                                   ence of social defeats increases serotonin transporter and
Creel, S. (2001). Social dominance and stress hormones.                monoamine oxidase A mRNA levels in raphe nuclei of
   Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 16(9), 491–497.                    male mice. Neuroscience Letters, 321(1), 25–28.
Creel, S., Creel, N. M., Mills, M. G. L., & Monfort, S. L.         Flügge, G. (1995). Dynamics of central nervous 5-HT1A-
   (1997). Rank and reproduction in cooperatively breeding             receptors under psychosocial stress. The Journal of
   African wild dogs: Behavioral and endocrine correlates.             Neuroscience, 15(11), 7132–7140.
   Behavioral Ecology, 8(3), 298–306.                              Flügge, G. (1996). Alterations in the central nervous α
Czéh, B., Michaelis, T., Watanabe, T., Frahm, J., de                   2-adrenoceptor system under chronic psychosocial stress.
   Biurrun, G., van Kampen, M., . . . Fuchs, E. (2001).                Neuroscience, 75(1), 187–196.
   Stress-induced changes in cerebral metabolites, hippo-          Flügge, G., Jöhren, O., & Fuchs, E. (1992). [3H]Rauwolscine
   campal volume, and cell proliferation are prevented by              binding sites in the brains of male tree shrews are related
   antidepressant treatment with tianeptine. Proceedings of            to social status. Brain Research, 597(1), 131–137.
   the National Academy of Sciences, 98(22), 12796–                Flügge, G., Kramer, M., Rensing, S., & Fuchs, E. (1998).
   12801.                                                              5HT1A‐receptors and behaviour under chronic stress:
Czéh, B., Müller-Keuker, J. I., Rygula, R., Abumaria, N.,              Selective counteraction by testosterone. European
   Hiemke, C., Domenici, E., & Fuchs, E. (2006). Chronic               Journal of Neuroscience, 10(8), 2685–2693.
   social stress inhibits cell proliferation in the adult medial   Fuchs, E., & Flügge, G. (1995). Modulation of binding sites
   prefrontal cortex: Hemispheric asymmetry and reversal               for corticotropin-releasing hormone by chronic psycho-
   by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology,                    social stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20(1), 33–51.
   32(7), 1490–1503.                                               Fuchs, E., & Flügge, G. (2002). Social stress in tree shrews:
Czéh, B., Welt, T., Fischer, A. K., Erhardt, A., Schmitt, W.,          Effects on physiology, brain function,and behavior of
   Müller, M. B., . . . Keck, M. E. (2002). Chronic psycho-            subordinate individuals. Pharmacology Biochemistry
   social stress and concomitant repetitive transcranial mag-          and Behavior, 73(1), 247–258.
   netic stimulation: Effects on stress hormone levels and         Funk, D., Li, Z., & Le, A. D. (2006). Effects of environ-
   adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Biological Psychiatry,              mental and pharmacological stressors on c-fos and corti-
   52(11), 1057–1065.                                                  cotropin-releasing factor mRNA in rat brain:
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., Akitsuki, Y., & Lahey, B. B.             Relationship to the reinstatement of alcohol seeking.
   (2009). Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with             Neuroscience, 138(1), 235–243.
SOCIAL STATUS AND NEURAL PLASTICITY                115

Gardner, K. L., Thrivikraman, K. V., Lightman, S. L.,                in social competition. Behavioural Pharmacology, 15
   Plotsky, P. M., & Lowry, C. A. (2005). Early life experi-         (5–6), A17.
   ence alters behavior during social defeat: Focus on ser-       Hoshaw, B. A., Evans, J. C., Mueller, B., Valentino, R. J., &
   otonergic systems. Neuroscience, 136(1), 181–191.                 Lucki, I. (2006). Social competition in rats: Cell prolif-
Gianaros, P. J., Horenstein, J. A., Cohen, S., Matthews,             eration and behavior. Behavioural Brain Research,
   K. A., Brown, S. M., Flory, J. D., . . . Hariri, A. R.            175(2), 343–351.
   (2007). Perigenual anterior cingulate morphology cov-          Isovich, E., Engelmann, M., Landgraf, R., & Fuchs, E.
   aries with perceived social standing. Social Cognitive            (2001). Social isolation after a single defeat reduces
   and Affective Neuroscience, 2(3), 161–173.                        striatal dopamine transporter binding in rats. European
Gianaros, P. J., Horenstein, J. A., Hariri, A. R., Sheu, L. K.,      Journal of Neuroscience, 13(6), 1254–1256.
   Manuck, S. B., Matthews, K. A., & Cohen, S. (2008).            Isovich, E., Mijnster, M. J., Flügge, G., & Fuchs, E. (2000).
   Potential neural embedding of parental social standing.           Chronic psychosocial stress reduces the density of dopa-
   Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 91–96.         mine transporters. European Journal of Neuroscience,
Goldman, S. L., Forger, N. G., & Goldman, B. D. (2006).              12(3), 1071–1078.
   Influence of gonadal sex hormones on behavioral com-            Jöhren, O., Flügge, G., & Fuchs, E. (1994). Hippocampal
   ponents of the reproductive hierarchy in naked mole-rats.         glucocorticoid receptor expression in the tree shrew:
   Hormones and Behavior, 50(1), 77–84.                              Regulation by psychosocial conflict. Cellular and
Gould, E., McEwen, B. S., Tanapat, P., Galea, L. A., &               Molecular Neurobiology, 14(3), 281–296.
   Fuchs, E. (1997). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of         Keeney, A., Jessop, D. S., Harbuz, M. S., Marsden, C. A.,
   the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress          Hogg, S., & Blackburn‐Munro, R. E. (2006). Differential
   and NMDA receptor activation. The Journal of                      effects of acute and chronic social defeat stress on
   Neuroscience, 17(7), 2492–2498.                                   hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function and hippo-
Gould, E., Tanapat, P., McEwen, B. S., Flügge, G., & Fuchs,          campal serotonin release in mice. Journal of
   E. (1998). Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the        Neuroendocrinology, 18(5), 330–338.
   dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress.        Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry,
   Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(6),           L. M., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities
   3168–3171.                                                        affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of
Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010).                Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(6), 1106–1115.
   Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights       Kole, M. H. P., Costoli, T., Koolhaas, J. M., & Fuchs, E.
   from human and animal research. Nature Reviews                    (2004). Bi-directional shift in the Cornu Ammonis 3
   Neuroscience, 11(9), 651–659.                                     pyramidal dendritic organization following brief stress.
Hawker, D. S., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years’                Neuroscience, 125, 337–347.
   research on peer victimization and psychosocial malad-         Kozicz, T., Bordewin, L. A. P., Czéh, B., Fuchs, E., &
   justment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional stu-          Roubos, E. W. (2008). Chronic psychosocial stress
   dies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4),          affects corticotropin-releasing factor in the paraventricu-
   441–455.                                                          lar nucleus and central extended amygdala as well as
Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A         urocortin 1 in the non-preganglionic Edinger-Westphal
   strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Developmental            nucleus of the tree shrew. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
   Review, 19(1), 97–132.                                            33(6), 741–754.
Holmes, M. M., Goldman, B. D., & Forger, N. G. (2008).            Kozorovitskiy, Y., & Gould, E. (2004). Dominance hierar-
   Social status and sex independently influence androgen             chy influences adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus.
   receptor expression in the eusocial naked mole-rat brain.         The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(30), 6755–6759.
   Hormones and Behavior, 54(2), 278–285.                         Lacey, E. A., & Sherman, P. W. (1991). Social organization of
Holmes, M. M., Goldman, B. D., Goldman, S. L., Seney,                naked mole-rat colonies: Evidence for divisions of labor. In
   M. L., & Forger, N. G. (2009). Neuroendocrinology and             P. W. Sherman, J. U. M. Jarvis, & R. D. Alexander (Eds.),
   sexual differentiation in eusocial mammals. Frontiers in          The Biology of the Naked Mole-Rat (pp. 275–336).
   Neuroendocrinology, 30(4), 519–533.                               Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Holmes, M. M., Rosen, G. J., Jordan, C. L., de Vries,             Lagerspetz, K. M., Björkqvist, K., Berts, M., & King, E.
   G. J., Goldman, B. D., & Forger, N. G. (2007). Social             (1982). Group aggression among school children in
   control of brain morphology in a eusocial mammal.                 three schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
   Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,                  23(1), 45–52.
   104(25), 10548–10552.                                          Lawson, G. M., Duda, J. T., Avants, B. B., Wu, J., & Farah,
Holmes, M. M., Seney, M. L., Goldman, B. D., & Forger,               M. J. (2013). Associations between children’s socioeco-
   N. G. (2011). Social and hormonal triggers of neural              nomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness.
   plasticity in naked mole-rats. Behavioural Brain                  Developmental Science, 16(5), 641–652.
   Research, 218(1), 234–239.                                     Leuner, B., Glasper, E. R., & Gould, E. (2010). Sexual
Holmes, M. M., Van Mil, S., Bulkowski, C., Goldman,                  experience promotes adult neurogenesis in the hippo-
   S. L., Goldman, B. D., & Forger, N. G. (2013).                    campus despite an initial elevation in stress hormones.
   Androgen receptor distribution in the social decision-            PLoS One, 5(7), e11597.
   making network of eusocial naked mole-rats.                    Lucas, L. R., Celen, Z., Tamashiro, K. L. K., Blanchard,
   Behavioural Brain Research, 256, 214–218.                         R. J., Blanchard, D. C., Markham, C., . . . McEwen, B. S.
Hoshaw, B. A., Evans, J. C., Mueller, B., & Lucki, I.                (2004). Repeated exposure to social stress has long-term
   (2004). P31 Reduced neurogenesis by subordinate rats              effects on indirect markers of dopaminergic activity in
116    MOONEY ET AL.

   brain regions associated with motivated behavior.                 A pilot-study in tree shrews. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
   Neuroscience, 124(2), 449–457.                                    25(4), 357–363.
Lucassen, P. J., Vollmann‐Honsdorf, G. K., Gleisberg, M.,         Panksepp, J., Burgdorf, J., Beinfeld, M. C., Kroes, R. A., &
   Czéh, B., De Kloet, E. R., & Fuchs, E. (2001). Chronic            Moskal, J. R. (2007). Brain regional neuropeptide
   psychosocial stress differentially affects apoptosis in hip-      changes resulting from social defeat. Behavioral
   pocampal subregions and cortex of the adult tree shrew.           Neuroscience, 121(6), 1364–1371.
   European Journal of Neuroscience, 14(1), 161–166.              Paul, E. D., Hale, M. W., Lukkes, J. L., Valentine, M. J.,
Magariños, A. M., McEwen, B. S., Flügge, G., & Fuchs, E.             Sarchet, D. M., & Lowry, C. A. (2011). Repeated social
   (1996). Chronic psychosocial stress causes apical den-            defeat increases reactive emotional coping behavior and
   dritic atrophy of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons in            alters functional responses in serotonergic neurons in the
   subordinate tree shrews. The Journal of Neuroscience,             rat dorsal raphe nucleus. Physiology & Behavior, 104(2),
   16(10), 3534–3540.                                                272–282.
Marini, F., Pozzato, C., Andreetta, V., Jansson, B., Arban,       Peragine, D. E., Simpson, J., Mooney, S. J., & Holmes,
   R., Domenici, E., & Carboni, L. (2006). Single exposure           M. M. (2013). Social control of adult neurogenesis in
   to social defeat increases corticotropin-releasing factor         the piriform cortex of naked mole-rats. Society for
   and glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in rat                Neuroscience abstracts, 655.21.
   hippocampus. Brain Research, 1067(1), 25–35.                   Perera, T. D., Dwork, A. J., Keegan, K. A.,
Markham, C. M., Luckett, C. A., & Huhman, K. L. (2012).              Thirumangalakudi, L., Lipira, C. M., Joyce, N., . . .
   The medial prefrontal cortex is both necessary and suffi-          Coplan, J. D. (2011). Necessity of hippocampal neuro-
   cient for the acquisition of conditioned defeat.                  genesis for the therapeutic action of antidepressants in
   Neuropharmacology, 62(2), 933–939.                                adult nonhuman primates. PloS One, 6(4), e17600.
McEwen, B. S., & Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the      Raizada, R. D., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P. K.
   brain in stress and adaptation: Links to socioeconomic            (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric spe-
   status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York               cialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young
   Academy of Sciences, 1186(1), 190–222.                            children. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1392–1401.
McKittrick, C. R., Blanchard, D. C., Blanchard, R. J.,            Raleigh, M. J., McGuire, M. T., Brammer, G. L., Pollack,
   McEwen, B. S., & Sakai, R. R. (1995). Serotonin recep-            D. B., & Yuwiler, A. (1991). Serotonergic mechanisms
   tor binding in a colony model of chronic social stress.           promote dominance acquisition in adult male vervet
   Biological Psychiatry, 37(6), 383–393.                            monkeys. Brain Research, 559(2), 181–190.
McKittrick, C. R., Magariños, A. M., Blanchard, D. C.,            Reber, S. O., Birkeneder, L., Veenema, A. H., Obermeier, F.,
   Blanchard, R. J., McEwen, B. S., & Sakai, R. R.                   Falk, W., Straub, R. H., & Neumann, I. D. (2007). Adrenal
   (2000). Chronic social stress reduces dendritic arbors in         insufficiency and colonic inflammation after a novel chronic
   CA3 of hippocampus and decreases binding to serotonin             psycho-social stress paradigm in mice: Implications and
   transporter sites. Synapse, 36(2), 85–94.                         mechanisms. Endocrinology, 148(2), 670–682.
Mitra, R., Sundlass, K., Parker, K. J., Schatzberg, A. F., &      Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Social status and health in humans
   Lyons, D. M. (2006). Social stress-related behavior               and other animals. Annual Review of Anthropology, 33,
   affects hippocampal cell proliferation in mice.                   393–418.
   Physiology & Behavior, 89(2), 123–127.                         Spritzer, M. D., & Galea, L. A. (2007). Testosterone and
Mooney, S. J., & Holmes, M. M. (2013). Social condition              dihydrotestosterone, but not estradiol, enhance survival
   and oxytocin neuron number in the hypothalamus of                 of new hippocampal neurons in adult male rats.
   naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber). Neuroscience,            Developmental Neurobiology, 67(10), 1321–1333.
   230, 56–61.                                                    Staff, R. T., Murray, A. D., Ahearn, T. S., Mustafa, N., Fox,
Morgan, D., Grant, K. A., Gage, H. D., Mach, R. H.,                  H. C., & Whalley, L. J. (2012). Childhood socioeco-
   Kaplan, J. R., Prioleau, O., . . . Nader, M. A. (2002).           nomic status and adult brain size: Childhood socioeco-
   Social dominance in monkeys: Dopamine D2 receptors                nomic status influences adult hippocampal size. Annals
   and cocaine self-administration. Nature Neuroscience,             of Neurology, 71(5), 653–660.
   5(2), 169–174.                                                 Sutanto, W., Oitzl, M. S., Rots, N. Y., Schöbitz, B., Van den
Nader, M. A., Nader, S. H., Czoty, P. W., Riddick, N. V.,            Berg, D. T., Van Dijken, H. H., . . . Koolhaas, J. M.
   Gage, H. D., Gould, R. W., . . . Reboussin, B. A. (2012).         (1992). Corticosteroid receptor plasticity in the central
   Social dominance in female monkeys: Dopamine recep-               nervous system of various rat models. Endocrine
   tor function and cocaine reinforcement. Biological psy-           Regulations, 26(3), 111–118.
   chiatry, 72(5), 414–421.                                       Tamashiro, K. L., Nguyen, M., Fujikawa, T., Xu, T., Yun
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., & Sowell, E. R.               Ma, L., Woods, S. C., & Sakai, R. R. (2004).
   (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the          Metabolic and endocrine consequences of social stress
   developing human brain. Developmental Science, 15(4),             in a visible burrow system. Physiology & Behavior,
   516–527.                                                          80(5), 683–693.
Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., &        Thomas, R. M., Hotsenpiller, G., & Peterson, D. A. (2007).
   McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Brain–behavior relationships            Acute psychosocial stress reduces cell survival in adult
   in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic             hippocampal neurogenesis without altering proliferation.
   factors. Developmental Science, 9(6), 642–654.                    The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(11), 2734–2743.
Ohl, F., Michaelis, T., Vollmann-Honsdorf, G. K.,                 Tomarken, A. J., Dichter, G. S., Garber, J., & Simien, C.
   Kirschbaum, C., & Fuchs, E. (2000). Effect of chronic             (2004). Resting frontal brain activity: Linkages to mater-
   psychosocial stress and long-term cortisol treatment on           nal depression and socio-economic status among adoles-
   hippocampus-mediated memory and hippocampal volume:               cents. Biological Psychology, 67(1), 77–102.
SOCIAL STATUS AND NEURAL PLASTICITY                117

Van Bokhoven, P., Oomen, C. A., Hoogendijk, W. J. G.,             social defeat episodes in mice: Effects on cell prolifera-
   Smit, A. B., Lucassen, P. J., & Spijker, S. (2011).            tion in the dentate gyrus. Behavioural Brain Research,
   Reduction in hippocampal neurogenesis after social             172(2), 344–350.
   defeat is long‐lasting and responsive to late antidepres-   Zhang, J., Fan, Y., Li, Y., Zhu, H., Wang, L., & Zhu, M. Y.
   sant treatment. European Journal of Neuroscience,              (2012). Chronic social defeat up‐regulates expression of
   33(10), 1833–1840.                                             the serotonin transporter in rat dorsal raphe nucleus and
Wang, F., Zhu, J., Zhu, H., Zhang, Q., Lin, Z., & Hu, H.          projection regions in a glucocorticoid‐dependent manner.
   (2011). Bidirectional control of social hierarchy by           Journal of Neurochemistry, 123(6), 1054–1068.
   synaptic efficacy in medial prefrontal cortex. Science,      Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., Bassett, D. S., Stein, J. L.,
   334(6056), 693–697.                                            & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2008). Know your place:
Yap, J. J., Takase, L. F., Kockman, L. J., Fornal, C. A.,         Neural processing of social hierarchy in humans.
   Miczek, K. A., & Jacobs, B. L. (2006). Repeated brief          Neuron, 58(2), 273–283.
You can also read