Vowel Length Expands Perceptual and Emotional Evaluations in Written Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words

Page created by Evelyn Sherman
 
CONTINUE READING
behavioral
              sciences
Article
Vowel Length Expands Perceptual and Emotional Evaluations
in Written Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words
Zihan Lin † , Nan Wang † , Yan Yan and Toshimune Kambara *

                                          Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Hiroshima 7398524, Japan;
                                          zihanlin51@gmail.com (Z.L.); 1151608137wn@gmail.com (N.W.); yanyan201904@gmail.com (Y.Y.)
                                          * Correspondence: tkambara@hiroshima-u.ac.jp; Tel.: +81-82-424-6280
                                          † These authors equally contributed to this study.

                                          Abstract: In this study, we examined whether vowel length affected the perceptual and emotional
                                          evaluations of Japanese sound-symbolic words. The perceptual and emotional features of Japanese
                                          sound-symbolic words, which included short and long vowels, were evaluated by 209 native Japanese
                                          speakers. The results showed that subjective evaluations of familiarity, visual imageability, auditory
                                          imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, arousal, and length were significantly higher
                                          for sound-symbolic words with long vowels compared to those with short vowels. Additionally,
                                          a subjective evaluation of speed was significantly higher for written Japanese sound-symbolic words
                                          with short vowels than for those with long vowels. The current findings suggest that vowel length
                                          in written Japanese sound-symbolic words increases the perceptually and emotionally subjective
                                          evaluations of Japanese sound-symbolic words.

                                          Keywords: Japanese; sound symbolism; vowel length; semantic differential scale; written sound-
                                          symbolic words; perceptual and emotional evaluations
         
         

Citation: Lin, Z.; Wang, N.; Yan, Y.;
Kambara, T. Vowel Length Expands          1. Introduction
Perceptual and Emotional
                                                In sound symbolism, linguistic features non-arbitrarily associate with perceptual or
Evaluations in Written Japanese
                                          emotional features [1–5]. For example, a pseudoword including a vowel “a” is associated
Sound-Symbolic Words. Behav. Sci.
                                          with a bigger figure than a pseudoword including a vowel “i” [5]. Previous studies of the
2021, 11, 90. https://doi.org/
                                          bouba-kiki effect showed that some pseudowords (e.g., bouba) were linked with round fig-
10.3390/bs11060090
                                          ures, whereas other pseudowords (e.g., kiki) were linked with spiky figures [6,7]. One study
                                          reported that auditory features of spoken pseudowords cross-modal associated with visual
Received: 28 April 2021
Accepted: 16 June 2021
                                          features of figures [8]. Linguistic features (specific consonants and vowels) can increase
Published: 21 June 2021
                                          the expected gustatory features [9]. Emotional features are also connected to linguistic
                                          information [10]. In addition to sound-symbolic associations between pseudowords (new
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
                                          words) and referents, real words can be sound symbolically associated with perceptual or
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
                                          abstract features [11,12]. Previous findings suggested that the oral shapes to pronounce
published maps and institutional affil-   linguistic features were associated with perceptual and emotional features [5,13], whereas
iations.                                  written linguistic features (alphabetic letters and other characters) were also associated
                                          with them [14–16]. Namba and Kambara (2020) reported that the oral shapes to produce
                                          Japanese vowels were connected to specific perceptual and emotional evaluations (e.g.,
                                          some oral shapes were perceived as bigger, wider, and higher than oral shapes used to
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
                                          produce other vowels) [13]. In addition, Cuskley et al. (2017) showed that pseudowords
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
                                          with round letters (e.g., gege) were associated with round figures, while those with spiky
This article is an open access article
                                          letters (e.g., zeze) were associated with spiky figures [15]. Based on these previous findings,
distributed under the terms and           combinations of spoken and written linguistic features could affect perceptual and emo-
conditions of the Creative Commons        tional sensitivity. Participants learned the sound-symbolic relationships between linguistic
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://     features and perceptual features in a first or second language ([17–20]; see for review [21]),
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/          and also learned non-sound-symbolic (arbitrary) relationships between linguistic features
4.0/).                                    and perceptual features in a first or second language [22–26]. For example, event-related

Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11060090                                              https://www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                             2 of 11

                           brain negativity occurred around 400 ms when infants detected mismatched pairs of linguis-
                           tic features and figures, compared to matched pairs of linguistic features and figures [17].
                           Sound-symbolic relationships could occur from already acquired associations between
                           linguistic and referential features in native languages [27]. Other studies also reported
                           that linguistic information was specifically associated with perceptually and emotionally
                           subjective evaluations by using semantic differential scales [14,28–30]. For example, partic-
                           ipants differentially evaluated familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile
                           imageability, emotional valence (preference), and arousal (excitement) of sound-symbolic
                           words including voiced consonants and sound-symbolic words including semi-voiced or
                           unvoiced consonants [28]. These findings of sound symbolism suggested that linguistic
                           information (written and spoken information) was non-arbitrarily associated with specific
                           perceptual and emotional features.
                                 Previous studies have examined specific associations between perceptual and emo-
                           tional features of words or pseudowords that included a specific vowel [14,31–33]. The vow-
                           els of the low second formant (F2) are a, u, and o, whereas vowels of the high F2 are i
                           and e [31,33,34]. Vowels with low F2 are back vowels, whereas vowels with high F2 are
                           front vowels [31]. The front vowels are associated with smaller features than the back
                           vowels [3,5,35,36]. Such associations between acoustic frequency and referential size are
                           called frequency codes [33]. Klink (2000) reported that products associated with names
                           which included front vowels were evaluated as lighter (relative to both darker and heavier),
                           smaller, more feminine, colder, faster, softer, thinner, friendlier, weaker, prettier, more
                           bitter, and milder than products associated with names which included back vowels [36].
                           Psycholinguistic research showed that a long distance between objects promoted speech
                           production of u and æ, whereas a short distance between objects promoted speech produc-
                           tion of i [37]. As another perspective of sound symbolism of vowels (vowel symbolism [32]),
                           Hamano theoretically showed that vowel length was associated with temporal and spatial
                           length of actions (i.e., speed and length [32]). Based on the vowel length, we can categorize
                           sound-symbolic words, including long vowel(s) and sound-symbolic words, including
                           short vowel(s). Vowel length in speech can be detected by infants [38,39], although the
                           learning of vowel length consists of many factors that affect vowel length (e.g., vowel
                           height [40]). In addition, another study suggested that 8- to 10-month-old infants preferred
                           bi-syllabic heavy to light words which included stimuli with long vowels [41]. Although
                           few psycholinguistic studies have examined the effects of vowel length in Japanese sound-
                           symbolic words, a German psycholinguistic study conducted by Bross [42] reported that
                           German pseudowords which included a long vowel (e.g., mutoh) were associated with long
                           or large objects, while German pseudowords, which included short vowels (e.g., mutto),
                           were associated with short or small objects. The author also showed that the associative
                           relations between vowel length and object length were strong connections, whereas the
                           associative relations between vowel length and object size were weak connections [42].
                           Although previous theoretical and experimental studies have shown that vowel length
                           was associated with time (i.e., speed) and distance (i.e., length [32]) or size [42], vowel
                           length would be also associated with other perceptual features (e.g., visual imageability,
                           auditory imageability, and tactile imageability) and emotional features (e.g., familiarity,
                           emotional valence, and arousal) based on previous psycholinguistic findings of vowel
                           symbolism [14,36].
                                 The aim of the current study is to investigate the effects of vowel length in writ-
                           ten Japanese sound-symbolic words. In this survey, participants evaluated each written
                           Japanese sound-symbolic word using eight semantic differential scales (familiarity, visual
                           imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, arousal, length,
                           and speed) on a 5-point semantic differential scale from 1 to 5 (e.g., familiarity: 1: unfa-
                           miliar; 5: familiar). We selected six semantic differential scales from a behavioral study
                           that examined written Japanese sound-symbolic words [28]. We also selected two semantic
                           differential scales (length and speed) based on a theoretical study of Japanese sound symbol-
                           ism [32] and an experiment that clarified the relationship between vowel length and object
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                            3 of 11

                           length [42]. The current study extended the previous studies by using selected semantic
                           differential scales. A previous study successfully compared 10 sound-symbolic words
                           which included two voiced consonants (e.g., ダラダラ in Japanese, daradara) with 10 sound-
                           symbolic words which included two unvoiced consonants (e.g., タラタラ in Japanese,
                           taratara) in order to identify sound-symbolic effects of voiced consonants [28]. Therefore,
                           we compared 10 sound-symbolic words with two long vowels (e.g., ブーブー in Japanese,
                           buubuu) with those with two short vowels (e.g., ブブ in Japanese, bubu) in order to identify
                           sound-symbolic effects of vowel length using the successful methods of the previous study.
                           The stimuli were written Japanese sound-symbolic words. Psycholinguistic studies have
                           focused not only on the sound-symbolic phenomenon in European languages [3,5] but
                           also on non-European languages, including the Japanese language [14,28]. The Japanese
                           language also includes numerous sound-symbolic words [32]. For instance, some dic-
                           tionaries only list Japanese sound-symbolic words [43]. In this study, the sample was
                           native speakers of Japanese to assess Japanese native speakers’ evaluations of Japanese
                           sound-symbolic words. Since previous studies on sound symbolism have focused on
                           young samples’ evaluations of vowels and sound-symbolic words [14,28], we collected
                           data from a wider range of samples from a crowdsourcing company and Google Forms
                           rather than samples of previous research in order to control for age effects on sound-
                           symbolic judgments. In addition, we pseudorandomized the presentation order of items
                           between participants to control the presentation order effects of stimuli [44]. We predicted
                           that written Japanese sound-symbolic words with long vowels would be perceived as
                           spatially longer and temporally slower than those with short vowels. This prediction was
                           consistent with previous theoretical and experimental findings [32,42]. A theoretical study
                           showed that Japanese sound-symbolic words with short vowels showed events which
                           were completed instantaneously (temporally speedy) and in a short distance (spatially
                           short), whereas Japanese sound-symbolic words with long vowels showed events that
                           took a long time (temporally slow) and space (spatially long) [32]. An experiment also
                           reported that vowel length was spatially associated with object length [42]. In addition,
                           we also predicted that the vowel length in written Japanese sound-symbolic words affects
                           familiarity, multisensory imageabilities, emotional valence, and arousal. A previous study
                           showed that each written Japanese vowel (a, i, u, e, and o) could be individually associated
                           with specific multisensory features including physical aspects and emotional features
                           including familiarity, emotional valence, and arousal [14]. Although the previous findings
                           did not directly examine effects of vowel length to familiarity, multisensory imageabilities,
                           emotional valence, and arousal, the previous results suggest that a written Japanese vowel
                           itself could be differentially connected to familiarity, multisensory imageabilities, emotional
                           valence, and arousal [14].

                           2. Materials and Methods
                           2.1. Participants
                                A total of 209 Japanese native speakers (115 females; Mage = 39.42; SDage = 9.25;
                           age range = 20–70 years) participated in this survey research. These participants were
                           recruited from a crowd-sourcing company (Crowdworks, Inc., Tokyo, Japan). Each partic-
                           ipant received 220 Japanese yen (JPY) as a monetary reward for their participation after
                           completion of the survey. After each participant read and consented to the written expla-
                           nation of the study, they answered the questionnaire. This research was approved by the
                           ethical committee of the Graduate School of Humanities and Social Sciences at Hiroshima
                           University (approval code: 2020001).

                           2.2. Materials
                                This study consisted of 20 Japanese sound-symbolic words (Appendix A). The sound-
                           symbolic words were selected from a Japanese sound-symbolic dictionary [43], whereas two
                           written Japanese sound-symbolic words (hyohyo and nyanya) checked by a native speaker
                           of Japanese (the last author) were added. Based on a previous study that examined sound-
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                            4 of 11

                           symbolic effects of consonants by comparing 10 pairs of sound-symbolic words which
                           included two voiced consonants (e.g., ダラダラ in Japanese, daradara) with sound-symbolic
                           words which included two unvoiced consonants (e.g., タラタラ in Japanese, taratara) [28],
                           we collected 10 pairs of sound-symbolic words which included two long vowels (e.g., ブー
                           ブー in Japanese, buubuu) and sound-symbolic words which included two short vowels
                           (e.g., ブブ in Japanese, bubu). These written sound-symbolic words were shown in Japanese
                           katakana moraic script, which is one of the traditional scripts of Japanese, alongside the
                           morphographic kanji and moraic hiragana scripts [45,46]. The Roman script was also
                           used in certain circumstances [45,46]. Katakana moraic script was used for loanwords
                           (foreign words) and native words, which included sound-symbolic words, whereas kanji
                           and hiragana scripts were used mainly for native words. A katakana symbol generally
                           represented a pair of a consonant or a palatalized consonant and vowel or a vowel itself
                           (e.g., a pair of a consonant and vowel: ブ in Japanese, bu; a vowel itself: ア in Japanese,
                           a). Palatalization was represented in the orthography by attaching small ya (ヤ), yu (ユ),
                           and yo (ヨ) to the preceding katakana or hiragana symbol (e.g., チュ in Japanese, chu [46]).
                           Each long vowel of a pair of a consonant and vowel or a vowel itself was shown as ー
                           (dash, chōonpu in Japanese; [46]) attached to a katakana or hiragana symbol in the Japanese
                           writing system (e.g., ブー in Japanese, buu; [46]).

                           2.3. Procedures
                                 We used Google Forms to conduct the survey. Before the survey, all participants read
                           an explanation about the task. Participants were asked “Please evaluate presented ono-
                           matopoeic words by using eight evaluative items of five levels from one to five. The eight
                           evaluative items of each onomatopoeic word consist of familiarity (1, unfamiliar; 5, fa-
                           miliar), visual imageability (1: not visually imageable, 5: visually imageable), auditory
                           imageability (1: not auditorily imageable, 5: auditorily imageable), tactile imageability
                           (1: not tactilely imageable, 5: tactilely imageable), emotional valence (1: dislike; 5: like),
                           arousal (1: calm; 5: excited), length (1: short; 5: long), and speed (1: slow; 5: fast). Please
                           choose the most appropriate number from 1 to 5 for each evaluative item.” The scales of the
                           evaluative items were semantic differential scales [30]. These semantic differential scales
                           were also based on theoretical linguistic and psycholinguistic research [13,14,28,32,47].
                           To control the order of the presentation of the stimuli, the order of the evaluative items
                           was pseudorandomized between participants by preparing two lists of items to evaluate.
                           We previously wrote a necessity to control the presentation order of written stimuli in
                           surveys as a future direction of a previous article [14].
                                 We used linear mixed-effects models to include participants and paired items as
                           random effects, vowel length (0: SV; 1: LV) as fixed effects, and subjective evaluations
                           of 5-point semantic differential scales (familiarity, visual imageability, auditory image-
                           ability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, arousal, length, and speed) as dependent
                           variables [24,28,48]. Based on the linear mixed-effects models, we clarified the differences
                           between the perceptual and emotional evaluations of LV and SV.

                           3. Results
                                 Descriptive statistics were analyzed with the means and standard deviations. Cron-
                           bach’s alphas were calculated to assess reliabilities of semantic differential scales and
                           a linear mixed-effects model analysis was conducted to examine the differences between
                           subjective evaluations of LV and SV. First, we calculated means and standard deviations
                           by using R [49] and a psych package [50] to assess the means and standard deviations
                           of all words, LV, SV, and each written Japanese sound-symbolic word. We also checked
                           whether the calculated means and standard deviations of R were consistent using Microsoft
                           Excel and the statistical software SPSS. The means and standard deviations of subjective
                           evaluations of all written Japanese sound-symbolic words, LV, and SV are shown in Table 1,
                           whereas the means and standard deviations of each written Japanese sound-symbolic word
                           are shown in Table S1.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                                     5 of 11

                           Table 1. Descriptive statistics for written Japanese sound-symbolic words including long vowels (LV)
                           and short vowels (SV).

                                                                             ALL                       LV                          SV
                                Subjective Evaluations
                                                                       M            SD           M            SD           M              SD
                                      Familiarity                     2.97         1.29         3.18         1.28         2.76           1.26
                                  Visual imageability                 2.85         1.40         3.20         1.40         2.49           1.31
                                 Auditory imageability                3.14         1.39         3.52         1.33         2.77           1.35
                                  Tactile imageability                2.44         1.32         2.69         1.36         2.19           1.22
                                  Emotional valence                   2.90         1.04         2.95         1.03         2.86           1.04
                                        Arousal                       2.65         1.06         2.71         1.10         2.59           1.00
                                         Length                       2.72         1.30         3.69         0.91         1.75           0.80
                                         Speed                        2.95         1.07         2.71         1.00         3.19           1.08
                           M: mean; SD: standard deviation; ALL: all written Japanese sound-symbolic words used in this study; LV:
                           written Japanese sound-symbolic words including long vowels; SV: written Japanese sound-symbolic words
                           including short vowels. These means and standard deviations were calculated in R [49] and psych [50]. In the
                           survey study, participants evaluated each written Japanese sound-symbolic word by using 5-point semantic
                           differential scales [29,30] associated with familiarity (1: unfamiliar; 5: familiar), visual imageability (1: not visually
                           imageable; 5: visually imageable), auditory imageability (1: not auditorily imageable; 5: auditorily imageable),
                           tactile imageability (1: not tactilely imageable; 5: tactilely imageable), emotional valence (1: dislike; 5: like),
                           arousal (1: calm; 5: excited), length (1: short; 5: long), and speed (1: slow; 5: fast).

                                 Second, we calculated Cronbach’s alphas to examine the reliability of each subjective
                           evaluation. Regarding written Japanese sound-symbolic words with long vowels, the Cron-
                           bach’s alphas (raw alphas in the psych package on R) for familiarity, visual imageabilities,
                           auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence (affection), arousal (excite-
                           ment), length, and speed were 0.73, 0.74, 0.78, 0.79, 0.72, 0.68, 0.89, and 0.75, respectively.
                           Regarding written Japanese sound-symbolic words with short vowels, the Cronbach’s
                           alphas (raw alphas in the psych package on R) for familiarity, visual imageability, audi-
                           tory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence (affection), arousal (excitement),
                           length, and speed were 0.77, 0.74, 0.79, 0.79, 0.72, 0.67, 0.83, and 0.87, respectively. Cron-
                           bach’s alphas were also assessed using SPSS statistical software. The Cronbach’s alphas
                           for the semantic differential scales associated with evaluations excluding arousal were
                           greater than 0.70. The semantic differential scales of arousal (0.68 in LV and 0.67 in SV,
                           respectively) were at least higher than 0.65. In general, a Cronbach’s alpha of more than
                           0.70 is acceptable [51]. The Cronbach’s alphas of arousal in this study were categorized
                           as arbitrary labels such as reasonable, adequate, moderate, and satisfactory as used in
                           previous studies [52]. Published research shows that a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.6 or 0.7 is
                           acceptable [53]. Thus, these findings suggested that the semantic differential scales would
                           be approximately reliable, although the Cronbach’s alphas of arousal were not higher than
                           0.7 (0.68 in LV and 0.67 in SV).
                                 Third, we conducted a linear mixed-effects model analysis to examine the differences
                           between subjective evaluations of written Japanese sound-symbolic words, with long and
                           short vowels. In the linear mixed-effects model, random effects were participants and
                           paired items (paired words), and fixed effects (independent variables) were vowel length (0:
                           SV; 1: LV). In addition, dependent variables were all the evaluations of the 5-point semantic
                           differential scales such as familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile
                           imageability, emotional valence, arousal, length, and speed. For the mixed-effects modeling,
                           we used R [49], lme4 [54,55], and lmerTest packages [56]. Each syntax of the mixed-effects
                           models was lmer (each subjective evaluation ~ vowel length + (1 | participant) + (1 |
                           paired items), data = data, control = lmerControl (optimizer = “bobyqa,” optCtrl = list
                           (maxfun = 100,000))) based on previous psycholinguistic research [24,28,48]. The results
                           showed that familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability,
                           emotional valence, arousal, and length were higher for LV than SV, while speed was only
                           higher for SV than LV (see Table 2).
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                                                6 of 11

      Table 2. Results of mixed-effects models for paired list of written Japanese sound-symbolic words including long vowels
      (LV) and short vowels (SV).

                                     F             VI              AI             TI             EV               A               L                 S
     Random Effects             Variance (SD)
        Participants
                                0.24 (0.49)    0.25 (0.50)    0.32 (0.57)     0.34 (0.58)    0.17 (0.41)     0.12 (0.35)     0.08 (0.28)       0.13 (0.36)
         (intercept)
       Paired items
                                0.42 (0.65)    0.45 (0.67)    0.39 (0.62)     0.32 (0.57)    0.25 (0.50)     0.27 (0.52)     0.02 (0.14)       0.04 (0.21)
        (intercept)
          Residual              1.00 (1.00)    1.17 (1.08)    1.13 (1.06)     1.04 (1.02)    0.69 (0.83)     0.75 (0.86)     0.64 (0.80)       0.91 (0.95)
       Fixed effects            Estimate (SE)
                                2.76 (0.21)    2.49 (0.22)     2.77 (0.20)    2.19 (0.18)    2.86 (0.16)     2.59 (0.17)     1.75 (0.05)       3.19 (0.07)
          Intercept
                                    ***            ***             ***            ***            ***             ***             ***               ***
                                0.43 (0.03)    0.71 (0.03)     0.75 (0.03)    0.51 (0.03)    0.10 (0.03)     0.13 (0.03)     1.95 (0.02)      −0.49 (0.03)
       Vowel length
                                    ***            ***             ***            ***            ***             ***             ***              ***
      F: familiarity; VI: visual imageability; AI: auditory imageability; TI: tactile imageability; EV: emotional valence; A: arousal; L: length; S:
      speed; SD: standard deviation; SE: standard error; ***: p < 0.001. The mixed-effects models were conducted in R [49], R packages included
      lme4 [54,55] and lmerTest [56]. Dependent variables are subjective evaluations, whereas participants and paired words are random effects.
      In addition, vowel length (0: SV; 1: LV) is a fixed effect. In the survey study, participants evaluated each written Japanese sound-symbolic
      word by using 5-point semantic differential scales [29,30] associated with familiarity (1: unfamiliar; 5: familiar), visual imageability (1: not
      visually imageable; 5: visually imageable), auditory imageability (1: not auditorily imageable; 5: auditorily imageable), tactile imageability
      (1: not tactilely imageable; 5: tactilely imageable), emotional valence (1: dislike; 5: like), arousal (1: calm; 5: excited), length (1: short; 5:
      long), and speed (1: slow; 5: fast).

                                         4. Discussion
                                              In this survey research, we investigated whether vowel length (long or short vowels) af-
                                         fected perceptual and emotional subjective evaluations of written Japanese sound-symbolic
                                         words. Two findings emerged from this survey: First, written Japanese sound-symbolic
                                         words with long vowels were perceived as more familiar, visually imageable, auditorily
                                         imageable, tactilely imageable, preferable, excited, and longer than those with short vowels.
                                         Second, written Japanese sound-symbolic words with short vowels were perceived as faster
                                         than those with long vowels. The current findings suggest that vowel length in written
                                         Japanese sound-symbolic words increases the perceptually and emotionally subjective
                                         evaluations of Japanese sound-symbolic words.

                                         4.1. Effects of Vowel Length in Written Words
                                              The current study showed that written Japanese sound-symbolic words with long
                                         vowels were perceived as more familiar, visually imageable, auditorily imageable, tactilely
                                         imageable, preferable, excited, and longer than those with short vowels. Our findings were
                                         congruent with previous findings. Hamano [32] theoretically showed that sound-symbolic
                                         words with a long vowel referred to spatially and temporally longer actions than those
                                         with a short vowel. In addition, Bross [42] showed that names with a long vowel were
                                         associated with longer and bigger objects than names with a short vowel. The current and
                                         previous findings suggest that the vowel length of sound-symbolic words in a language
                                         increases the temporal and spatial length of referents (actions and objects).
                                              Alternatively, the results of the current research also showed that the vowel length of
                                         sound-symbolic words increased multisensory imageabilities, emotional valence, and arousal.
                                         Words which included long vowel(s) increased the perceptual and emotional referents of
                                         words which included sound-symbolic words. In fact, Pathak et al. reported that words
                                         which included long vowels were more associated with sweet food than words which
                                         included short vowels [57]. Another study showed that words which included long vowels
                                         negatively correlated with sharpness [58], although this finding does not suggest that
                                         words which included long vowels increased the perceptual and emotional referents of
                                         words. Taken together, words including long vowels could affect multisensory referents
                                         of words.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                             7 of 11

                                 The perceptual and emotional sensitivity can also be orthographically affected by
                           written sound-symbolic words. A previous study reported that initial consonants of written
                           sound-symbolic words affected multisensory imageabilities, emotional valence, and arousal
                           (e.g., garigari vs. karikari [28]). Cuskley and colleagues reported that written pseudowords
                           with written round letters were associated with round figures, while pseudowords with
                           spikey letters were associated with spiky figures [15]. Another study showed that written
                           angular letters in spiky shapes as frames facilitated response times in a lexical decision
                           task [16]. These previous findings suggest that visual properties of linguistic features (e.g.,
                           visual characteristics of letters and font styles) could be congruent with the visual properties
                           of figures as referents (spiky or round figures [15]) or background (e.g., frames [16]). In this
                           study, we used written Japanese sound-symbolic words which included long vowels (e.g.,
                           フーフー, fuufuu) and short vowels (e.g., フフ, fufu). The long vowels are represented as
                           a dash (ー: chōonpu in Japanese [46]). The written form of the dash (ー: chōonpu) could
                           affect the perceptually and emotionally subjective evaluations of written Japanese sound-
                           symbolic words. Ando et al. (2021) reported that native speakers of Japanese evaluated
                           that the written Japanese vowel i (イ) was thinner than other written Japanese vowels such
                           as a (ア), u (ウ), e (エ), and o (オ [14]). In the previous case, the visual feature of the written
                           Japanese vowel i (イ) might be perceived as thinner than the others. Similar to previous
                           findings, in this study, participants might have perceived written Japanese sound-symbolic
                           words, which included two long vowels shown with 2 dashes (e.g., ブーブー in Japanese,
                           buubuu) as more familiar, visually imageable, auditorily imageable, tactilely imageable,
                           preferable, excited, longer, and slower than written Japanese sound-symbolic words which
                           included short vowels shown with no dash (e.g., ブブ in Japanese, bubu). As another
                           perspective of the differences between written Japanese sound-symbolic words, including
                           long and short vowels, character spacing might affect the evaluations. Previous research
                           has shown that text spacing was essential for increasing reading performance, readability,
                           and preference, as well as decreasing fatigue in reading [59,60]. In this study, since the two
                           dashes (ー: chōonpu) in written Japanese sound-symbolic words made text spacing larger
                           between characters, participants could easily perceive written Japanese sound-symbolic
                           words which included two long vowels, which might increase the perceptual and emotional
                           evaluations.

                           4.2. Future Directions
                                 Future studies should develop these findings in terms of research methods. First,
                           spoken sound-symbolic words were used as stimuli for the survey. If researchers use
                           spoken sound-symbolic words as stimuli, they may need to control the auditory features
                           of the stimuli (e.g., sound volume, sound pressure, and speakers). Second, researchers
                           may directly use perceptual references including pictures (e.g., line drawings), sounds,
                           objects [61], foods, or drinks [62] as the referents of sound-symbolic words in rating or
                           matching tasks. The advantage of using perceptual referents is that participants can directly
                           evaluate non-arbitrary relationships between written, spoken, or other forms of real words,
                           including sound-symbolic words or pseudowords and perceptual or emotional referents.
                           Alternatively, researchers may also use wh-questions or naming tasks as the tasks for the
                           production of words or pseudowords [63–67]. Third, researchers can also examine how
                           participants associate sound-symbolic words with referents by using associative learning
                           methods. Researchers have investigated arbitrarily associative learning of meaningless
                           or unfamiliar words in a native or second language with its referential features (mean-
                           ings) [23,24,68–70], while other researchers have also examined non-arbitrarily associative
                           learning of words and referents [17–21]. Finally, although we compared written Japanese
                           sound-symbolic words including long vowels with those including short vowels, the sub-
                           jective evaluations of the written Japanese sound-symbolic words might be affected by
                           not only the vowel length, but also the meanings (referents) of the presented Japanese
                           sound-symbolic words. In fact, the presented pairs of the written Japanese sound-symbolic
                           words in this study include differential meanings (referents) associated with subjective eval-
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                     8 of 11

                           uations measured in this study (see Appendix A), since we could not find pairs of written
                           Japanese sound-symbolic words associated with the same meanings (referents). In sound
                           symbolism, researchers have hypothesized that vowels and consonants in words directly
                           affect referential features of sound-symbolic words in languages [3,5,28,32]. However,
                           if researchers can find pairs of sound-symbolic words associated with the same or more
                           similar meanings (referents), researchers may rigorously clarify effects of vowel length in
                           sound-symbolic words by using the controlled stimuli and methods used in this study.

                           5. Conclusions
                                 In this survey, in which Japanese native speakers subjectively evaluated each writ-
                           ten Japanese sound-symbolic word, we examined the effects of vowel length in written
                           Japanese sound-symbolic words by using eight semantic differential scales associated
                           with familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional
                           valence, arousal, length, and speed. Two findings emerged from this survey. First, written
                           Japanese sound-symbolic words with long vowels were perceived as more familiar, visu-
                           ally imageable, auditorily imageable, tactilely imageable, preferable, excited, and longer
                           than those with short vowels. Second, written Japanese sound-symbolic words with short
                           vowels were perceived as faster than those with long vowels. Taken together, these find-
                           ings suggest that vowel length in written Japanese sound-symbolic words increases the
                           perceptually and emotionally subjective evaluations of Japanese sound-symbolic words.

                           Supplementary Materials: The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10
                           .3390/bs11060090/s1, Table S1: Descriptive statistics for written Japanese sound-symbolic words
                           including long vowels (LV) and short vowels (SV).
                           Author Contributions: Conceptualization, T.K.; methodology, Z.L., N.W., Y.Y., and T.K.; software,
                           Z.L., N.W., and T.K.; validation, Z.L., N.W., and T.K.; formal analyses, Z.L., N.W., and T.K.; investiga-
                           tion, Z.L. and N.W.; resources, T.K.; data curation, Z.L. and N.W.; writing—original draft preparation,
                           Z.L., N.W., and T.K.; writing—review and editing, Z.L., N.W., and T.K.; visualization, Z.L., N.W., and
                           T.K.; supervision, T.K.; project administration, T.K.; funding acquisition, T.K. The first author (Z.L.)
                           and the second author (N.W.) contributed equally to this study. All authors have read and agreed to
                           the published version of the manuscript.
                           Funding: The corresponding author (T.K.) was supported by the Hiroshima University Grant-in-
                           Aid for Scientific Research, KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Research Activity Start-up, KAKENHI
                           Grant-in-Aid for Early-Career Scientists, and KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C). In
                           addition, this research was conducted as part of the School of Education Joint Research Project 2020
                           at Hiroshima University, and received research support from the School of Education.
                           Institutional Review Board Statement: This study was approved by the ethical committee of the
                           Graduate School of Humanities and Social Sciences at Hiroshima University (approval code: 2020001).
                           This study was conducted in accordance with the code of ethics set by the Declaration of Helsinki
                           and all its amendments.
                           Informed Consent Statement: After each participant read and consented to the written explanation
                           of the study, they answered the questionnaire.
                           Data Availability Statement: The data are available on request to the corresponding author.
                           Acknowledgments: We would like to thank the editors and reviewers for their supportive handling
                           and reviews for improving this article. We also thank Yutao Yang, Misa Ando, Ukwueze Jonah
                           Obinna, and other students and faculty members of the Department of Psychology at Hiroshima
                           University for their kind support in facilitating this study.
                           Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                                        9 of 11

Appendix A

           Table A1. Stimuli of Written Japanese Sound-Symbolic Words including Long Vowels (LV) and Short Vowels (SV).

                                   LV                                                                          SV
       Japanese Characters,                                                       Japanese Characters,
        Alphabetic Letters                   English Meanings                      Alphabetic Letters                    English Meanings
         (Pronunciation)                                                            (Pronunciation)
                                                                                                                      giggling at something or
          フーフー, fuufuu                    breathing on something                        フフ, fufu
                                                                                                                             somebody
                                        buzz of a cicada; buzz of bell;                                             buzz of an insect, squeak, or
           ジージー, jiijii                                                                  ジジ, jiji
                                        sound of burning something                                                  sound of burning something
                                         crying voice or behavior to
                                         hardship; bird song; throat                                                 sound of fast movement of
           ヒーヒー, hiihii                                                                 ヒヒ, hihi
                                        sound in difficulty breathing;                                                 arrow or wind; beeping
                                                  beeping
                                          peeing; sound of peeing;
          シーシー, shiishii                getting out crumbs between                     シシ, shishi                              sobbing
                                         teeth by using a toothpick
                                           squeak; chirp; sucking                                                    chirp; sucking something;
      チューチュー, chuuchuu                                                              チュチュ, chuchu
                                                something                                                                      muah
                                         sound of pig or car; state of
         ブーブー, buubuu                                                                   ブブ, bubu                            buzz of a bee
                                              dissatisfaction
                                                                                                                    sound or behavior of fast and
      ヒョーヒョー, hyoohyoo                   sound of a trumpet or flute                ヒョヒョ, hyohyo
                                                                                                                          light movement
      ニャーニャー, nyaanyaa                              meow                            ニャニャ, nyanya                       meow; acting like a cat
                                          flow of a large amount of                                                   flow of a large amount of
        ジャージャー, jaajaa                                                                ジャジャ, jaja
                                                 something                                                                   something
                                        barking of a fox; crowing of a
          コーコー, kookoo                   rooster; a faint sound heard                   ココ, koko                        screech of a monkey
                                               from a distance
      LV: written Japanese sound-symbolic words including long vowels; SV: written Japanese sound-symbolic words including short vowels.
      The Japanese character was a katakana character that are one type of Japanese character [45,46]. English meanings do not reflect the degree
      of each evaluation (familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, arousal, length, and
      speed). We translated written Japanese sound-symbolic words in a dictionary of sound-symbolic words [43] to English meanings, whereas
      we originally added two Japanese sound-symbolic words including hyohyo and nyanya and translated them to English after checking
      meanings with a native speaker of Japanese (the last author).

References
1.    Dingemanse, M.; Blasi, D.E.; Lupyan, G.; Christiansen, M.H.; Monaghan, P. Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language.
      Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 603–615. [CrossRef]
2.    Kawahara, S. Sound symbolism and theoretical phonology. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2020, 14, 12372. [CrossRef]
3.    Newman, S.S. Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. Am. J. Psychol. 1933, 45, 53. [CrossRef]
4.    Perniss, P.; Thompson, R.L.; Vigliocco, G. Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages.
      Front. Psychol. 2010, 1, 227. [CrossRef]
5.    Sapir, E. A study in phonetic symbolism. J. Exp. Psychol. 1929, 12, 225–239. [CrossRef]
6.    Köhler, W. Gestalt Psychology; Liveright: New York, NY, USA, 1947.
7.    Ramachandran, V.S.; Hubbard, E.M. Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought, and language. J. Conscious. Stud. 2001, 8,
      3–34. Available online: https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2001/00000008/00000012/1244 (accessed on 17
      June 2021).
8.    Lacey, S.; Jamal, Y.; List, S.M.; McCormick, K.; Sathian, K.; Nygaard, L.C. Stimulus parameters underlying sound-symbolic
      mapping of auditory pseudowords to visual shapes. Cogn. Sci. 2020, 44, e12883. [CrossRef]
9.    Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Park, J.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C.; Sugiura, M. Tasting names: Systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds
      associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80, 103801. [CrossRef]
10.   Aryani, A.; Isbilen, E.S.; Christiansen, M.H. Affective arousal links sound to meaning. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 978–986. [CrossRef]
11.   Sidhu, D.M.; Pexman, P.M. The sound symbolism of names. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 28, 398–402. [CrossRef]
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                            10 of 11

12.   Sidhu, D.M.; Westbury, C.; Hollis, G.; Pexman, P.M. Sound symbolism shapes the English language: The maluma/takete effect in
      English nouns. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2021, 1–9. [CrossRef]
13.   Namba, S.; Kambara, T. Semantics based on the physical characteristics of facial expressions used to produce Japanese vowels.
      Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 157. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14.   Ando, M.; Liu, X.; Yan, Y.; Yang, Y.; Namba, S.; Abe, K.; Kambara, T. Sound-symbolic semantics of written Japanese vowels in a
      paper-based survey study. Front. Commun. 2021, 6, 617532. [CrossRef]
15.   Cuskley, C.; Simner, J.; Kirby, S. Phonological and orthographic influences in the bouba-kiki effect. Psychol. Res. 2015, 81, 119–130.
      [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16.   De Carolis, L.; Marsico, E.; Arnaud, V.; Coupé, C. Assessing sound symbolism: Investigating phonetic forms, visual shapes and
      letter fonts in an implicit bouba-kiki experimental paradigm. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0208874. [CrossRef]
17.   Asano, M.; Imai, M.; Kita, S.; Kitajo, K.; Okada, H.; Thierry, G. Sound symbolism scaffolds language development in preverbal
      infants. Cortex 2015, 63, 196–205. [CrossRef]
18.   Imai, M.; Kita, S.; Nagumo, M.; Okada, H. Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition 2008, 109, 54–65. [CrossRef]
      [PubMed]
19.   Kantartzis, K.; Imai, M.; Kita, S. Japanese sound-symbolism facilitates word learning in English-speaking children. Cogn. Sci.
      2011, 35, 575–586. [CrossRef]
20.   Yang, J.; Asano, M.; Kanazawa, S.; Yamaguchi, M.K.; Imai, M. Sound symbolism processing is lateralized to the right temporal
      region in the prelinguistic infant brain. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–10. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
21.   Imai, M.; Kita, S. The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philos. Trans.
      R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130298. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
22.   Havas, V.; Taylor, J.S.H.; Vaquero, L.; De Diego-Balaguer, R.; Rodríguez-Fornells, A.; Davis, M.H. Semantic and phonological
      schema influence spoken word learning and overnight consolidation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2018, 71, 1469–1481. [CrossRef]
23.   Kambara, T.; Tsukiura, T.; Shigemune, Y.; Kanno, A.; Nouchi, R.; Yomogida, Y.; Kawashima, R. Learning-dependent changes
      of associations between unfamiliar words and perceptual features: A 15-day longitudinal study. Lang. Sci. 2013, 35, 80–86.
      [CrossRef]
24.   Liu, X.; Horinouchi, H.; Yang, Y.; Yan, Y.; Ando, M.; Obinna, U.J.; Namba, S.; Kambara, T. Pictorial referents facilitate recognition
      and retrieval speeds of associations between novel words in a second language (L2) and referents. Front. Commun. 2021, 6, 605009.
      [CrossRef]
25.   Takashima, A.; Bakker, I.; van Hell, J.G.; Janzen, G.; McQueen, J. Richness of information about novel words influences how
      episodic and semantic memory networks interact during lexicalization. NeuroImage 2014, 84, 265–278. [CrossRef]
26.   Takashima, A.; Bakker, I.; van Hell, J.G.; Janzen, G.; McQueen, J.M. Interaction between episodic and semantic memory networks
      in the acquisition and consolidation of novel spoken words. Brain Lang. 2017, 167, 44–60. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27.   Saji, N.; Akita, K.; Kantartzis, K.; Kita, S.; Imai, M. Cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound symbolism in novel
      words elicited by locomotion videos in Japanese and English. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0218707. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28.   Kambara, T.; Umemura, T. The relationships between initial consonants in Japanese sound symbolic words and familiarity,
      multi-sensory imageability, emotional valence, and arousal. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2021, 1–12. [CrossRef]
29.   Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J. Factor analysis of meaning. J. Exp. Psychol. 1955, 50, 325–338. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
30.   Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.; Tannenbaum, P. The Measurement of Meaning; University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL, USA, 1957.
31.   Berlin, B. The first congress of ethnozoological nomenclature. J. R. Anthr. Inst. 2006, 12, S23–S44. [CrossRef]
32.   Hamano, S. The Sound-Symbolic System of Japanese; Studies in Japanese Linguistics (Series); CSLI Publications: Stanford, CA, USA,
      1998.
33.   Ohala, J.J. The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In Sound Symbolism; Hinton, L., Nichols, J.,
      Ohala, J.J., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1994; pp. 325–347.
34.   Nishi, K.; Strange, W.; Akahane-Yamada, R.; Kubo, R.; Trent-Brown, S.A. Acoustic and perceptual similarity of Japanese and
      American English vowels. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2008, 124, 576–588. [CrossRef]
35.   Shinohara, K.; Kawahara, S. A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual
      Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society; Linguistic Society of America: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 396–410. [CrossRef]
36.   Klink, R.R. Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound symbolism. Mark. Lett. 2000, 11, 5–20. [CrossRef]
37.   Vainio, L. Magnitude sound symbolism influences vowel production. J. Mem. Lang. 2021, 118, 104213. [CrossRef]
38.   Mugitani, R.; Pons, F.; Fais, L.; Dietrich, C.; Werker, J.F.; Amano, S. Perception of vowel length by Japanese- and English-learning
      infants. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 236–247. [CrossRef]
39.   Sato, Y.; Sogabe, Y.; Mazuka, R. Discrimination of phonemic vowel length by Japanese infants. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 106–119.
      [CrossRef]
40.   Bion, R.A.H.; Miyazawa, K.; Kikuchi, H.; Mazuka, R. Learning phonemic vowel length from naturalistic recordings of Japanese
      infant-directed speech. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e51594. [CrossRef]
41.   Hayashi, A.; Mazuka, R. Emergence of Japanese infants’ prosodic preferences in infant-directed vocabulary. Dev. Psychol. 2017,
      53, 28–37. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
42.   Bross, F. Cognitive associations between vowel length and object size: A new feature contributing to a bouba/kiki effect. In
      Proceedings of the Conference on Phonetics & Phonology; Humboldt-Universität: Berlin, Germany, 2018; pp. 17–20.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 90                                                                                                            11 of 11

43.   Ono, M. (Ed.) Nihongo Onomatope Jiten [Japanese Onomatopoeia Dictionary]; Shogakukan: Tokyo, Japan, 2007.
44.   Francis, A.L.; Ciocca, V. Stimulus presentation order and the perception of lexical tones in Cantonese. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003,
      114, 1611–1621. [CrossRef]
45.   Goetry, V.; Urbain, S.; Morais, J.; Kolinsky, R. Paths to phonemic awareness in Japanese: Evidence from a training study. Appl.
      Psycholinguist. 2005, 26, 285–309. [CrossRef]
46.   Yamaguchi, T. Japanese Linguistics: An Introduction; Continuum: New York, NY, USA, 2007.
47.   Kambara, T.; Umemura, T.; Ackert, M.; Yang, Y. The relationship between psycholinguistic features of religious words and core
      dimensions of religiosity: A survey study with Japanese participants. Religions 2020, 11, 673. [CrossRef]
48.   Baayen, R.; Davidson, D.; Bates, D. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J. Mem. Lang.
      2008, 59, 390–412. [CrossRef]
49.   R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria,
      2013; Available online: https://www.r-project.org (accessed on 4 March 2020).
50.   Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research; Northwestern University: Evanston, IL, USA, 2013; Available
      online: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psychVersion=1.3.2. (accessed on 14 June 2021).
51.   Cortina, J.M. What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J. Appl. Psychol. 1993, 78, 98–104. [CrossRef]
52.   Taber, K.S. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ.
      2018, 48, 1273–1296. [CrossRef]
53.   Van Griethuijsen, R.A.L.F.; Van Eijck, M.W.; Haste, H.; Brok, P.J.D.; Skinner, N.C.; Mansour, N.; Gencer, A.S.; Boujaoude, S. Global
      patterns in students’ views of science and interest in science. Res. Sci. Educ. 2015, 45, 581–603. [CrossRef]
54.   Bates, D.M. Fitting Linear Mixed Models in R. R News 2005, 5, 27–30. Available online: https://cran.opencpu.org/doc/Rnews/
      Rnews_2005-1.pdf#page=27 (accessed on 14 June 2021).
55.   Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48.
      [CrossRef]
56.   Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H.B. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82,
      1–26. [CrossRef]
57.   Pathak, A.; Calvert, G.A.; Motoki, K. Long vowel sounds induce expectations of sweet tastes. Food Qual. Preference 2020, 86,
      104033. [CrossRef]
58.   Fujisawa, N.; Iwamiya, S.-I.; Takada, M. Auditory imagery associated with Japanese onomatopoeic representation. J. Physiol.
      Anthr. Appl. Hum. Sci. 2004, 23, 351–355. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
59.   Ling, J.; van Schaik, P. The influence of line spacing and text alignment on visual search of web pages. Displays 2007, 28, 60–67.
      [CrossRef]
60.   Wang, L.; Sato, H.; Rau, P.-L.P.; Fujimura, K.; Gao, Q.; Asano, Y. Chinese text spacing on mobile phones for senior citizens. Educ.
      Gerontol. 2008, 35, 77–90. [CrossRef]
61.   Sakamoto, M.; Watanabe, J. Bouba/kiki in touch: Associations between tactile perceptual qualities and Japanese phonemes. Front.
      Psychol. 2018, 9, 295. [CrossRef]
62.   Sakamoto, M.; Watanabe, J. Cross-modal associations between sounds and drink tastes/textures: A study with spontaneous
      production of sound-symbolic words. Chem. Senses 2015, 41, 197–203. [CrossRef]
63.   Ikegaya, N.; Motoi, H.; Iijima, K.; Takayama, Y.; Kambara, T.; Sugiura, A.; Silverstein, B.H.; Iwasaki, M.; Asano, E. Spatiotemporal
      dynamics of auditory and picture naming-related high-gamma modulations: A study of Japanese-speaking patients. Clin.
      Neurophysiol. 2019, 130, 1446–1454. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
64.   Iwaki, H.; Sonoda, M.; Osawa, S.-I.; Silverstein, B.H.; Mitsuhashi, T.; Ukishiro, K.; Takayama, Y.; Kambara, T.; Kakinuma, K.;
      Suzuki, K.; et al. Your verbal questions beginning with ’what’ will rapidly deactivate the left prefrontal cortex of listeners. Sci.
      Rep. 2021, 11, 1–14. [CrossRef]
65.   Kambara, T.; Brown, E.C.; Silverstein, B.H.; Nakai, Y.; Asano, E. Neural dynamics of verbal working memory in auditory
      description naming. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 15868. [CrossRef]
66.   Kambara, T.; Sood, S.; Alqatan, Z.; Klingert, C.; Ratnam, D.; Hayakawa, A.; Nakai, Y.; Luat, A.F.; Agarwal, R.; Rothermel, R.; et al.
      Presurgical language mapping using event-related high-gamma activity: The Detroit procedure. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2018, 129,
      145–154. [CrossRef]
67.   Nakai, Y.; Jeong, J.-W.; Brown, E.; Rothermel, R.; Kojima, K.; Kambara, T.; Shah, A.; Mittal, S.; Sood, S.; Asano, E. Three- and
      four-dimensional mapping of speech and language in patients with epilepsy. Brain 2017, 140, 1351–1370. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
68.   Horinouchi, H.; Liu, X.; Kabir, R.S.; Kobayashi, R.; Haramaki, Y.; Kambara, T. Associative pairs of a new word form and single
      referent are learned better than associative pairs of a new word form and multiple referents in a single-day learning context. J.
      Psycholinguist. Res. 2021. in preparation for resubmission.
69.   Yan, Y.; Yang, Y.; Ando, M.; Liu, X.; Kambara, T. Associative learning of new word forms in a first language and gustatory stimuli:
      A behavioral experiment. Front. Commun. 2021. under review.
70.   Yang, Y.; Yan, Y.; Ando, M.; Liu, X.; Kambara, T. Associative learning of new word forms in a first language (L1) and haptic
      referents in a single-day experiment. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2021. accepted.
You can also read